Abstract
Human brains are capable of modulating innate activities to adapt to novel environments and tasks; for sensorimotor neural system this means acquisition of a rich repertoire of activity patterns that improve behavioral performance. To directly map the process of acquiring the neural repertoire during tasks onto performance improvement, we analyzed net neural populational activity during the learning of its voluntary modulation by brain-computer interface (BCI) operation in female and male humans. The recorded whole-head high-density scalp electroencephalograms (EEGs) were subjected to dimensionality reduction algorithm to capture changes in cortical activity patterns represented by the synchronization of neuronal oscillations during adaptation. Although the preserved variance of targeted features in the reduced dimensions was 20%, we found systematic interactions between the activity patterns and BCI classifiers that detected motor attempt; the neural manifold derived in the embedded space was stretched along with motor-related features of EEG by model-based fixed classifiers but not with adaptive classifiers that were constantly recalibrated to user activity. Moreover, the manifold was deformed to be orthogonal to the boundary by de novo classifiers with a fixed decision boundary based on biologically unnatural features. Collectively, the flexibility of human cortical signaling patterns (i.e., neural plasticity) is only induced by operation of a BCI whose classifier required fixed activities, and the adaptation could be induced even the requirement is not consistent with biologically natural responses. These principles of neural adaptation at a macroscopic level may underlie the ability of humans to learn wide-ranging behavioral repertoires and adapt to novel environments.
- brain-computer interface
- de novo learning
- neural plasticity
- nonlinear dimensionality reduction
- sensorimotor activity
Significance Statement
We investigated adaption of macroscopic neural activities during brain-computer interface (BCI) operation to directly map the process of acquiring the neural repertoire for performance improvement. When the classifier incorporated in BCI was fixed and based on the desynchronization of neural oscillations, the distribution of activity patterns (neural manifold) showed the improved separability along with the motor-related component of electroencephalograms (EEGs) to improve BCI controllability. Meanwhile the adaptive classifier constantly fitted to current user activity did not elicit such adaptation of neural activity patterns. Moreover, even the classifiers based on biologically unnatural model induced the adaptation, captured by deformation of neural manifold. Neural adaptation processes at a macroscopic level may underlie the ability of humans to learn wide-ranging behavioral repertoires.
Introduction
Human beings can sophisticate motor plans and subsequent actions to dynamically interact with the external environment (Burdet et al., 2001; Todorov and Jordan, 2002; Scott, 2004). One surprising demonstration is an adaptation to changes in the properties of physical interfaces such as the use of novel tools or loss and augmentation of body parts by tuning distributed sensorimotor circuitries to achieve smooth interaction with surroundings (Imamizu et al., 2000; Quallo et al., 2009; Penaloza and Nishio, 2018; Mehring et al., 2019; Choi et al., 2020; Kieliba et al., 2021; Rossi et al., 2021).
The internal representation of sensorimotor adaptation has been sought by electrophysiology and neuroimaging techniques (Karni et al., 1995; Nudo et al., 1996; Kleim et al., 2004; Diedrichsen et al., 2005; Berlot et al., 2020). In particular, the primary motor cortex (M1) exhibits covariance patterns of multiple neural units, namely, the neural manifold which reliably represents ongoing behavior and its correction (Gallego et al., 2018, 2020; Perich et al., 2018; Shenoy and Kao, 2021). Moreover, direct mapping of behavior and single neuron activity patterns achieved with brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) revealed monkeys are capable of endogenous modulation of the patterns inside the manifold, but not those outside (Sadtler et al., 2014). Although the conception of the neural manifold describes cell-neuron level principles of learning within a single local region (Golub et al., 2018; Chaudhuri et al., 2019; Oby et al., 2019), little is known about the constraints on the adaptation of the macroscopic sensorimotor system, that is shaped by the synchronization and desynchronization of net populational neural activities across multiple brain regions (Wander et al., 2013; Fries, 2015). Because the summation of activity of locally interconnected −107 neurons cancels out the property of a single neuron and only maintains their synchronized activities which mediate information processing in the human cortical system, the principles governing the cortical adaptation processes at the macroscopic scale are putatively distinct from the local unit activities in a single region (Kelso, 2012; Tognoli and Kelso, 2014).
To investigate human adaptability at the sensorimotor network level, we used BCI operation tasks based on scalp electroencephalograms (EEG) with a variety of incorporated classifiers (Fig. 1A). Since users attempted to move a virtual object by exploring mental actions that effectively modulates EEG signals to control BCI, this experimental paradigm allows us to examine the relationship between BCI properties and process in the cortical adaptation (i.e., changes in neural activity patterns to fit the rule of BCI classifier). As shown in Figure 1B, we specifically hypothesized two distinct adaptation processes induced to improve BCI operation performance: (1) separation: rescaling of cortical activity patterns that increases geometric distances between two brain states and (2) rotation: deforming of the configuration of two brain states induced by changes in the whole-brain activity patterns. The former represents changes in the separability along with the targeted EEG feature and the latter represents rotational changes in the activity patterns toward perpendicular to the BCI classifier, respectively. The geometric analysis in the dimensionality-reduced space offers the opportunities to capture the reorganization of whole-brain neural dynamics.
To test whether the cortical adaptation process is influenced by BCI configurations, we employed three types of BCIs whose classifiers were based on different rules: model-based, de novo and adaptive classifiers. The model-based and de novo classifiers were based on fixed scalp EEG feature to induce adaptation of neural activity patterns. The model-based classifier was based on hand-area motor cortical activities and users were informed a mental strategy to successfully control BCIs, meanwhile, the de novo classifier was based on temporo-parietal activities and users were encouraged to explore the suitable strategy (Shibata et al., 2011). The adaptive classifier was designed to adapt to the current brain activity patterns using the whole-head EEG signals as input, facilitating the classifier-side adaptation by a block-by block calibration. We hypothesized the fixed type BCIs require reorganization of whole-brain activity patterns while the adaptive BCIs rather induce the classifier-side adaptation by intermittent calibration. Difference in the adaptation process would be characterized by geometric changes in the low-dimensional representation of neural activity patterns; BCI operation with fixed types of classifiers would lead the progress in the separation and rotation because of enhanced discriminability of brain states along with the EEG feature for classifier input. Meanwhile, the adaptive BCI would not induce those changes since the classifier-side constant calibration can optimize the classifier to fit the current brain state without changing user-side activities.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Twenty-one neurologically healthy adults (9 females, 12 males, mean age: 22.6 ± 3.23) naive to BCI operation participated in this experiment. The appropriate sample size for this study was determined by an a priori power analysis (α = 0.05, 1-β = 0.8, two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank tests) focusing on the deforming effect induced by de novo BCI. The statistical package G*Power 3 (Faul et al., 2007) was used to estimate the sample size that shows large Cohen’s d = 0.90 reported in the previous EEG-based neurofeedback literatures (Soekadar et al., 2015; Hayashi et al., 2020).
All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were asked to provide written informed consent before participating in the experiment. This study was conducted according to the ethics of the Declaration of Helsinki. The experimental protocol was approved by the ethical committee of the affiliated organization (Approval number 2020-36).
Experimental setup
Participants were seated on a comfortable chair in a quiet room. A display was placed about one meter in front of the chair to provide task instructions and visual feedback from BCIs. EEG signals during the experiment were acquired with a 128-channel HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net (HCGSN, EGI). The layout of channels followed the international 10–10 electrode positions shown in Figure 2A (Luu and Ferree, 2005). The reference channel was set to Cz. The impedance of all channels was maintained below 50 kΩ throughout the experiment. The EEG data were collected with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz.
Experimental procedure
Participants underwent 16 BCI operation blocks comprised of 20 trials. All experimental procedures were conducted within 2 h to guarantee the reversibility of any potential effect of induced unnatural neural plasticity and investigate the initial phase of learning to operate the BCIs (Marins et al., 2019; Mehler et al., 2019; Hayashi et al., 2020). After every two blocks, participants were given a break of up to 5 min. Participants were randomly allocated to one of the three classifiers without informing the configuration of BCI, the existence of multiple types of classifiers and the allocated type of classifier was used throughout experiment (also, see below, Online processing of EEG signals).
A trial began with a 5-s “Rest” period and a 5-s “Imagine” and a 3-s “Break” period followed (Fig. 2B). During the “Rest” period, participants were instructed to relax without having any specific thoughts and with opened eyes. In the “Imagine” period, participants were instructed to perform motor imagery tasks based on the allocated classifiers. Participants with the model-based and adaptive classifiers imagined extending the right-hand while those with the de novo classifier tried moving a tail to match the attempted movement with the object on display (Fig. 2C). Since tail moving is not intuitive for human beings, at the beginning of the experiment, participants were encouraged to explore strategies that enables better controllability of the BCI. The strategy adopted in each block was freely determined by each participant, but they were instructed to try to use the same strategy throughout one block to acquire sufficient data and report the adopted strategy at the end of each block. Since the visual feedback for participants of the model-based and adaptive BCIs were configured to increase grasp aperture when classifier detect the motor attempt (Fig. 2C, left panel), they tried to keep the virtual hand opened during “Imagine” period and closed “Rest” period. Likewise, those of de novo BCIs were configured to move the tail toward left (Fig. 2C, right panel), participants tried to keep the virtual tail left side of the display during “Imagine” period and right side “Rest” period. Participants were asked not to exert overt movement during the feedback period and its compliance was visually inspected by the experimenters.
The performance of each trial was quantified by scores provided by BCI and participants were encouraged to maximize the culminative sum of score within a block. Scores were determined by the predicted presence/absence of motor attempt by classifiers. The absence of motor attempt during “Rest” periods and the presence during “Imagine” periods increased scores (reward), while the opposite prediction decreased (punishment). The changing rates of these scores were pertinent to the metrics used for feedback by each classifier and were regulated linearly to fit the score range from −100 to 100. For the adaptive classifier, the common spatial pattern (CSP)-support vector machine (SVM) model was trained with data from the previous block and the trained model was used in the next block. Note that the first block of users allocated to the adaptive classifier was identical to that of the model-based, to collect a dataset for the adaptive classifier training.
Online processing of EEG signals
To test the initial adaptation process during BCI use, we prepared three types of binary EEG classifier that detects presence of human motor attempt from based on different EEG features. The following processing was conducted using MATLAB R2019a (The MathWorks, Inc.) and Unity (version 2019.2.4f1, Unity Technologies). Online acquired EEG signals were processed with a 1651-point, minimum-phase, FIR 8- to 30-Hz bandpass temporal filter and then processed with one of the three types of BCI classifiers. Online processed EEG signals were used to detect the presence of motor attempt with one of the three types of classifiers: model-based, adaptive, or de novo. Each classifier was designed with different rules, and electrodes of interest were defined as shown in Figure 2A. During experiment, users were instructed to use one of three BCIs at the time course defined as Figure 2B (also, see above, Experimental procedure).
The model-based classifier was constructed based on those used in sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) BCIs (Buch et al., 2008; Kraus et al., 2016). Because accumulated evidence suggests that event-related desynchronization of SMR (SMR-ERD) contralateral to the hand that attempted to move reflects the excitability of SM1 (Hummel et al., 2002; Takemi et al., 2013; Naros et al., 2020), EEG signals around the left SM1 (i.e., channel C3) were only used to detect the attempted movement. In online processing, a large Laplacian filter was applied to EEG signals from channel C3 to extract sensorimotor activity (McFarland et al., 1997; Tsuchimoto et al., 2021). Subsequently, the band power of SMR (SMR-power; 8–13 Hz) was extracted by Fourier transform with a 1-s window and Hamming window function. The magnitude of SMR-ERD (dB) was computed from the obtained SMR-power with the following formula:
The de novo classifier had a fixed classifier plane as did the model-based classifier, however, its characteristics were biologically unnatural; the de novo classifier was based on EEG signals around the temporo-parietal region (i.e., channel Cz) that are associated with not only sensorimotor, but also attentional features (Benedek et al., 2014; Misselhorn et al., 2019). Actively exploring suitable mental strategies, users attempted to move their body or a visual object on the display during the BCI task. However, the motor imagery of corresponding body parts at the region (i.e., foot) and increased attention do not contribute to the spectral power attenuation in the α band (8–13 Hz) required by the classifier. Specifically, since the α band power was increased by the motor attempt of moving the feet or by internal attention at the targeted channel (Pfurtscheller et al., 2006; Benedek et al., 2014), such intrinsic responses did not contribute to the BCI operation, Online computed ERD magnitude with the procedure identical to that from channel C3 in the model-based classifier was exploited to decode the absence/presence of attempted movement and index for neurofeedback. The angle of tail was discretized to 100 steps (0 dB: right limit, 10 dB: left limit). Note that the rules for object movement were identical to those of the model-based classifier.
Lastly, the adaptive classifier was constructed using whole-head scalp EEG signals based on a common spatial pattern (CSP) algorithm and a support vector machine (SVM; Pfurtscheller et al., 2006; Blankertz et al., 2007). To adapt to the current activity patterns of users, CSP components that maximize the separability of the two conditions “Rest” and “Imagine” were trained at the end of each block. SVM classifiers were constructed to perform a binary classification of the two conditions based on 6 CSP components. Although the CSP-based feature extraction did not employ time-frequency transformation for spectral power calculation used in the model-based and de novo, users of adaptive BCIs were also required to perform kinesthetic motor imagery which modulates spectral power of scalp EEG. The posterior probability for a data point classified as presence of motor attempt was used as an index for neurofeedback; the index for the adaptive classifier was defined to be linearly related to the posterior probability in the range of 50% to 100%. Note that the rules for object movement and for obtaining scores were identical to those in the other two types of classifiers.
Evaluation of BCI performance
For each participant, online-calculated scores were individually subjected to linear regression analysis to summarize whether performance of participant improved over blocks for each classifier (Gruzelier, 2014; Kober et al., 2018; Witte et al., 2018). The score obtained during a given block was used as a dependent variable and block number was used as a predictor valuable. If scores increased during the experiment, the regression coefficient for the predictor valuable was positive. After the regression coefficients were derived from scores of each participant, they were subjected to a group-by-group Wilcoxon rank-sum test with a false discovery rate correction to test whether the obtained regression coefficients were significantly different from zero (Benjamini–Hochberg method; Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). If significant positive shift of the slopes were observed, the result indicated systematic progress of controllability improvement for the BCI. However, note that the comparison of the learning rate across groups are not applicable because of the difference in score calculation procedure. Moreover, to capture the difference in performance at the beginning and end of experiment, acquired scores were compared with Wilcoxon signed-rank test for first and last four blocks of each BCI operation (early and late period, respectively).
Offline EEG preprocess
The recorded EEG signals were first preprocessed with EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) to reject artifacts and enhance the computational efficiency with downsampling (Bigdely-Shamlo et al., 2015) The raw EEG data were filtered with a zero-phase 1–45 Hz FIR bandpass filter, downsampled to 100 Hz and bad channels identified by clean raw data plugin were removed from further analysis. The removed channels were interpolated spherically to minimize a potential bias when re-referencing the electrodes to a common average reference. Subsequently, large-amplitude artifacts caused by blinking or head displacement were removed with artifact subspace reconstruction algorithm (Kothe and Makeig, 2013). The electrodes were then re-referenced to the common average reference to extract activity specific to the electrodes (McFarland et al., 1997).
The continuous EEG data were then segmented into trials to evaluate the middle 8-s periods of the online BCI training trials (i.e., the last 4 s of the “Rest” period and the first 4 s of the “Imagine” period). To obtain the independent EEG components of the segmented dataset, we used adaptive mixture independent component analysis (AMICA; Palmer et al., 2011). Finally, an automatic artifact rejection was applied using ICLabel that distinguished genuine EEG components from artifacts induced by eye, muscle, heart, line noise, and channel noises (Pion-Tonachini et al., 2019).
To investigate cortical adaptation processes during brain-computer interfacing, the band-power features were used as a raw-vector that represents instantaneous overall brain state. Computed band-power from each EEG channel was subdivided into five functionally distinct frequency bands (δ: 1–4 Hz, θ: 4–8 Hz, α: 8–13 Hz, β: 13–31 Hz, γ: 31–45 Hz; Hayashi et al., 2020). The averaged band-power was log-transformed and normalized to the z score in a trial-by-trial manner to cancel baseline drifting. Thereby, the original number of dimensions of the feature vector
D was
Feature extraction of EEG-dataset using t-distributed stochastic neighbor embedding (t-SNE) algorithm
The preprocessed EEG dataset (645 × 11,520 matrix) was subjected to a subject-by-subject t-SNE analysis, which converted the pairwise distances between data points in the original feature space to conditional probabilities (Van Der Maaten and Hinton, 2008). The t-SNE algorithm minimized the Kullback–Leibler divergence representing the distance between the conditional probability in the original and embedded space, where conditional probability that the data points
The t-SNE-based dimensionality reduction and quantitative analysis in embedded space
Feature extraction using dimensionality reduction is popularly conducted for high-dimensional neural data across modalities (Cunningham and Yu, 2014; Lord et al., 2019). The t-SNE algorithm we employed for dimensionality reduction is advantageous for geometric evaluation as it preserves original distances in the embedded space. Because t-SNE unfolds the nonlinear structure of a given dataset, the linear distance in the embedded space can be interpreted as an approximation of geometric distance in the original space. It illustrates how different one brain activity pattern is from another; however, it should be noted that to properly interpret the results, (1) distance scales in the embedded space were rearranged and were variable across iterations of t-SNE, (2) distance scales in different clusters might have differed, and (3) direct comparisons of distances between clusters were not acceptable because distances within two clusters were arbitrary. To deal with the above concerns, two approaches were adopted: (1) data points were bridged to prevent the formation of multiple clusters, and (2) statistical distances, namely, Hotelling’s t-squared statistical values, were used instead of Euclidean metrics. Because distances between nearby points are well preserved in embedded space, the distance scale of distant points were kept similar for enough data points, which acts as a bridge and prevents the formation of sparse multiple clusters. We also adopted the concept of “short-circuiting” (Lee and Verleysen, 2005) by constructing the feature vectors with overlapped time-windows so that points were smoothly connected, and all data acquired from single participants were subjected to t-SNE algorithm at once. Thus, distances from point to point shared the same scale across all points (i.e., only one cluster was generated in embedded space as shown in Fig. 3B).
Hotelling’s t-squared statistic was adopted as the distance metrics between two group of points (Hotelling, 1992). Assume
x and
y are two groups of points lying in a p-dimensional space,
Hotelling’s t-squared statistic is suitable for measurements of statistical distance in the t-SNE-embedded space, as they were invariant to the distance scale. The distribution of
To normalize the distribution, the square root of
The vector representing the directional relationship between two classes was defined as a 3D vector
tVec:
Data points were divided into two classes: “Rest” and “Imagine” according to their relative times in the trials.
Classifier plane and geometric assessment of EEG data
To investigate the influence of BCI classifiers on the cortical adaptation in the t-SNE-embedded space, the classifier plane and classifier normal vector were linearly projected into the embedded space (see Fig. 3C). A 3D classifier normal vector
Then, the equation of the classifier plane is given as follows:
Because
Geometry-based analysis in the embedded space
The geometry-based analysis was conducted in the embedded space, as geometric relationships of the points reflected the similarities in the original space. The transition process from one brain condition to another (i.e., absence to presence of attempted movement) was assessed by the spatial arrangement and separability of points from the “Rest” and “Imagine” periods in the t-SNE dimension (Fig. 1). Emergence of the two temporal phenomena were defined as follows:
Separation: The separability of the two conditions (Rest and Imagine) increases with respect to a fixed axis. Separation is interpreted as the enhancement of specific cortical activity patterns.
Rotation: The relationship of positions in the two conditions changes direction. Deforming is interpreted as an alteration of a cortical activity pattern that is adopted as the rotational changes towards perpendicular to the classifier plane indicates the reconfiguration of activity patterns contributing to BCI performance improvement.
To quantify the two distinct adaptation process, the following metrics were defined. Scaling and deforming between the
ith and
jth blocks were, respectively, quantified by the difference of
If adaptation progresses toward the targeted neural activity patterns required to control BCIs, the
Cortical source estimation
To localize the source of neural signaling during BCI operation, EEG signals were subjected to sLORETA analysis for cortical source estimation (Pascual-Marqui, 2002). Because the motivation for conducting the source analysis was to test whether the targeted region of the classifier was successfully activated during the late period of BCI training, averaged data from early and late periods were subjected to a nonparametric permutation test (Nichols and Holmes, 2002).
Results
Participants learnt BCI operation based on the mental actions
Twenty-one participants operated BCIs with one of three randomly allocated classifiers that provided scores contingent on BCI. Since culminated sum of scores in a block represents the overall performance of BCI operation, we tested whether the performance improvement was systematically observed in participants of each BCI (Fig. 4A). While BCI performance scores from the model-based and adaptive classifier generally increased over blocks, those for the de novo classifier did not. Regression coefficients of linear regression analysis were computed based on acquired scores from each participant, using the block numbers as the explanatory variable and the acquired scores as response variables. Statistical tests to test for computed regression coefficients revealed significant differences from zero for BCIs based on the model-based and adaptive classifiers (model-based: p = 0.0078, d = 1.86, adaptive: p = 0.023, d = 0.97, de novo: p = 0.055, d = 0.74, Wilcoxon rank-sum test, FDR corrected). The comparison of acquired scores at early and late period indicate significant difference across groups (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, all p < 0.05; Fig. 4B), indicating even de novo BCI elicits adaptation of participants through training. Note that direct comparison of the coefficients among classifiers is not possible because scores from each classifier were computed based on different EEG features (Figs. 4C, 5). As shown in Figure 4C, the time-frequency representations of scalp EEG signals derived from channels of interest for the fixed classifiers (i.e., C3 channel for model-based and Cz for de novo), qualitatively exhibited the changes in the SMR-ERD magnitude during motor imagery (from 0 to 4 s) from early to late period of BCI operation training. For the fixed classifiers using a predefined feature for the motor attempt detection, the spatial representation was visualized by the cortical source estimation (Fig. 5). The corresponding features exhibited sensorimotor activity corresponding to the feature on interest (i.e., model-based classifier: activity around contralateral SM1, de novo classifier: activity around temporo-parietal region).
Geometric quantification of cortical adaptation process revealed distinct adaptation processes to classifier’s separating plane
BCI training requires users to control the voluntary control of targeted activity which classifiers use for motor attempt detection. However, not only the targeted features, those derived from regions interconnected with the target would also reorganize through learning (Wander et al., 2013; Corsi et al., 2020). To examine differences in cortical adaptation processes, we investigated changes in whole-head EEG signals for the early and late period (first and last four blocks of BCI operation, respectively). An example of data from the model-based classifier BCI is shown in Figure 3A. As the participant performed the BCI operation, data during attempted movement (blue points) moved across the classifier plane, where the sign of relative SMR power flips (Fig. 3B). In this case, the defined metrics
Figure 6 indicates changes in the norm of
The identical evaluation was conducted for the de novo classifiers. Figure 7B depicts changes in
As the classifier planes changed from one block to the next for the adaptive classifiers trained with the data from the previous blocks, each metric was calculated against the classifier plane determined with the dataset from the previous block. No significant differences in
In summary, short-term BCI operation training elicited different cortical adaptation processes depending on the BCI types; the model-based and adaptive classifier elicited group-level systematic learning while de novo did not. Meanwhile the two fixed classifiers induced adaptation of neural activity patterns to improve BCI operation performance by reorganizing the whole-brain cortical activity patterns evaluated in the t-SNE space. Further, the performance improvement elicited by the adaptive classifier was mainly driven by the classifier-side adaptation rather than the cortical adaptation as suggested by the no evidence of changes in any of metrics of neural activity patterns.
Discussion
In the present study, participants performed BCI operations with one of three classifiers: model-based, adaptive, or de novo. Because BCI paradigm allows experimenters to set an arbitrary relationship between the BCI model and users (Sadtler et al., 2014), changes in acquired scores are fully attributed to changes in the targeted feature. Although learning curve of acquired scores indicated model-based and adaptive classifiers exhibited significant improvement for BCI control, the adaptation processes were likely distinct. Each classifier elicited a different cortical adaptation process consistent with their characteristics; for the model-based classifier the t-SNE analyses in embedded space revealed decrease in |
Users of model-based BCI demonstrated overall improvement of acquired scores and the increase in
Another fixed classifier, namely, de novo classifier did not elicit the systematic changes in the
Rotational effect was quantified by another metric for geometric evaluation,
One potential limitation of the t-SNE analysis employed in the present study is that the variance of features explained by the classifier vector becomes relatively low because of the nature of dimensionality reduction algorithms. Since the high-dimensional brain activity patterns (645 dimensions) were compressed into 3D spaces, the explained variance of features by classifier normal vectors became overall 20%. Nevertheless, because we observed consistent statistical significance of linear regression models across participants and the preserved variance was sufficient to detect the reorganization process along with the features targeted by BCIs in keeping with the univariate analyses on acquired scores during BCI operation, the estimated classifier normal vectors were statistically reliable representation of features targeted by BCIs. Given that t-SNE algorithm preserves relative distances of each data point in the original space, we believe the t-SNE analysis would be beneficial as a complementary analysis for univariate analyses to evaluate the adaptation process at the whole-brain level.
The present study demonstrated neuroplastic changes in the whole-brain macroscopic activity patterns induced by brain-computer interfacing in the first 2 h. Although the primary focus of the present study was to detect the differential interaction of human brain and classifiers at the early period, the difference elicited by long-term use is not mentioned in the study. While previous BCI studies have demonstrated the long-term co-adaptation is one successful strategy for efficient training (Perdikis et al., 2018; Silversmith et al., 2021), however, the limited amount of training period in the study did not elicit the adaptation of neural activity patterns by the adaptive classifier use. Such differences in the adaptation process depending on time scale are warranted in the further investigation.
Although the flexibility of the human brain enabled adaptation to model-based classifiers and partly to the de novo, the adaptive classifier did not elicit adaptation of neural activity patterns, manifested by the absence of any changes in geometric assessment at least the early stage of BCI operation training. It would be because the adaptive BCI enhanced its performance by classifier-side adaptation, that maximized the separability of two brain states for the current data by classifier reconfiguration. In contrast to the previous studies demonstrating co-adaptation of brain and classifiers using the trial-by-trial classifier adaptation (Wolpaw and McFarland, 2004; Orsborn et al., 2014), the block-by-block calibration procedure waived the previously optimized parameters and reconstructed an entirely new model, the classifier-side adaptation could have been dominant. As the user did not receive neurofeedback based on the constant rule (i.e., parameters of the classifier) putatively because of the abrupt changes in the classifier parameters as well as the CSP features, it could have achieved the high separability of two brain states without engaging cortical adaptation process yet interfered the user’s attempt to adapt to the classifier. In summary, fixation of the classifier plane is an essential element for inducing neural plasticity via a brain-computer interaction based on macroscopic neural populational activities, and adaptation to a BCI based on unnaturalistic features without the instruction of suitable strategies for control is partly possible in the initial stage of BCI operation training. This demonstration may in part explain human adaptability to external environment that continuously changes over time, underlying the flexibility of our motor performance.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements: We thank Hiromichi Suetani and Keiichi Kitajo for their valuable comments regarding t-SNE-based analysis and Yumiko Kakubari, Shoko Tonomoto, and Aya Kamiya for their technical supports.
Footnotes
J.U. is a founder and representative director of the university startup company, LIFESCAPES Inc., involved in the research, development, and sales of rehabilitation devices including brain-computer interfaces, he receives a salary from LIFESCAPES Inc., and holds shares in LIFESCAPES Inc. This company does not have any relationships with the device or setup used in the current study. All other authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by the Keio Institute of Pure and Applied Sciences (KiPAS) Research Program, the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science KAKENHI Grant Number 20H05923 (to J.U.), and the Japan Science and Technology Agency, CREST Grant Number JPMJCR17A3 (to J.U.), including the AIP Challenge Program, Japan.
- Received April 4, 2022.
- Revision received October 27, 2022.
- Accepted November 2, 2022.
- Copyright © 2022 Iwama et al.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.