Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
Research ArticleNew Research, Development

Integrative Analysis of Disease Signatures Shows Inflammation Disrupts Juvenile Experience-Dependent Cortical Plasticity

Milo R. Smith, Poromendro Burman, Masato Sadahiro, Brian A. Kidd, Joel T. Dudley and Hirofumi Morishita
eNeuro 15 December 2016, 3 (6) ENEURO.0240-16.2016; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0240-16.2016
Milo R. Smith
1Department of Neuroscience, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
2Department of Genetics and Genomic Sciences, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
3Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
4Department of Ophthalmology, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
5Mindich Child Health and Development Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
6Graduate School of Biomedical Sciences, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
7Icahn Institute for Genomics and Multiscale Biology, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
8Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Poromendro Burman
1Department of Neuroscience, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
3Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
4Department of Ophthalmology, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
5Mindich Child Health and Development Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
8Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Masato Sadahiro
1Department of Neuroscience, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
3Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
4Department of Ophthalmology, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
5Mindich Child Health and Development Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
6Graduate School of Biomedical Sciences, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
8Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Masato Sadahiro
Brian A. Kidd
2Department of Genetics and Genomic Sciences, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
7Icahn Institute for Genomics and Multiscale Biology, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Brian A. Kidd
Joel T. Dudley
2Department of Genetics and Genomic Sciences, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
7Icahn Institute for Genomics and Multiscale Biology, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hirofumi Morishita
1Department of Neuroscience, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
3Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
4Department of Ophthalmology, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
5Mindich Child Health and Development Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
8Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Hirofumi Morishita

Visual Abstract

Figure
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint

Abstract

Throughout childhood and adolescence, periods of heightened neuroplasticity are critical for the development of healthy brain function and behavior. Given the high prevalence of neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism, identifying disruptors of developmental plasticity represents an essential step for developing strategies for prevention and intervention. Applying a novel computational approach that systematically assessed connections between 436 transcriptional signatures of disease and multiple signatures of neuroplasticity, we identified inflammation as a common pathological process central to a diverse set of diseases predicted to dysregulate plasticity signatures. We tested the hypothesis that inflammation disrupts developmental cortical plasticity in vivo using the mouse ocular dominance model of experience-dependent plasticity in primary visual cortex. We found that the administration of systemic lipopolysaccharide suppressed plasticity during juvenile critical period with accompanying transcriptional changes in a particular set of molecular regulators within primary visual cortex. These findings suggest that inflammation may have unrecognized adverse consequences on the postnatal developmental trajectory and indicate that treating inflammation may reduce the burden of neurodevelopmental disorders.

  • bioinformatics
  • critical period
  • inflammation
  • plasticity
  • transcriptome
  • visual cortex

Significance Statement

During childhood and adolescence, heightened neuroplasticity allows the brain to reorganize and adapt to its environment. Disruptions in these malleable phases can result in permanent neurodevelopmental disorders. To identify pathological mechanisms that disrupt developmental neuroplasticity, we applied a systematic computational screen of hundreds of diseases for their impact on neuroplasticity. We discovered that inflammation would putatively disrupt neuroplasticity and validated this hypothesis in an in vivo experimental mouse model of developmental cortical plasticity. This work suggests that inflammation during the childhood period could have unrecognized negative consequences on the neurodevelopmental trajectory.

Introduction

During childhood and adolescence, the human brain undergoes tremendous reorganization during windows of heightened neuroplasticity. These windows of plasticity are critical periods that allow brain circuits to be refined by sensory and social experiences, which help to establish normal perception and higher cognitive function (Johnson and Newport, 1989; Nikolopoulos et al., 1999; Lewis and Maurer, 2005; Schorr et al., 2005; Nelson et al., 2007; Fox et al., 2010). Disruption of these critical periods can alter neural circuits that shape function and behavior, which may, in turn, contribute to a wide range of psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism (Weinberger, 1987; LeBlanc and Fagiolini, 2011; Takesian and Hensch, 2013; Lee et al., 2014). Previous studies focused on several genes relevant to autism spectrum disorders (MeCP2, Ube3a, Fmr1) and identified marked disruptions in developmental cortical plasticity (Tropea et al., 2009; Yashiro et al., 2009; Harlow et al., 2010). To our knowledge, no studies have conducted a systematic evaluation of pathological mechanisms that may disrupt developmental plasticity. The goal of this study was to leverage the growing repository of publically available transcriptome data from diverse disease states to identify pathological processes with the capacity to disrupt developmental cortical plasticity.

To identify pathological processes that disrupt developmental plasticity, we designed an integrative bioinformatics approach that identifies disruptive pathways through systematic evaluation of molecular profiles of disease states in humans and animals. Our approach adapts molecular matching algorithms from computational drug repurposing (for review, see Hodos et al., 2016) to match transcriptional signatures of disease to those of neuroplasticity. To model plasticity, we leveraged the paradigmatic ocular dominance model of in vivo developmental plasticity (Wiesel and Hubel, 1963) and generated transcriptional signatures from primary visual cortex (V1). We matched plasticity and disease signatures to produce a diverse list of diseases ranked by their likelihood to dysregulate developmental plasticity. Across this ranked list, we sought to identify shared pathophysiology, rather than generate hypotheses about individual disease matches. To quantify shared pathophysiology, we developed and applied a novel Disease Leverage Analysis (DLA) that identifies shared molecular patterns of disease signatures to reveal novel disruptors of developmental plasticity. By examining shared pathophysiology, DLA identified a strong relationship between the molecular signatures of inflammation and developmental plasticity. We tested the hypothesis that inflammation disrupts developmental plasticity in the ocular dominance model of developmental V1 plasticity and found that functional, experience-dependent plasticity in vivo was suppressed by systemic inflammation. Our study demonstrates the utility of an integrative bioinformatics approach for identifying disruptors of developmental neuroplasticity and suggests that inflammation may be an unrecognized risk factor for neurodevelopmental disorders.

Materials and Methods

Animals

Male C57BL/6 mice (Charles River Laboratories) and Lynx1−/− mice (Miwa et al., 2006; a gift from Dr. Nathaniel Heintz, Rockefeller University, New York, NY) were group housed (three to five animals per cage) under a standard 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 A.M., lights off at 7:00 P.M.) with constant temperature (23°C) and ad libitum access to food and water. The Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai approved all procedures involving animals.

Substances

Lyophilized Escherichia coli lipopolysaccharide (LPS; 600,000 endotoxin units/mg; serotype 0127:B8, catalog #L5024, lot 073M4024V, Sigma-Aldrich) was reconstituted in sterile saline (0.9% NaCl) to yield a stock solution of 2 mg/ml, which was diluted with saline on the day of injection to yield a working concentration of 0.03 mg/ml.

Plasticity signature generation

Transcriptomes were profiled with microarray to generate plasticity signatures. Experiment-naive juvenile C57BL/6 mice at postnatal day 29 (P29), adult Lynx1−/− mice (>P60), and adult C57BL/6 mice (>P60; n = 3 each group) were anesthetized with isoflurane and cervically dislocated; bilateral V1 was removed, immediately frozen on dry ice, and stored at −80°C until processed. Total RNA was extracted from V1 using RNeasy Lipid Tissue Mini kit (Qiagen) and stored at −80°C. A total of 4.5 μg of cRNA was hybridized to Illumina WG-6 2.0 microarrays (750 ng/subarray). A juvenile plasticity signature was generated via differential expression analyses of juvenile versus adult V1 transcriptomes by first quantile normalizing probe-level data with limma (Smyth, 2005) and then computing rank-based differential expression with RankProd (Hong et al., 2006; both R packages available through the Bioconductor repository) to yield 193 unique mouse Entrez IDs. For downstream analysis, mouse Entrez IDs were mapped to human orthologs using the Mouse Genome Informatics homology reference to yield a 176 gene juvenile plasticity signature. We generated a Lynx1−/− signature in an analogous manner to juvenile, to yield a Lynx1−/− plasticity signature of 98 genes. Raw data for plasticity signatures is freely available at the Gene Expression Omnibus under accession number GSE89757.

Molecular matching algorithm

To identify diseases that are predicted to dysregulate plasticity signature genes, we developed a molecular matching score, which is the sum of the absolute value of the rank-difference gene expression measure of disease signatures (for details of this expression measure, see Dudley et al., 2009) that intersect with neuroplasticity signature genes. The absolute value was chosen to simplify downstream interpretation. This score is similar in spirit to the approach taken by Zhang and Gant (2008), except in our implementation high scores indicate significant overlap between disease and plasticity signatures, whereas low scores indicate little or no overlap. To compare match scores (M) across diseases, we normalized the scores with n = 10,000 permutations of scores using Formula p Values were estimated using the Generalized Pareto Distribution (Knijnenburg et al., 2009) on n permutations and were multiple test corrected using the method of Benjamini and Hochberg (1995).

Disease Leverage Analysis

We developed DLA to infer pathological processes that are shared across diseases and predicted to dysregulate plasticity signature genes. For pathological processes, we used the 50 “hallmark” gene sets (MSigDb; Subramanian et al., 2005). We computed a pathology score for each hallmark gene set for each disease signature, for a total of 50 × 436 = 21,800 scores. A pathology score is the sum of the absolute value of the normalized disease signature gene expression that is shared with a hallmark gene set. The absolute value was chosen because the direction of effect for gene sets is not necessarily known. We next calculated a linear regression between the pathology scores for a specific gene set and the plasticity–disease molecular match scores. We estimated the p value for the association between the pathology scores and disease–plasticity scores (the β1 coefficient) by computing 20,000 permutations of pathology scores using gene sets the same length as the input gene set and then calculating the regression on the permuted scores. If Formula , where 𝟙 is the indicator function (i.e., the value is 1 when the conditional is satisfied and 0 otherwise) and n = the number of permutations, the p value was the empirical estimate: Formula ; otherwise, the Generalized Pareto Distribution was used to estimate the p value (Knijnenburg et al., 2009). Bonferroni’s method was used to correct for multiple hypothesis tests (denoted pcorrected in the text). To account for the probability of a large coefficient by chance, actual β1 coefficients were normalized by the permutated distribution of β1 according to Formula Positive β values are pathological processes associated with diseases that were predicted to disrupt plasticity signatures. Negative β values are pathological processes associated with diseases that are predicted to not disrupt plasticity signatures. To calculate enrichments of top DLA gene sets, we chose a conservative cutoff of pcorrected < 5 × 10−5 and then calculated the over-representation of inflammation gene sets among positive DLA associations. To do so, we used the hypergea (Bönn, 2016) R package, which uses a conditional maximum likelihood estimate to compute the odds ratio (OR) on adjusted cell counts (to avoid empty cells) and obtains two-sided p values from the hypergeometric distribution.

Quantitative PCR

Experiment-naive juvenile mice (P26; n = 5/group) were lightly anesthetized with isoflurane to avoid additional stressors and injected intraperitoneally before 12:00 noon eastern standard time with a dose of LPS that does not cross the blood–brain barrier (Banks and Robinson, 2010; 300 μg/kg, ∼4.5 μg/mouse) or vehicle (150 μl of saline). Four hours later, mice were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane and decapitated, and bilateral V1 was removed under RNAse-free conditions, briefly rinsed in sterile saline (0.9% NaCl), immediately frozen on dry ice, and transferred to −80°C storage until processed. Total RNA was extracted from V1 using the RNeasy Lipid Tissue Mini Kit (Qiagen) and stored at −80°C. RNA yields ranged from 4.5 to 10 μg/sample and RNA integrity numbers ranged from 8.7 to 10 (mean, 9.8; SD, 0.32). Total V1 RNA was converted to cDNA using a High-Capacity cDNA Reverse Transcription Kit (Life Technologies). Quantitative PCR (qPCR) was performed by the Mount Sinai Quantitative PCR core facility using TaqMan probes (catalog numbers: NogoR: 00445861, Lynx1: 01204957_g1, S100a8: 00496696_g1, Lrg1: 01278767_m1, Lcn2: 01324470_m1, PirB: 01700366_m1, Cldn5: 00727012_s1, Egr2: 00456650_m1, Npas4: 01227866_g1, Il1: 00434228_m1, Agmat: 01348862_m1, Ch25h: 00515486_s1, Alox12b: 01325300_gH, Evpl: 01700609_m1, Slc40a1: 00489835_g1, Arc: 01204954_g1, S100a9: 00656925_m1, H2D1/H2K1: 04208017_mH, BDNF: 04230607_s1, Nptx2: 00479438_m1, and Ppp3ca: 01317678_m1, Applied Biosystems). Quantification of the fold change was derived via the –ΔΔ CT method (equivalent to a log2 fold change) and significance was computed with parametric t tests of the Δ CTs, given the approximately normal distribution of Δ CTs. To prioritize qPCR validations, we identified the most differentially dysregulated juvenile neuroplasticity genes by an independent LPS brain study (GSE3253) by subsetting with the LPS brain expression whose absolute expression statistic was ≥2 after conversion to z-score.

In vivo electrophysiology

Under light isoflurane anesthesia, the contralateral eye of experiment-naive P26 mice was sutured, and the animal was immediately injected intraperitoneally with LPS (300 μg/kg, ∼4.5 μg/mouse) or vehicle (150 μl saline). Three days later, single-unit electrophysiological recordings were taken in binocular zone of V1 in response to visual stimuli presented to each eye separately (Gordon and Stryker, 1996). Briefly, recording was conducted under nembutal/chlorprothixene anesthesia. Visually evoked single-unit responses were recorded with 16-channel silicone probes (NeuroNexus) in response to a high-contrast single bar generated by visage system (Cambridge Research Systems). The signal was amplified and thresholded (OmniPlex, Plexon). To ensure single-unit isolation, the waveforms of recorded units were further examined off-line (Offline Sorter, Plexon). For each animal, approximately 3–10 single units were recorded in each of the four to six vertical penetrations spaced evenly (250 μm intervals) across the mediolateral extent of V1 to map the monocular and binocular zones and to avoid sampling bias. Monocular zone was identified when three consecutive units solely registered contralateral responses within a single penetration [ocular dominance score (ODS) of 1, see below for definition of ODS]. Secondary visual cortex was identified by the reversal of retinotopy seen as the electrode was moved into the secondary visual cortex (Gordon and Stryker, 1996). Mice that experienced opening of the sutured eye or had poor recordings (<10 cells/mouse or <3 penetrations/mouse or lack of positive identification of monocular zone and secondary visual cortex) were excluded from further study. To analyze the electrophysiology data, normalized ocular dominance index (ODI) of single neurons was computed by a custom-made MATLAB code by peristimulus time histogram analysis of peak to baseline spiking activity in response to each eye: {[Peak(ipsi) − baseline(ipsi)] − [Peak(contra) − baseline(contra)]}/{[Peak(ipsi) − baseline(ipsi)] + [Peak(contra) − baseline(contra)]}, which produces a range of [−1,+1] where −1 is a completely contralateral dominated cell and +1 is a completely ipsilateral dominated cell. ODI is linearly transformed by assigning [−1.0, −0.5) = 1, [−0.5, −0.3) = 2, [−0.3, −0.1) = 3, [−0.1, +0.1] = 4, (+0.1, +0.3] = 5, (+0.3, +0.5] = 6, (+0.5, +1.0] = 7 to produce the ODS. Finally, the contralateral bias index (CBI), a monocular weighted, animal-level summary statistic, is computed from the ODS, as follows: [(n1 − n7) + 2/3(n2 − n6) + 1/3(n3 − n5) + N]/2N, where N = total number of cells and nx = number of cells corresponding to ODS of x. Thus, a CBI value of 0.7 is contralateral dominant, and a CBI value of 0.4 is ipsilateral dominant. For statistical comparison of ocular dominance, ODSs of single neurons were plotted as a proportion histogram and compared via a nonparametric χ2 test, and CBI values of single animals were compared via a t test. Saline-treated juvenile animals (age P26) were the comparison group. The experimenter was blind to the sample group. Sample sizes were statistically estimated prior to undertaking experimental work to be n = 6 per group, assuming the effect sizes seen in previous relevant work.

Statistical analysis

All statistical and computational analyses conducted with R (version 3.2.2) and Python (version 2.7.10). Parametric Welch t tests were two sided, unless otherwise noted. Sample sizes (denoted n) always indicate the number of mice. The influenza 95% confidence interval (CI) for the incidence rate ratio was estimated using the Katz log approach (Fagerland et al., 2015) Formula , where a and b are the successes and m and n are the totals (totals determined by dividing the successes by the published incidence rates. The detail of statistical analysis is shown in Table 1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1:

Statistics

Results

Molecular matching between plasticity and disease signatures

To enable molecular matching between plasticity and disease, we compared V1 transcriptomes of juvenile wild-type mice, during the critical period of elevated ocular dominance plasticity in V1 (Gordon and Stryker, 1996), with adult wild-type mice with reduced plasticity, to identify a differential expression signature of 176 genes (Fig. 1a; Table 2). We computationally matched this signature to 436 disease signatures derived from public microarray data using a previously described method (Dudley et al., 2009; Fig. 1a). This systematic method applies a rank-based molecular matching algorithm to determine the molecular concordance between the plasticity signature and a given disease signature, where high scores indicate plasticity genes are significantly dysregulated by the disease and low scores indicate that the disease has no impact on plasticity genes (for details, see Materials and Methods; Fig. 1b). The molecular matching procedure produced a list of 436 diseases ordered by their prediction to disrupt the plasticity signature. Interestingly, highly ranked diseases included not only brain disorders known to disrupt plasticity, such as Huntington's disease (Usdin et al., 1999; Murphy et al., 2000; Milnerwood et al., 2006), but also non-neurologic disorders (e.g., bacterial infections, inflammatory bowel disease, metabolic diseases), suggesting that a broad range of disease states may impact molecular pathways involved in plasticity.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Diseases that dysregulate the juvenile plasticity signature are associated with inflammatory processes. a, b, Juvenile plasticity signature of 176 genes (represented by green bars) generated by differential expression of P29 vs >P60 C57BL/6 male mice primary visual cortex was computationally matched to 436 disease signatures (represented by orange bars; a) using rank-based molecular matching where large scores indicate shared transcriptional phenotype (b). c, DLA systematically identified processes associated to diseases that dysregulate the plasticity signature. Seven of the 14 largest associations were inflammation-related gene sets, and 7 of 7 of inflammation-related gene sets were strongly associated with plasticity (all at pcorrected < 5 × 10−5: OR = 25.8, 95% CI = 1.4–491.2, p = 2.7 × 10−3, Fisher’s exact test). See Figure 1-1 for source DLA data.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2:

Juvenile plasticity signature

Disease Leverage Analysis (DLA) identifies inflammatory processes as putative disruptors of plasticity

We sought to identify shared pathophysiology across the diverse list of diseases predicted to dysregulate plasticity signatures. To do so, we developed DLA. This approach calculates the association between diseases that dysregulate the plasticity signature genes and the 50 gene sets in the hallmark library (Subramanian et al., 2005) that represent well defined and distinct biological pathways. Specifically, DLA computes a linear regression between the molecular match score (a measure of strength of association between disease and plasticity signatures) and the pathology score (a measure of activity of the biological pathway in a given disease). Large regression coefficients indicate that a given biological pathway is highly active in diseases that dysregulate plasticity gene signatures and may be pathological to developmental plasticity. Using a multiple test-corrected, empirical p value threshold of p < 5 × 10−5, we found that 7 of 14 largest DLA associations were inflammation-related gene sets and that every inflammation-related gene set in the hallmark library was strongly associated with diseases that dysregulate the plasticity signature (7 of 7 inflammation gene sets at a threshold of pcorrected < 5 × 10−5: OR = 25.8, 95% CI = 1.4–491.2, p = 2.7 × 10−3a, Fisher’s exact test; Figs. 1c, 1–1). Moreover, two of these gene sets, tumor necrosis factor-α (TNF-α) signaling via nuclear factor-κB (NF-κB) and interferon-γ (IFN-γ) response, reflect pathways involved in critical period plasticity (Kaneko et al., 2008; Nagakura et al., 2014).

Figure 1-1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1-1.

DLA identifies biological processes common to diseases that perturb the juvenile plasticity signature. To identify shared pathophysiology across the diverse list of diseases predicted to dysregulate the juvenile plasticity signature, we applied DLA. This approach calculates the association between diseases that dysregulate the plasticity signature genes and 50 well defined and distinct biological pathways. To do so, it computes a regression between the molecular match score (a measure that indicates the strength of association between the disease and plasticity signatures; see Table 3 and Materials and Methods) and the pathology score (a measure of activity of the biological pathway in that disease; see Materials and Methods). Large regression coefficients indicate that the biological pathway may disrupt juvenile plasticity. Using a multiple test-corrected, empirical p < 5 × 10−5, seven of the 14 largest DLA associations were inflammation-related gene sets, and every inflammation-related gene set in the hallmark library was strongly associated with diseases that dysregulate plasticity genes (seven of seven inflammation gene sets at pcorrected < 5 × 10−5: OR = 25.8, 95% CI = 1.4–491.2, p = 2.7 × 10−3, Fisher’s exact test). A total of 20,000 permutations of the gene sets was used to estimate p values and to normalize the regression coefficients to allow comparison between effect sizes for different biological pathways. Inflammation-related gene sets: TNF-α signaling via NF-κB, IFN-γ response, inflammatory response, complement, IL-2–Stat5 signaling, IFN-α response, IL-6–Jak–Stat3 signaling.

To control for nonplasticity aspects of age, we repeated the entire analysis using a Lynx1−/− plasticity signature of 98 genes, which was identified by computing the differential expression between adult Lynx1−/− and adult wild-type V1 (Table 4). By releasing the Lynx1 brake on plasticity, Lynx1−/− mice have juvenile-like plasticity in adulthood (Morishita et al., 2010; Bukhari et al., 2015). Indeed, functional similarity is reflected in signature similarity, as juvenile and Lynx1−/− plasticity signatures significantly overlap (35 genes shared, OR = 37.1, 95% CI = 23.8–58.0, p < 2.2 × 10−16b). Applying DLA to the Lynx1−/− molecular matches, we found a strong association between diseases predicted to disrupt Lynx1−/− plasticity and inflammation-related gene sets (7 of 7 inflammation gene sets at a threshold of pcorrected < 5 × 10−5: OR = 63.0, 95% CI = 3.2–1229.8, p = 7.6 × 10−5c Fisher’s exact test; Figs. 2, 2-1). Together, the bioinformatics analyses indicate that inflammation is a process central to diseases predicted to dysregulate plasticity gene expression, suggesting that inflammation may disrupt developmental plasticity.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Diseases that dysregulate the adult Lynx1−/− plasticity signature are associated with inflammatory processes. Using an adult Lynx1−/− plasticity signature of 98 genes (generated by differential expression of primary visual cortex from >P60 Lynx1−/− vs >P60 C57BL/6 male mice), DLA systematically identified biological processes associated with diseases that dysregulate the adult Lynx1−/− plasticity signature genes. Using adult Lynx1−/− animals controls for age, as these adult animals have elevated plasticity similar to that of juvenile animals. Seven of the 11 largest associations were inflammation-related gene sets, and every inflammation-related gene set was strongly associated with plasticity (7 of 7 inflammation gene sets at pcorrected < 5 × 10−5: OR = 63.0, 95% CI = 3.2–1229.8, p = 7.6 × 10−5, Fisher’s exact test). See Figure 2-1 for source DLA data.

Figure 2-1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2-1.

DLA identifies biological processes common to diseases that perturb the Lynx1−/− plasticity signature. To identify shared pathophysiology across the diverse list of diseases predicted to dysregulate the Lynx1−/− plasticity signature, we applied DLA. Large regression coefficients indicate that the biological pathway may disrupt Lynx1−/− plasticity. As with the juvenile plasticity signature, using a multiple test-corrected, empirical p < 5 × 10−5, we found that every inflammation-related gene set in the hallmark library was strongly associated with diseases that dysregulate plasticity genes (7 of 7 inflammation gene sets at pcorrected < 5 × 10−5: OR = 63.0, 95% CI = 3.2–1229.8, p = 7.6 × 10−5 Fisher’s exact test). A total of 20,000 permutations of the gene sets were used to estimate p values and to normalize the regression coefficients to allow comparison between effect sizes for different biological pathways. Inflammation-related gene sets: TNF-α signaling via NF-κB, IFN-γ response, inflammatory response, complement, IL-2–Stat5 signaling, IFN-α response, IL-6–Jak–Stat3 signaling.

Lipopolysaccharide model of inflammation suppresses developmental cortical plasticity

Based on the DLA findings, we hypothesized that inflammatory processes disrupt developmental cortical plasticity. To test this hypothesis, we induced a systemic inflammatory response via LPS and measured the impact on developmental plasticity and related gene expression. We injected a low dose of LPS (300 μg/kg, intraperitoneal) at the peak of juvenile ocular dominance plasticity at P26 and found a strong inflammatory response in V1, as indicated by a 2.4 log2 fold increase of interleukin (IL)-1β compared with vehicle control (p = 3.4 × 10−4d, t test of Δ CTs, n = 5 mice/group). To identify a focused subset of plasticity genes likely to be regulated by LPS (regardless of age or specific brain region) to test in vivo, we investigated a highly significant molecular match between the juvenile plasticity signature and an adult brain-derived LPS transcriptome (GSE3253; rank #14, p = 7.9 × 10−4e; empirical p value calculated using molecular match algorithm; Fig. 3a; Table 3). Next, we identified a subset of genes from GSE3253 that is likely to play a larger role in the under-lying biology (the genes in the extremes using a z-score cutoff) and intersected it with the plasticity signature to identify 16 shared genes (Fig. 3b). Notably, among these shared genes, we identified a negative correlation in their expression pattern (cor = −0.77, p = 0.0007f, Spearman’s correlation). Among these 16 shared genes, the adult LPS data indicated the direction of expression of 13 genes would be reversed by LPS. Indeed, the majority (61.5%) of the 13 genes showed a complete reversal in their differential expression pattern in V1 after peripheral LPS administration during the critical period (qPCR, all reversed genes p < 5 × 10−4g, t test of Δ CTs, n = 5 mice per group; Fig. 3c). These data indicate that genes in the plasticity signature that are also regulated by LPS act in an antagonistic fashion and naturally led us to the hypothesis that inflammation may suppress plasticity. Consistent with this logic, the established brakes of plasticity, PirB (Syken et al., 2006) and H2K1 and H2D1 (Datwani et al., 2009), showed increased expression compared with vehicle (p = 0.04h and p = 0.03i, respectively, t test of Δ CTs, n = 5 mice per group; Fig. 4a,b). In addition, a trigger of plasticity that increases across development, BDNF (Huang et al., 1999), which we predicted in silico to decrease after LPS (Fig. 3b), showed decreased expression compared with vehicle (p = 0.009j, t test of ΔCTs, n = 5 mice/group; Fig. 4b). In contrast, other known plasticity effectors (Takesian and Hensch, 2013) were not changed relative to vehicle (Nptx2, Lynx1, NogoR, Ppp3ca; p > 0.1k, t test of Δ CTs, n = 5 mice per group), indicating that LPS may act through a specific subset of known and novel plasticity effectors (Fig. 4b).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

LPS reverses plasticity signature gene expression. a, LPS disease signature shares plasticity signature genes in silico (molecular match rank #14, p = 7.9 × 10−4; Table 3; disease signature is from GSE3253: adult mouse whole-brain homogenate harvested 4 h after peripheral LPS injected intraperitoneally; genes with absolute value of the standardized expression (z-score) ≥2 SDs from the mean were selected as the most differentially expressed by LPS). b, The expression of the 16 genes shared between juvenile plasticity and the LPS disease signatures is anticorrelated (Spearman’s ρ = −0.77, p = 7.4 × 10−4; LPS disease signature gene expression values fell in the range [−1,+1]; for plotting purposes, the plasticity gene expression fold change was linear transformed to [−1,+1]). c, Of the 13 genes of the 16 predicted to be reversed by LPS, the majority (8 of 13; 61.5%) showed a complete reversal in their differential expression pattern in V1 after LPS administration (300 μg/kg LPS, intraperitoneally, at P26 during the peak of juvenile plasticity) relative to saline. LPS downregulated Cldn5 and Slc40a1 and upregulated Alox12b, S100a9a, Ch25h, Lrg1, S100a8, and Lcn2 (n = 5 mice per group). ***p < 0.001, **p > 0.001 and ≤0.01, *p > 0.01 and ≤0.05 (two-sided t tests of ΔCTs). Log2 fold change is −ΔΔ CT. Error bars indicate the SEM.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3:

Molecular matching among 436 disease signatures and the juvenile plasticity signature indicates diverse diseases may disrupt plasticity

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Inflammation induced by LPS suppresses experience-dependent plasticity in juvenile cortex. a, Juvenile mice (P26) during the peak of ocular dominance plasticity were injected intraperitoneally with either LPS (300 μg/kg) or saline. b, c, Mice were either subjected to qPCR analysis of plasticity effectors from V1 4 h after the injection (b) or underwent 3 d of MD followed by in vivo extracellular recordings to assess ocular dominance plasticity (c). b, LPS increased known plasticity brakes PirB and H2K1/H2D1, and decreased the plasticity trigger BDNF. LPS had no effect on the plasticity effectors Nptx2, Lynx1, NogoR, or Ppp3ca. Log2 FC (fold change) is −ΔΔ CT (n = 5 mice per group). Error bar indicates the SEM. ***p < 0.001, **p > 0.001 and ≤0.01, *p > 0.01 and ≤0.05 (t test of Δ CTs). c, Neurons from peripheral LPS-treated mice (purple histogram: n = 7 mice, 129 cells) showed decreased cortical responsivity to light in the ipsilateral vs contralateral eye, as quantified by a reduced right shift in the ODS distribution after 3 d of MD compared with control saline-treated juvenile mice with MD (gray histogram: 6 mice, 135 cells; χ2 test of ODS distribution: p = 2.6 × 10−6). Animal-level quantification of ocular dominance plasticity by CBI reflects the extent of the ocular dominance shift after 3 d of MD (right-side plot; low CBI indicates higher plasticity). CBI was strongly increased in the LPS-treated group (purple discs: CBI = 0.64 ± 0.02, 7 mice) compared with saline-treated group (gray discs: CBI = 0.49 ± 0.04, 6 mice), indicating that proinflammatory LPS had suppressed developmental plasticity (LPS vs saline, **p = 6 × 10−3, one-sided t test). qPCR data are reported as the mean ± SEM. Horizontal bars for the CBI plot indicate the mean.

Finally, we tested whether inflammation suppresses experience-dependent developmental cortical plasticity in vivo. We administered LPS (300 μg/kg, intraperitoneal) or saline on P26 immediately after suturing one eye to induce ocular dominance plasticity via monocular deprivation (MD; Fig. 4a). After 3 d of MD, we conducted in vivo single-unit recordings of activity-driven changes in the eye preference of single neurons (ocular dominance) in binocular V1 in response to light (Gordon and Stryker, 1996). In mice treated with saline, we observed the expected shift in cortical responsivity to light stimulation from the deprived contralateral to nondeprived ipsilateral eye, as quantified by a decrease in the animal-level CBI (CBI = 0.49 ± 0.04; six mice; 135 cells), indicating the presence of developmental plasticity (Fig. 4c, right-hand plot). In contrast, LPS significantly suppressed the shift in cortical responsivity from the deprived contralateral eye to the nondeprived ipsilateral eye, which was quantified by an increase in CBI and an elimination of the right shift in the distribution of ODSs of single neurons (CBI = 0.64 ± 0.02, seven mice, 129 cells; one-sided t test of CBIs: p = 0.006l; χ2 test of ODS distribution: p = 2.6 × 10−6m), indicating impaired plasticity during the critical period in V1 (Fig. 4c, left-hand plot). Together, these data are consistent with our informatics-derived hypothesis by demonstrating that peripheral injection of LPS induces an inflammatory response in the brain and suppresses developmental cortical plasticity in vivo.

Discussion

Using an integrative bioinformatics approach, we found that inflammation disrupts developmental cortical plasticity. Our study demonstrates the utility of this approach for both identifying diseases that may disrupt plasticity and generating hypotheses on the molecular mechanisms underlying these disruptions. Moreover, our novel Disease Leverage Analysis facilitates novel hypothesis generation, because seemingly unrelated phenotypes, such as neuroplasticity and inflammation, can be connected based on apparently disparate tissues and diseases. Previous work indicating that the disease signal harmonizes across tissues (Dudley et al., 2009) supports this approach and in the present study suggests that the molecular pathways underlying plasticity are shared in diverse tissues and are dysregulated in many disease states, including apparently non-neurological phenotypes (e.g., bacterial infections, inflammatory bowel disease, metabolic diseases). Importantly, the biological relevance of any given molecular match between plasticity and disease must be interpreted with care. In all cases, molecular matches indicate that plasticity and the disease phenotype share underlying molecular machinery. However, a given disease state in a specific tissue may or may not have an impact on functional plasticity or related gene expression if the disease state or tissue is sufficiently localized and segregated from neural tissue. Consequently, we developed Disease Leverage Analysis to use the information of all matches collectively to identify common disease processes and simultaneously shrink the hypothesis space to a manageable set of disease process-oriented hypotheses that bind together the diverse matches. This approach facilitated the unbiased and systematic use of apparently disparate disease signatures to generate novel hypotheses about shared disease mechanisms that may dysregulate plasticity. We find here that a common theme among these dysregulations is inflammation, a biological process that is well suited to communicate peripheral signals to the brain, disrupting plasticity.

We demonstrate several lines of evidence supporting a hypothesis that plasticity and inflammatory processes share components of underlying molecular networks. We computationally predicted associations between plasticity signature-perturbing diseases and TNF-α and IFN-γ pathways (Figs. 1, 2) and also experimentally identified associations between systemic inflammation and increases in the plasticity brakes PirB and MHC-1 in the brain (Fig. 4). These predictions and observations confirm the known role of pathways involving TNF-α, IFN-γ, Pirb, and MHC-I on regulating developmental plasticity (Syken et al., 2006; Kaneko et al., 2008; Datwani et al., 2009; Nagakura et al., 2014) and extend them to the context of inflammation. We also showed that BDNF, a neurotrophic factor essential to the opening of the critical period (Huang et al., 1999), is decreased after LPS administration (Fig. 4), which is consistent with the reported antagonistic relationship of peripheral LPS with BDNF (Guan and Fang, 2006; Schnydrig et al., 2007). Interestingly, we also found that the microglial activator Lcn2 (Jang et al., 2013) is a member of both the juvenile and Lynx1−/− plasticity signatures (Tables 2, 4) and is dramatically increased after LPS in V1 during the critical period (Fig. 3). Activation may inhibit microglia from carrying out their “resting-state” role in mediating experience-dependent plasticity (Sipe et al., 2016), contributing to the dampening of plasticity by inflammation. Collectively, our work suggests a conflict between developmental cortical plasticity and immune-related molecular networks during inflammation, ultimately resulting in the suppression of plasticity during inflammation. Our study provides a novel subset of transcripts that can be used to guide future mechanistic studies into inflammation–plasticity interactions.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4:

Lynx1−/− plasticity signature

Our efforts to understand the molecular machinery involved in the suppression of developmental plasticity focused on immediate changes in gene expression in V1 via acute inflammation (qPCR 4 h after a single intraperitoneal injection of LPS). We expected this time point to be particularly sensitive to disruption because the earliest experience-dependent changes occur within hours to a day of MD at the level of firing rate of parvalbumin interneurons (Aton et al., 2013; Kuhlman et al., 2013; Reh and Hensch, 2014) and protease (Mataga et al., 2002) and microglia activity (Sipe et al., 2016) as triggers for subsequent global ocular dominance plasticity, which takes a few days to be detected by single-unit recordings (Gordon and Stryker, 1996). Importantly, such trigger events occur only during the juvenile critical period when our assay was performed, but not in the adult period (Kuhlman et al., 2013). Thus, we reasoned that the baseline cortical expression signature at this early time point would be critical to gate global ocular dominance plasticity. In addition, peak acute inflammatory response, as measured by an increase in IL-1β in brain after peripheral LPS injection, is between 1 and 4 h (Layé et al., 1994; Eklind et al., 2006; Richwine et al., 2009), a time course well suited to disrupt these earliest experience-dependent plasticity events. While we speculate that LPS disrupted these early trigger events unique to juvenile cortex, more work needs to be performed to understand the molecular events underlying the functional changes seen within hours of MD and to dissect the impact of inflammation on these events. In addition to the early phase of plasticity, inflammation may also impact plasticity mechanisms during the later phases of MD because TNF-α is essential to nondeprived eye potentiation via a homeostatic mechanism at 5-6 d of MD (Kaneko et al., 2008). Ultimately, further work is necessary to tease out the interaction between inflammatory and plasticity mechanisms that contribute to the suppression of functional plasticity across multiple days of experience deprivation. Performing such work comparing acute versus chronic inflammatory models would provide fascinating insights into neuroimmune biology and would help to inform the important clinical question of the potential impact of acute and chronic inflammation on the neurodevelopmental trajectory in children.

While our experimental efforts focused on the impact of acute inflammation on plasticity, our list of diseases predicted to impact plasticity include diseases that accompany chronic inflammation (Table 3). Efforts to study human disease and animal models shed light on how acute versus chronic inflammation may affect plasticity. For example, components of plasticity and inflammation are dysregulated in epilepsy (Vezzani and Granata, 2005). Acute inflammation from low to high doses (LPS) decreases the threshold for the induction of seizure (Sayyah et al., 2003), and a single early life inflammatory insult increases susceptibility to seizure even into adulthood (Galic et al., 2008). Chronic overexpression of inflammation-related genes in rodents causes an increased or decreased susceptibility to seizure, depending on gene dose (Vezzani and Granata, 2005), and seizure itself appears to chronically induce inflammatory markers (De Simoni et al., 2000). This evidence indicates a potential bidirectional effect of epilepsy and inflammation, wherein acute and chronic inflammation may have immediate and long-term effects on epilepsy-related plasticity mechanisms. In addition to epilepsy, cortical lesions and hypoxia-ischemia induce a robust inflammatory response that can endure chronically (Bona et al., 1999; Schroeter et al., 2002) and disrupt ocular dominance plasticity weeks after the injury (Failor et al., 2010; Greifzu et al., 2011). Interestingly, anti-inflammatory (ibuprofen) treatment rescues MD-induced sensory learning (increased visual acuity of the nondeprived eye) in adult (P70–P110) after cortical injury via photothrombosis in the nearby primary somatosensory; however, ibuprofen did not restore ocular dominance plasticity (Greifzu et al., 2011). It is possible that the anti-inflammatory regimen or mechanism of action used was not sufficient to eliminate the inflammation and rescue ocular dominance plasticity, or it may reflect distinct mechanisms of plasticity and their modulation by inflammation in the juvenile cortex versus the adult. While it has been proposed that causes of plasticity disruption may be cortical deafferentiation in the case of cortical lesions or disruption of inhibitory interneurons in the case of hypoxia-ischemia, it is possible that inflammation downstream of injury disrupts plasticity and should be investigated further. In sum, there is evidence that chronic and acute inflammation go hand in hand with disrupted plasticity across a variety of brain disorders, on different time scales, and as a function of different underlying mechanisms. Going forward, work is necessary to understand the contribution of diverse inflammatory mechanisms in the disruption of various types of plasticity across a wide variety of neurological and neurodevelopmental conditions.

Our finding that inflammation suppresses developmental cortical plasticity suggests a potential public health concern related to neurodevelopmental trajectory. During the height of the critical period for visual plasticity (peak is 0.5–2 years in humans; Morishita and Hensch, 2008), children <5 years of age have the highest incidence of contracting LPS-carrying Gram-negative foodborne pathogens relative to other childhood or adult periods (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2013). Other infections that induce inflammation also show an increased incidence during the peak of developmental plasticity in humans; >80% of children <3 years of age experience otitis media (ear infection; Marom et al., 2014) and children <5 years of age are hospitalized for influenza-related complications nearly an order of magnitude more often than children 5–17 years of age (incidence rate ratio = 8.1, 95% CI = 7.3–9.0; data are from Dawood et al., 2010). Our work suggests that this increased incidence of infection during postnatal periods of developmental plasticity (relative to older ages) may be an unrecognized mechanism by which inflammation alters the neurodevelopmental trajectory. Most directly, the suppression of visual cortex plasticity could disrupt the development of binocular matching (Wang et al., 2010), a process central to the development of normal vision that specifically depends on heightened plasticity during the critical period for visual development. In addition, higher-order cognitive processes could be disrupted, due to the hierarchical dependency of various critical periods of plasticity (Takesian and Hensch, 2013). In addition, given that mechanisms of plasticity identified in the visual critical period have translated to other brain regions and functions (Levelt and Hübener, 2012; Nabel and Morishita, 2013; Werker and Hensch, 2015), it is likely that inflammation could disrupt plasticity in other systems.

Our work is a natural extension into the postnatal epoch of the growing body of research indicating deleterious brain and behavioral outcomes due to prenatal inflammatory exposure (Choi et al., 2016; Steullet et al., 2016; Weber-Stadlbauer et al., 2017) and suggests that inflammation may have a more extensive impact on postnatal neurodevelopment and brain function than previously realized. In fact, childhood infections and inflammation are associated with subsequent diagnoses of autism, depression, and schizophrenia as well as declines in cognitive capacity (Dalman et al., 2008; Atladóttir et al., 2010; Khandaker et al., 2014; Benros et al., 2015). The elevated incidence rate of infections during childhood neurodevelopment (relative to older ages) and the association of childhood infection with subsequent neurodevelopmental disorder may indicate a partial explanation for the observed onset of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence (Lee et al., 2014). Our work showing that inflammation disrupts developmental cortical plasticity suggests an unrecognized risk factor for neuropsychiatric disorders and provides a starting point to investigate the underlying pathophysiology.

We show here that an integrative bioinformatics approach is well suited to interrogate the interactions between disease processes and disruptions in developmental plasticity. To extend this approach further, molecular matching could be expanded to the >71,000 experiments publically available (as of 4 August 2016, there were 71,885 Gene Expression Omnibus “Series”), and Disease Leverage Analysis could be expanded to the universe of biologically defined gene sets (as of 4 August 2016, MSigDb alone contained 13,311 sets), facilitating more comprehensive interrogation of the disease space and generation of more specific hypotheses about disease processes that disrupt plasticity. Moreover, this approach is not limited to interrogating neurodevelopment but can be extended to other neurological signatures beyond plasticity. We expect it will be useful for identifying connections between disease processes and other brain phenotypes that can be appropriately represented by a transcriptional signature.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgments: We thank Dr. N. Bukhari for assistance with the gene expression experiments, Dr. B. Readhead for helpful discussions, and Dr. Nathaniel Heintz (Rockefeller University) for providing the Lynx1−/− mice.

Footnotes

  • Authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This research was funded by a Traineeship, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development-Interdisciplinary Training in Systems and Developmental Biology and Birth Defects Grant T32H-D0-75735 (to M.R.S.); the Mindich Child Health and Development Institute Pilot Fund (to J.T.D. and H.M.); National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences Grant P30-ES-023515 (to J.T.D. and H.M.), the Knights Templar Eye Foundation (to H.M.); the March of Dimes (to H.M.); the Whitehall Foundation (to H.M.); and National Institutes of Health Grants R01-DK-098242 and U54-CA-189201 (to J.T.D.), R01-EY-024918, R01-EY-026053, and R21 MH106919 (to H.M.).

  • Received August 9, 2016.
  • Revision received November 1, 2016.
  • Accepted November 12, 2016.
  • Copyright © 2016 Smith et al.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. Atladóttir HO, Thorsen P, Schendel DE, Østergaard L, Lemcke S, Parner ET ( 2010) Association of hospitalization for infection in childhood with diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders: a Danish cohort study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 164:470–477. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.9 pmid:20439799
  2. Aton SJ, Broussard C, Dumoulin M, Seibt J, Watson A, Coleman T, Frank MG (2013) Visual experience and subsequent sleep induce sequential plastic changes in putative inhibitory and excitatory cortical neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:3101–3106. doi:10.1073/pnas.1208093110
  3. Banks WA, Robinson SM (2010) Minimal penetration of lipopolysaccharide across the murine blood–brain barrier. Brain Behav Immun 24:102–109. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2009.09.001 pmid:19735725
  4. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol 57:289–300.
  5. Benros ME, Sørensen HJ, Nielsen PR, Nordentoft M, Mortensen PB, Petersen L (2015) The association between infections and general cognitive ability in young men—a nationwide study. PLoS One 10:e0124005. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0124005 pmid:25970427
  6. Bona E, Andersson A-L, Blomgren K, Gilland E, Puka-Sundvall M, Gustafson K, Hagberg H (1999) Chemokine and inflammatory cell response to hypoxia-ischemia in immature rats. Pediatr Res 45:500–509. doi:10.1203/00006450-199904010-00008 pmid:10203141
  7. Bönn M (2016) hypergea: hypergeometric tests, version 1.2.3. R Foundation.
  8. Bukhari N, Burman PN, Hussein A, Demars MP, Sadahiro M, Brady DM, Tsirka SE, Russo SJ, Morishita H (2015) Unmasking proteolytic activity for adult visual cortex plasticity by the removal of Lynx1. J Neurosci 35:12693–12702. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4315-14.2015 pmid:26377459
  9. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2013) Incidence and trends of infection with pathogens transmitted commonly through food: Foodborne Diseases Active Surveillance Network, 10 U.S. sites, 1996-2012. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 62:283–287.
  10. Choi GB, Yim YS, Wong H, Kim S, Kim H, Kim SV, Hoeffer CA, Littman DR, Huh JR (2016) The maternal interleukin-17a pathway in mice promotes autism-like phenotypes in offspring. Science 351:933–939. doi:10.1126/science.aad0314 pmid:26822608
  11. Dalman C, Allebeck P, Gunnell D, Harrison G, Kristensson K, Lewis G, Lofving S, Rasmussen F, Wicks S, Karlsson H (2008) Infections in the CNS during childhood and the risk of subsequent psychotic illness: a cohort study of more than one million Swedish subjects. Am J Psychiatry 165:59–65. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07050740
  12. Datwani A, McConnell MJ, Kanold PO, Micheva KD, Busse B, Shamloo M, Smith SJ, Shatz CJ (2009) Classical MHCI molecules regulate retinogeniculate refinement and limit ocular dominance plasticity. Neuron 64:463–470. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.10.015
  13. Dawood FS, Fiore A, Kamimoto L, Bramley A, Reingold A, Gershman K, Meek J, Hadler J, Arnold KE, Ryan P, Lynfield R, Morin C, Mueller M, Baumbach J, Zansky S, Bennett NM, Thomas A, Schaffner W, Kirschke D, Finelli L (2010) Burden of seasonal influenza hospitalization in children, United States, 2003 to 2008. J Pediatr 157:808–814. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.05.012
  14. De Simoni MG, Perego C, Ravizza T, Moneta D, Conti M, Marchesi F, De Luigi A, Garattini S, Vezzani A (2000) Inflammatory cytokines and related genes are induced in the rat hippocampus by limbic status epilepticus. Eur J Neurosci 12:2623–2633. pmid:10947836
  15. Dudley JT, Tibshirani R, Deshpande T, Butte AJ (2009) Disease signatures are robust across tissues and experiments. Mol Syst Biol 5:307. doi:10.1038/msb.2009.66
  16. Eklind S, Hagberg H, Wang X, Sävman K, Leverin A-L, Hedtjärn M, Mallard C (2006) Effect of lipopolysaccharide on global gene expression in the immature rat brain. Pediatr Res 60:161–168. doi:10.1203/01.pdr.0000228323.32445.7d pmid:16864697
  17. Fagerland MW, Lydersen S, Laake P (2015) Recommended confidence intervals for two independent binomial proportions. Stat Methods Med Res 24:224–254. doi:10.1177/0962280211415469 pmid:21996567
  18. Failor S, Nguyen V, Darcy DP, Cang J, Wendland MF, Stryker MP, McQuillen PS (2010) Neonatal cerebral hypoxia–ischemia impairs plasticity in rat visual cortex. J Neurosci 30:81–92. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5656-08.2010 pmid:20053890
  19. Fox SE, Levitt P, Nelson CA III. (2010) How the timing and quality of early experiences influence the development of brain architecture. Child Dev 81:28–40. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01380.x pmid:20331653
  20. Galic MA, Riazi K, Heida JG, Mouihate A, Fournier NM, Spencer SJ, Kalynchuk LE, Teskey GC, Pittman QJ (2008) Postnatal inflammation increases seizure susceptibility in adult rats. J Neurosci 28:6904–6913. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1901-08.2008 pmid:18596165
  21. Gordon JA, Stryker MP (1996) Experience-dependent plasticity of binocular responses in the primary visual cortex of the mouse. J Neurosci 16:3274–3286. pmid:8627365
  22. Greifzu F, Schmidt S, Schmidt K-F, Kreikemeier K, Witte OW, Löwel S (2011) Global impairment and therapeutic restoration of visual plasticity mechanisms after a localized cortical stroke. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:15450–15455. doi:10.1073/pnas.1016458108 pmid:21873250
  23. Guan Z, Fang J (2006) Peripheral immune activation by lipopolysaccharide decreases neurotrophins in the cortex and hippocampus in rats. Brain Behav Immun 20:64–71. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2005.04.005
  24. Harlow EG, Till SM, Russell TA, Wijetunge LS, Kind P, Contractor A (2010) Critical period plasticity is disrupted in the barrel cortex of Fmr1 knockout mice. Neuron 65:385–398. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.024 pmid:20159451
  25. Hodos RA, Kidd BA, Shameer K, Readhead BP, Dudley JT (2016) In silico methods for drug repurposing and pharmacology. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Syst Biol Med 8:186–210. doi:10.1002/wsbm.1337 pmid:27080087
  26. Hong F, Breitling R, McEntee CW, Wittner BS, Nemhauser JL, Chory J (2006) RankProd: a bioconductor package for detecting differentially expressed genes in meta-analysis. Bioinformatics 22:2825–2827. doi:10.1093/bioinformatics/btl476 pmid:16982708
  27. Huang ZJ, Kirkwood A, Pizzorusso T, Porciatti V, Morales B, Bear MF, Maffei L, Tonegawa S (1999) BDNF regulates the maturation of inhibition and the critical period of plasticity in mouse visual cortex. Cell 98:739–755. pmid:10499792
  28. Jang E, Lee S, Kim J-H, Kim J-H, Seo J-W, Lee W-H, Mori K, Nakao K, Suk K (2013) Secreted protein lipocalin-2 promotes microglial M1 polarization. FASEB J 27:1176–1190. doi:10.1096/fj.12-222257 pmid:23207546
  29. Johnson JS, Newport EL (1989) Critical period effects in second language learning: the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cogn Psychol 21:60–99. pmid:2920538
  30. Kaneko M, Stellwagen D, Malenka RC, Stryker MP (2008) Tumor necrosis factor-α mediates one component of competitive, experience-dependent plasticity in developing visual cortex. Neuron 58:673–680. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.023
  31. Khandaker GM, Pearson RM, Zammit S, Lewis G, Jones PB (2014) Association of serum interleukin 6 and c-reactive protein in childhood with depression and psychosis in young adult life: a population-based longitudinal study. JAMA Psychiatry 71:1121–1128. pmid:PMC4561502
  32. Knijnenburg TA, Wessels LFA, Reinders MJT, Shmulevich I (2009) Fewer permutations, more accurate P-values. Bioinformatics 25:i161–i168. doi:10.1093/bioinformatics/btp211 pmid:19477983
  33. Kuhlman SJ, Olivas ND, Tring E, Ikrar T, Xu X, Trachtenberg JT (2013) A disinhibitory microcircuit initiates critical-period plasticity in the visual cortex. Nature 501:543–546. doi:10.1038/nature12485
  34. Layé S, Parnet P, Goujon E, Dantzer R (1994) Peripheral administration of lipopolysaccharide induces the expression of cytokine transcripts in the brain and pituitary of mice. Mol Brain Res 27:157–162. pmid:7877446
  35. LeBlanc JJ, Fagiolini M (2011) Autism: a “critical period” disorder? Neural Plast 2011:921680. doi:10.1155/2011/921680
  36. Lee FS, Heimer H, Giedd JN, Lein ES, Šestan N, Weinberger DR, Casey BJ (2014) Adolescent mental health—opportunity and obligation. Science 346:547–549. doi:10.1126/science.1260497 pmid:25359951
  37. Levelt CN, Hübener M (2012) Critical-period plasticity in the visual cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 35:309–330. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113813 pmid:22462544
  38. Lewis TL, Maurer D (2005) Multiple sensitive periods in human visual development: evidence from visually deprived children. Dev Psychobiol 46:163–183. doi:10.1002/dev.20055 pmid:15772974
  39. Marom T, Tan A, Wilkinson GS, Pierson KS, Freeman JL, Chonmaitree T (2014) Trends in otitis media-related health care use in the United States, 2001-2011. JAMA Pediatr 168:68–75. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.3924 pmid:24276262
  40. Mataga N, Nagai N, Hensch TK (2002) Permissive proteolytic activity for visual cortical plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:7717–7721. doi:10.1073/pnas.102088899 pmid:12032349
  41. Milnerwood AJ, Cummings DM, Dallérac GM, Brown JY, Vatsavayai SC, Hirst MC, Rezaie P, Murphy KP (2006) Early development of aberrant synaptic plasticity in a mouse model of Huntington’s disease. Hum Mol Genet 15:1690–1703.
  42. Miwa JM, Stevens TR, King SL, Caldarone BJ, Ibanez-Tallon I, Xiao C, Fitzsimonds RM, Pavlides C, Lester HA, Picciotto MR (2006) The prototoxin lynx1 acts on nicotinic acetylcholine receptors to balance neuronal activity and survival in vivo. Neuron 51:587–600. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.025 pmid:16950157
  43. Morishita H, Hensch TK (2008) Critical period revisited: impact on vision. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18:101–107. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2008.05.009 pmid:18534841
  44. Morishita H, Miwa JM, Heintz N, Hensch TK (2010) Lynx1, a cholinergic brake, limits plasticity in adult visual cortex. Science 330:1238–1240. doi:10.1126/science.1195320 pmid:21071629
  45. Murphy KP, Carter RJ, Lione LA, Mangiarini L, Mahal A, Bates GP, Dunnett SB, Morton AJ (2000) Abnormal synaptic plasticity and impaired spatial cognition in mice transgenic for exon 1 of the human Huntington’s disease mutation. J Neurosci 20:5115–5123.
  46. Nabel EM, Morishita H (2013) Regulating critical period plasticity: insight from the visual system to fear circuitry for therapeutic interventions. Front Psychiatry 4:146. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00146
  47. Nagakura I, Wart AV, Petravicz J, Tropea D, Sur M (2014) STAT1 regulates the homeostatic component of visual cortical plasticity via an AMPA receptor-mediated mechanism. J Neurosci 34:10256–10263. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0189-14.2014 pmid:25080587
  48. Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, Smyke AT, Guthrie D (2007) Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Science 318:1937–1940. doi:10.1126/science.1143921 pmid:18096809
  49. Nikolopoulos TP, O’Donoghue GM, Archbold S (1999) Age at implantation: its importance in pediatric cochlear implantation. Laryngoscope 109:595–599. doi:10.1097/00005537-199904000-00014 pmid:10201747
  50. Reh R, Hensch TK (2014) Transient gamma power peaks upon monocular deprivation during critical period plasticity. Soc Neurosci Abstr 40:819.16. Washington, D.C.
  51. Richwine AF, Sparkman NL, Dilger RN, Buchanan JB, Johnson RW (2009) Cognitive deficits in interleukin-10-deficient mice after peripheral injection of lipopolysaccharide. Brain Behav Immun 23:794–802. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2009.02.020 pmid:19272439
  52. Sayyah M, Javad-Pour M, Ghazi-Khansari M (2003) The bacterial endotoxin lipopolysaccharide enhances seizure susceptibility in mice: involvement of proinflammatory factors nitric oxide and prostaglandins. Neuroscience 122:1073–1080. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.08.043
  53. Schnydrig S, Korner L, Landweer S, Ernst B, Walker G, Otten U, Kunz D (2007) Peripheral lipopolysaccharide administration transiently affects expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor, corticotropin and proopiomelanocortin in mouse brain. Neurosci Lett 429:69–73. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2007.09.067
  54. Schorr EA, Fox NA, Wassenhove V, Knudsen EI (2005) Auditory-visual fusion in speech perception in children with cochlear implants. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:18748–18750. doi:10.1073/pnas.0508862102 pmid:16339316
  55. Schroeter M, Jander S, Stoll G (2002) Non-invasive induction of focal cerebral ischemia in mice by photothrombosis of cortical microvessels: characterization of inflammatory responses. J Neurosci Methods 117:43–49. doi:10.1016/S0165-0270(02)00072-9
  56. Sipe GO, Lowery RL, Tremblay M-È, Kelly EA, Lamantia CE, Majewska AK (2016) Microglial P2Y12 is necessary for synaptic plasticity in mouse visual cortex. Nat Commun 7:10905. doi:10.1038/ncomms10905 pmid:26948129
    1. Gentleman R,
    2. Huber W,
    3. Carey VJ,
    4. Irizarry RA,
    5. Dudoit S
    Smyth GK (2005) limma: linear models for microarray data. In: Bioinformatics and computational biology solutions using R and Bioconductor (Gentleman R, Huber W, Carey VJ, Irizarry RA, Dudoit S), pp 397–420 New York: Springer. New York: Springer, p 397–420.
  57. Steullet P, Cabungcal JH, Monin A, Dwir D, O’Donnell P, Cuenod M, Do KQ (2016) Redox dysregulation, neuroinflammation, and NMDA receptor hypofunction: a “central hub” in schizophrenia pathophysiology? Schizophr Res 176:41–51. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2014.06.021
  58. Subramanian A, Tamayo P, Mootha VK, Mukherjee S, Ebert BL, Gillette MA, Paulovich A, Pomeroy SL, Golub TR, Lander ES, Mesirov JP (2005) Gene set enrichment analysis: a knowledge-based approach for interpreting genome-wide expression profiles. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:15545–15550. doi:10.1073/pnas.0506580102 pmid:16199517
  59. Syken J, GrandPre T, Kanold PO, Shatz CJ (2006) PirB restricts ocular-dominance plasticity in visual cortex. Science 313:1795–1800. doi:10.1126/science.1128232 pmid:16917027
    1. Merzenich M,
    2. Nahum M,
    3. Van Vleet TM
    Takesian AE, Hensch TK (2013) Balancing plasticity/stability across brain development. In: Changing brains: applying brain plasticity to advance and recover human ability, Chap 1 (Merzenich M, Nahum M, Van Vleet TM, eds), pp 3–34. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Elsevier, p 3–34.
  60. Tropea D, Giacometti E, Wilson NR, Beard C, McCurry C, Fu DD, Flannery R, Jaenisch R, Sur M (2009) Partial reversal of Rett syndrome-like symptoms in MeCP2 mutant mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:2029–2034. doi:10.1073/pnas.0812394106 pmid:19208815
  61. Usdin MT, Shelbourne PF, Myers RM, Madison DV (1999) Impaired synaptic plasticity in mice carrying the Huntington’s disease mutation. Hum Mol Genet 8:839–846. pmid:10196373
  62. Vezzani A, Granata T (2005) Brain inflammation in epilepsy: experimental and clinical evidence. Epilepsia 46:1724–1743. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2005.00298.x pmid:16302852
  63. Wang B-S, Sarnaik R, Cang J (2010) Critical period plasticity matches binocular orientation preference in the visual cortex. Neuron 65:246–256. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.002 pmid:20152130
  64. Weber-Stadlbauer U, Richetto J, Labouesse MA, Bohacek J, Mansuy IM, Meyer U (2017) Transgenerational transmission and modification of pathological traits induced by prenatal immune activation. Mol Psychiatry 22:102–112.
  65. Weinberger DR (1987) Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 44:660–669. pmid:3606332
  66. Werker JF, Hensch TK (2015) Critical periods in speech perception: new directions. Annu Rev Psychol 66:173–196. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104 pmid:25251488
  67. Wiesel TN, Hubel DH (1963) Single-cell responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye. J Neurophysiol 26:1003–1017.
  68. Yashiro K, Riday TT, Condon KH, Roberts AC, Bernardo DR, Prakash R, Weinberg RJ, Ehlers MD, Philpot BD (2009) Ube3a is required for experience-dependent maturation of the neocortex. Nat Neurosci 12:777–783. doi:10.1038/nn.2327 pmid:19430469
  69. Zhang S-D, Gant TW (2008) A simple and robust method for connecting small-molecule drugs using gene-expression signatures. BMC Bioinformatics 9:258. doi:10.1186/1471-2105-9-258

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Margaret McCarthy, Univ of Maryland School of Medicine

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Henner Koch.

Two reviewers appreciate the significance of the work but ask for several points of clarification as well as some moderation of the interpretation. There is no need for additional experiments but the authors should provide a comprehensive and detailed response to the reviewers comments and revise the manuscript accordingly.

Reviewer #1:

This manuscript applies bioinformatics to identify gene signatures correlated with the transcriptomic change occurring between P29 and >P90. They interpret their data posing that the change in plasticity occurring at the end of the critical period is the most important functional change associated with this transcriptomic change. They find that inflammation is the biological function in common between age-dependent regulation of transcription and many disease signatures. Moreover, inflammation associated pathways are also regulated in adult Lynx Ko mice that display juvenile OD plasticity. Finally they test the role of inflammation by inducing systemic inflammation with IP injection of LPS in P26 mice and finding an impaired plasticity.

The approach is original and the results would be interesting, however there are many parts especially concerning Fig 2-4 that are not clearly described in the text and make difficult to judge the data.

In particular in the results section describing fig. 3 it is stated that

"8 of 13 (61.5%) genes predicted to be anti-correlated to the plasticity signature showed a complete reversal in their differential expression pattern after LPS administration (all reversed genes p less than 5 x 10-4d, t-test of deltaCTs, n = 5 mice per group) (Figure 3)" It is unclear where is this prediction coming from. If it is from GSE3253 (only stated in fig legend) these data were obtained by LPS injection in aged mice. Lowel's lab showed that OD plasticity continues to decrease with age, and aged mice are particularly impervious to monocular deprivation. Is the anticorrelation present at the whole transcriptome level? Moreover, is the fact that 13 genes are reversed significant? Why validation (done in juvenile mice) excludes few of the 13 genes? For example, judging from fig. 4b RT-PCR analysis BDNF is downregulated by LPS, however fig 3b would predict upregulation.

In the analysis of the Lynx KO the authors claim that inflammation is again involved. However, it should be clarified whether the regulated genes are the same (or significantly enriched with) genes of the P29 vs adult comparison albeit with opposite regulation, or whether different genes (even if belonging to similar categories) are involved.

Considering the results from Lowel's lab that a cortical lesion impairs OD plasticity the LPS ephys data are not so unexpected although I accept that they are needed to logically close the argument.

"Mice that experienced opening of the sutured eye or that had poor recordings (less than 10 cells/mouse or less than 3 penetrations / mouse or lack of positive identification of monocular zone and secondary visual cortex) were excluded from further analysis". How did the author perform identification of monocular and secondary visual cortices?

Reviewer #2:

In this study the authors investigated the correlation between the transcriptome of a large number of diseases taken from public microarray data and the "plasticity signature" of the V1 area of the cortex (an area that is known to undergo a critical period with plasticity in juvenile mice). The plasticity signature was generated by comparing the transcriptome of juvenile and adult mice and also to adult Linx (-/-) mice (which have also a plasticity response in adulthood). They then used this plasticity signature to match it to the disease signatures of the public microarray data.In this first step the authors identified inflammation pathways to be related to a dysregulation of plasticity.

In a second step the authors used an experimental approach (LPS exposure) to verify their hypothesis concluded from the first step by artificially inducing an inflammatory response in mice and investigate, if similar signatures can be detected in the experimental model. Last the authors used an in vivo measurement of plasticity to show that the LPS exposure is suppressing the cortical plasticity response (in V1) in juvenile mice determined by a monocular deprivation test. The manuscript is well written and the overall findings of the study are interesting. The methods are appropriate and some important control experiments were performed. However, I have some concerns of the methods used and the interpretation of the data and think the caveats and limitations of the approach should be clearly stated in the discussion.

Concerns:

1) The samples taken to generate the set of disease signatures stem from public microarray data and are derived from different tissues and also species (Table 2). The authors cite and propose that the study by Dudley et al., 2009 showed, that this is a valid approach. However, even in this original study, the authors provide some hints for limitations of the approach: "We acknowledge several limitations to the approach taken by this study. Foremost, we acknowledge that experimental investigators will generally draw samples from tissues that are relevant to the disease condition under study. Therefore, we cannot assert that disease concordance would be maintained in samples drawn from tissues that would not commonly be chosen in the study of a disease". So I think also in the discussion of this study this should be clearly stated to give the reader a fair impression of the caveats of the approach.

2) The authors provide a nice experimental set to strengthen the hypothesis, that inflammation is sufficient to suppress plasticity in the model used. It would be interesting to know why the tissue samples were taken 4 hours after LPS injection and the in vivo measurements were done 3 days after LPS injection. Was it technically not possible to also take the tissue after the in vivo measurements? Maybe other signatures would be present at this time point? Also it might be that the plasticity response is still normal after 4 hours, since some plasticity changes occur only over longer time periods (i.e homeostatic plasticity, which was shown to be influenced by TNF-alpha and other inflammatory molecules.)

3) The identified diseases include acute and chronic diseases. Several studies have proposed chronic inflammation as a potential cause of dysregulation of plasticity. Posttraumatic epilepsy, for example, has been linked to a dysregulation of the plasticity due to inflammation of neuronal networks (De Simoni et al. 2000, Vezzani - 2005). It would be nice to discuss the differential effects of chronic vs. acute inflamation on plasticity.

4) Why did also diseases that have no clear neurological phenotype show a strong correlation in their transcriptomes found in this study. I think this also needs to be addressed in the discussion.

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.