Abstract
Children born very preterm (<33 weeks of gestation) are at a higher risk of developing socio-emotional difficulties compared with those born at term. In this longitudinal study, we tested the hypothesis that diffusion characteristics of white matter (WM) tracts implicated in socio-emotional processing assessed in the neonatal period are associated with socio-emotional development in 151 very preterm children previously enrolled into the Evaluation of Preterm Imaging study (EudraCT 2009-011602-42). All children underwent diffusion tensor imaging at term-equivalent age and fractional anisotropy (FA) was quantified in the uncinate fasciculus (UF), inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), and superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF). Children’s socio-emotional development was evaluated at preschool age (median = 4.63 years). Exploratory factor analysis conducted on the outcome variables revealed a three-factor structure, with latent constructs summarized as: “emotion moderation,” “social function,” and “empathy.” Results of linear regression analyses, adjusting for full-scale IQ and clinical and socio-demographic variables, showed an association between lower FA in the right UF and higher “emotion moderation” scores (β = −0.280; p < 0.001), which was mainly driven by negative affectivity scores (β = −0.281; p = 0.001). Results further showed an association between higher full-scale IQ and better social functioning (β = −0.334, p < 0.001). Girls had higher empathy scores than boys (β = −0.341, p = 0.006). These findings suggest that early alterations of diffusion characteristics of the UF could represent a biological substrate underlying the link between very preterm birth and emotional dysregulation in childhood and beyond.
- diffusion MRI
- preterm children
- socio-emotional development
- tractography
Significance Statement
Children born very preterm are at a higher risk of developing socio-emotional difficulties compared with those born at term. Our study showed that early alterations of diffusion characteristics of the uncinate fasciculus (UF) in very preterm infants assessed at term-equivalent age were associated with emotional dysregulation in childhood. The identification of early biological substrates linked to emotional development could create opportunities for the prevention and targeting of emerging emotional problems to enhance children’s mental health.
Introduction
Children who were born very preterm (<33 weeks of gestation) are at heightened risk of experiencing socio-emotional difficulties, which include diminished social competence and reduced ability to self-regulate their emotions and behaviors (Spittle et al., 2009; Jones et al., 2013; Witt et al., 2014; Montagna and Nosarti, 2016). Socio-emotional difficulties in childhood have been associated with the later emergence of psychiatric symptoms (Woodward et al., 2017; Thomson et al., 2019).
Our current understanding of the etiology of socio-emotional difficulties associated with very preterm birth is limited. Possible underlying causes include altered neurodevelopment, implicating brain structural and functional connectivity (Counsell et al., 2014; Duerden et al., 2015; Keunen et al., 2017; Dimitrova et al., 2020). However, studies directly investigating the association between brain alterations and children’s socio-emotional outcomes following very preterm birth are scarce (Fischi-Gómez et al., 2015; Mossad et al., 2017; Urbain et al., 2019). Only a few investigations to date have used a longitudinal design to identify neural features present in the neonatal period that are associated with later socio-emotional problems (Rogers et al., 2012), and studies have predominantly focused on resting state functional connectivity. These studies have suggested associations between alterations in neonatal functional amygdala connectivity and internalizing symptoms, and between alterations in ventral attention-default mode network connectivity and behavioral inhibition in preterm born toddlers (Rogers et al., 2017; Sylvester et al., 2018). Given that functional connectivity is constrained by the anatomic structure of the human cerebral cortex (Honey et al., 2009), investigating relationships between neonatal white matter (WM) diffusion characteristics and later socio-emotional outcomes may further add to our understanding of how the brain’s emerging architecture contributes to shaping very preterm children’s development.
Socio-emotional processing is underpinned by integrated activity across an extended socio-emotional network (Catani et al., 2013). Therefore, structural connectivity alterations within this system may represent a neural substrate of social-emotional impairments. Emotional disorders have been characterized by WM diffusion characteristic alterations in several tracts, including the uncinate fasciculus (UF), which connects the temporo-amygdala-orbitofrontal network, the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), which connects the dorsolateral and inferolateral frontal cortex with the occipital and posterior temporal cortex, the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), which links occipital and temporal lobes, and the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), which connects parietal to frontal cortical regions (Jenkins et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2016).
These different tracts have been studied in relation to various behavioral outcomes, such as memory and cognition (Riley et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2020; Koshiyama et al., 2020), but also to specific aspects of socio-emotional processing: the UF with emotion regulation (Von Der Heide et al., 2013; Eden et al., 2015), the IFOF and ILF with the ability to decode human facial emotions (Philippi et al., 2009; Unger et al., 2016), and the SLF with emotional empathy (Parkinson and Wheatley, 2014).
Differences in the diffusion characteristics of these tracts between very preterm and term-born individuals have also been investigated, with previous studies showing inconsistent results, such as reduced fractional anisotropy (FA) values in the UF (Mullen et al., 2011; Travis et al., 2015; Vollmer et al., 2017; Young et al., 2018), both increased (Dodson et al., 2017) and decreased FA values in the IFOF (Salvan et al., 2014; Vollmer et al., 2017; Young et al., 2018), decreased FA values in the ILF (Travis et al., 2015; Vollmer et al., 2017; Young et al., 2018) and SLF (Young et al., 2018), and no group differences in the SLF (Vollmer et al., 2017). Further, very preterm infants and children also display increased diffusivities when compared with term-born controls, including mean diffusivity (MD), radial diffusivity (RD), and axial diffusivity (AD; Young et al., 2018; Lautarescu et al., 2020; Brenner et al., 2021).
The current longitudinal investigation aimed to test the hypothesis that diffusion characteristics of WM tracts implicated in socio-emotional processing assessed at term equivalent age are associated with socio-emotional development during the preschool years in children who were born very preterm.
Materials and Methods
Participants
A total of 511 participants were enrolled into the Evaluation of Preterm Imaging study (ePrime, (EudraCT 2009-011602-42); Edwards et al., 2018). They were recruited at birth in 2010–2013 from hospitals within the North and Southwest London Perinatal Network. Inclusion criteria were birth before 33 weeks of gestational age (GA) and maternal age over 16 years. Exclusion criteria included the presence of major congenital malformation, prior magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), metallic implants, parents unable to speak English, or being subject to child protection proceedings. Infants underwent MRI at term-equivalent age, defined as 38–44 weeks of GA (mean = 42.24; SD = 1.41). At four to seven years of age, children were invited to the Centre for the Developing Brain, St Thomas’ Hospital, London, for a neurodevelopmental assessment. Invitations for follow-up were sent in chronological order of birth to all children who were past their fourth birthday. The study closed on September 1, 2019. Written informed consent was obtained from participants’ carer(s) following procedures approved by the Stanmore Research Ethics Committee (14/LO/0677). The study was conducted in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).
Figure 1 provides detailed recruitment information. To summarize, 55 children invited for follow-up study were not assessed at four to seven years for the reasons listed in Figure 1. A further one hundred children were excluded from the final analyses, because of incomplete assessment data (n = 46), motion artefacts on MRI (n = 38) or significant focal perinatal brain injury (n = 16), defined as periventricular leukomalacia, hemorrhagic parenchymal infarction and other ischemic or hemorrhagic lesions (Barnett et al., 2018), but not including punctate lesions or diffuse excessive high signal in WM on T2-weighted images. The final sample consisted of 151 very preterm born participants who had T1-weighted and T2-weighted MRI and diffusion MRI (dMRI) at 38–44 weeks of term-equivalent and subsequently participated in the follow-up assessment at four to seven years.
Procedure
Perinatal clinical and socio-demographic data
Perinatal clinical and socio-demographic data were collected, with permission, from the Standardized Electronic Neonatal Database. Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) score was computed from the postcode of the parent at the time of infant birth (Department for Communities and Local Government, 2011; https://tools.npeu.ox.ac.uk/imd/) and provided a proxy for family socio-economic status. The IMD measures social risk by comparing each neighborhood to all others in the country and is based on seven domains of deprivation (with varying weighting, as follows): income (22.5%), employment (22.5%), education skills and training (13.5%), health and disability (13.5%), barriers to housing and services (9.3%), living environment (9.3%), and crime (9.3%). Maternal education was defined as age on leaving full-time education, split into two groups (1) at or before 19 years; (2) after 19 years (Kleine et al., 2020).
MRI acquisition and analysis
A pediatrician experienced in MRI procedures supervised all MR imaging. Pulse oximetry, temperature, and electrocardiography data were monitored throughout the session. Silicone-based putty (President Putty, Coltene Whaledent), as well as neonatal earmuffs (MiniMuffs, Natus Medical Inc.), were used for ear protection. Oral chloral hydrate (25–50 mg kg−1) was administered to infants whose parents chose sedation for the procedure.
MR imaging was performed on a 3-Tesla system (Philips Medical Systems) sited on the neonatal intensive care unit using an eight-channel phased array head coil. High-resolution anatomic images were acquired with pulse sequence parameters: T2-weighted fast-spin echo imaging: TR = 8670 ms, TE = 160 ms, flip angle 90°, slice thickness 2 mm with 1-mm overlapping slices, in-plane resolution 0.86 × 0.86 mm. dMRI data were acquired in the transverse plane in 32 non-collinear directions with the following parameters: TR = 8000 ms, TE = 49 ms, voxel size: 2 mm isotropic, b value: 750 s/mm2, sense factor of 2, 1 non-diffusion-weighted image, b = 0.
Diffusion-weighted images were visually inspected in three orthogonal planes for the presence of motion artifact and corrupt diffusion-weighted volumes were excluded before tensor fitting. All participants included in analyses had five or fewer excluded volumes. Non-brain tissue was removed using BET (version 2.1; http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/BET; Smith, 2002; Jenkinson et al., 2012), images were corrected for eddy current artefacts using eddy_correct (Andersson and Sotiropoulos, 2016), and tensor model was fitted using dtifit from FSL (FMRIB; http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk).
Tract-specific analysis (TSA; Yushkevich et al., 2008) was used to derive dMRI measures for selected WM tracts, as described in detail in Pecheva et al. (2017). TSA is a WM analysis method that creates skeleton models of individual WM tracts onto which diffusion data can be projected for statistical analysis. All subjects were registered to a study-specific template using a tensor-based algorithm (Zhang et al., 2006). Following registration, tracts of interest were delineated from the template using deterministic tractography based on the FACT approach (Mori et al., 1999). Whole-brain tractography was seeded from a WM mask, defined by thresholding the template FA map at 0.1, and regions of interest were drawn manually according to the protocol described previously (Wakana et al., 2007), by a single rater (D.P.; Table 1). The following tracking parameters were used: maximum angle threshold of 45°, step size of 0.5 mm and minimum FA threshold of 0.1. From the tractography, a surface skeleton representation with clearly-defined tract boundaries was derived for each WM pathway. TSA samples data to be projected onto each point of the skeleton by searching for the maximum FA value along the unit normal from that point to the tract boundary. This is done for each subject. The data projection step serves two main purposes. First, it is a dimensionality reduction step which increases sensitivity, similar to smoothing (Yushkevich et al., 2008). Furthermore, as tract FA values tend to be higher in the center of a tract, projecting the maximum FA value accounts for residual misalignments and improves inter-subject correspondence by forcing a comparison between tract centers across subjects (Smith et al., 2006).
From the tractography results, a medial surface was determined for the UF, IFOF, ILF, and SLF. The medial surface simultaneously defined the tract skeleton and boundary (Yushkevich and Zhang, 2013). Diffusion data from every subject was then projected onto the skeleton. TSA sampled data to be projected onto each point of the skeleton by searching along the unit normal from that point to the tract boundary (Pecheva et al., 2017). FA values were calculated for each tract. Examples of surface representation of tracts are shown in Figure 2.
Neurodevelopmental outcomes
The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI-IV; Wechsler, 2012) was used to estimate children’s full-scale IQ. Parents completed the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, 1997), a behavioral screening measure for general childhood psychopathology, comprising 25 items categorized into five subscales: emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems and prosocial behavior.; the Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire–Very Short Form (CBQ; Putnam and Rothbart, 2006) which assesses the child’s temperament using 36 items summarized into three broad scales (negative affectivity, effortful control and surgency); the Empathy Questionnaire (EmQue; Rieffe et al., 2010), a 20 item questionnaire which measures empathy-related behaviors in young children, summarized into three scales: emotion contagion, attention to others’ emotions and prosocial responses to others’ emotions; and the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS-2; Constantino and Gruber, 2012), an assessment of social impairments associated with autism-spectrum behaviors, which provides subscale scores for social awareness, social cognition, social communication, social motivation, restricted interests and repetitive behaviors.
To measure facial emotion recognition abilities, a new task was created based on (Gao and Maurer, 2009), which used static stimuli from the validated Dartmouth database of children’s faces (Dalrymple et al., 2013). Four boys and four girls were chosen from the database and 6 emotions (happy, surprise, fear, anger, disgust and sadness) plus neutral faces were used. The task consisted of two testing blocks, whereby the first of these included pictures showing happy, surprised and fearful expressions, and the second contained pictures showing sad, disgusted and angry expressions, with both blocks including neutral faces. Stimuli were presented singly, and participants were asked to identify which emotion each image was representing. Accuracy was measured based on the correct identification of emotions. Male and female models were allocated evenly between the emotion and neutral expressions. For every emotion, two levels of intensity were created by morphing a neutral face with the emotional face of the same model, to create, for example, 50% and 100% happy faces. The use of different intensities has been shown to promote the detection of subtle differences in abilities to recognize emotions (Horning et al., 2012; Kessels et al., 2014). Fantamorph software was used to create these morphed images (http://www.fantamorph.com/index.html), by manually positioning points on the anatomic landmarks in the photograph of each face. Distortions caused by the morphing process in the eye and mouth regions were edited using Photoshop. In total, there were 56 stimuli [(2 intensity levels × 6 emotions × 4 models) + (2 neutral expressions × 4 models)]. The task created here has no working memory load, and therefore only participant’s accuracy is measured. The total number of correct responses the child made was added to produce an emotion recognition score.
Lower accuracy scores on the emotion recognition task correlated with higher SRS-2 T-scores, indicating increased social difficulties [social information processing (r = −0.261; p < 0.001), social communication (r = −0.201; p = 0.003), social motivation (r = −0.201; p = 0.003), restricted interests and repetitive behavior (r = −0.228; p = 0.001), social communication index (SCI; r = −0.234; p = 0.001), and total score (r = −0.237; p < 0.001)]. Crucially, the task was most strongly associated with social information processing, suggesting it successfully measured the key construct it was designed to, in pediatric samples.
Statistical analyses
Twenty-eight perinatal clinical variables obtained from all ePrime participants (n = 511) were summarized using principal component analysis (PCA) applying Promax rotation, using SPSS 26. Maternal variables were: preeclampsia and pregnancy induced hypertension, antenatal hypertension, placental abruption or antenatal hemorrhage, premature rupture of membranes, urinary tract infection, gestational diabetes, oligohydramnios, polyhydramnios, drug abuse, in vitro fertilization, bacterial infection (all y/n), and mode of delivery (vaginal/elective/emergency). Infant variables were: sex, GA (weeks and days), birth weight (grams), multiple pregnancy (singleton/multiple), antenatal steroid administration (no/partial/full course), twin-to-twin transfusion, chorioamnionitis, intrauterine growth restriction, surfactant administration, treatment for patent ductus arteriosus, surgical treatment for necrotizing enterocolitis, formula feeding, feeding on maternal expressed breast milk (all y/n), days on mechanical ventilation, days on continuous positive airway pressure (cPAP), and days on parenteral nutrition (TPN). All variables were coded so that higher values reflected greater clinical risk. Communalities were checked, and as all were above 0.2, no items were removed (Costello and Osborne, 2005).
R using R studio was used to perform all following analyses, using a non-random experimental design. Maximum-likelihood factor analysis with Varimax rotation was performed using the stats v3.6.2 package. Factor analysis included the following outcome variables: four SDQ subscales (emotional symptoms, conduct problems, peer relationship problems and prosocial behavior), three CBQ subscales (negative affectivity, surgency and effortful control), three EmQue subscales (emotion contagion, attention to others’ feelings and prosocial actions); the SRS-2 SCI and accuracy on the emotion recognition task. All neurodevelopmental subscale scores were standardized. Factors were extracted based on the criterion of having eigenvalues >1 and examination of scree plots. Factor scores were then used in subsequent analyses.
In order to find the best predictors of children’s socio-emotional outcomes, best-fit linear models were selected using an automated model selection process, using the glmulti package (Calcagno and de Mazancourt, 2010). Before model selection, multicollinearity between the chosen tracts was assessed by calculating a variance inflation factor (VIF) for each tract, which consists of comparing the overall model variance to the variance of a model that includes only that single independent tract. Two variables with VIF ≥ 10 were excluded from subsequent analyses (Hair, 2009), i.e., FA values of the left and right IFOF. For each socio-emotional outcome (emotion moderation, social function, and empathy), model comparison was performed using Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1974) and 8450 models were compared. Variables included in each model were: FA values of the left UF, ILF and SLF; right UF, ILF and SLF; postmenstrual age at scan (PMA), corrected age at follow-up assessment, IQ, IMD, maternal education, sex, and neonatal sickness index. Linear regression was then performed to study the association between predictor variables included in the best-fit model and socio-emotional factor scores.
Results
Participants’ characteristics are summarized in Table 2. Results of PCA performed to summarize perinatal clinical variables showed one factor that explained 72% of their variance. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was 0.73, Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (χ2 = 3597.79, p < 0.01), and all factor loadings were above 0.2. Factor weights were between 0.767 and 0.898 for GA, days on TPN, days on cPAP, days on mechanical ventilation and surfactant administration. This factor was labeled as “neonatal sickness index.” Internal reliability assessed using Cronbach’s α was good (α = 0.86).
Children who were included in the final analyses had lower neonatal sickness index scores (t = 2.721, p = 0.007) and higher IQ scores (t = −3.449, p = 0.001) compared with those who were not included. Results were similar after exclusion of non-participating children with significant focal perinatal brain injury on neonatal MRI (neonatal sickness index score: t = 2.903, p = 0.004; IQ: t = −2.752, p = 0.006).
Socio-emotional outcomes
Descriptive statistics for children’s socio-emotional outcome measures are shown in Table 3.
Exploratory factor analysis conducted on the 12 socio-emotional outcomes showed a three-factor structure. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was 0.76, Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (χ2 = 469.977, p < 0.001), and all factor loadings were above 0.4. The three latent constructs accounted for 42% of the variance in socio-emotional outcomes and are graphically displayed in Figure 3. These are summarized as: factor 1 or “emotion moderation,” characterized by positive loadings (between 0.48 and 0.93) for CBQ negative affectivity and CBQ effortful control scores. Factor 2 or “social function,” which loads onto higher SDQ emotional symptoms, SDQ conduct problems, SDQ peer relationship problems scores and SRS-2 SCI; as well as lower scores on SDQ prosocial behavior, EmQue prosocial actions and CBQ surgency (between –0.67 and 0.74), indicating more emotional and behavior problems, heightened antisocial behavior and more socializing difficulties. Factor 3 or “empathy” is defined by positive loadings for EmQue emotion contagion and EmQue attention to others’ scores (both 0.56), indicating a higher degree of empathic displays. Emotion recognition scores did not substantially load onto any of the factors.
Diffusion properties of WM tracts
Mean FA values of the UF, IFOF, ILF and SLF, are shown in Table 4.
Best-fit predictors of socio-emotional outcomes
Best-fit predictors of emotion moderation were right UF FA values and full-scale IQ (AIC value = 387.387). Lower FA values in the right UF were associated with higher emotion moderation scores (β = −0.280, p < 0.001; Fig. 4). In order to aid interpretation, post hoc analyses decomposed the emotion moderation factor into the two variables that loaded onto it (CBQ negative affectivity and CBQ effortful control scores). Results showed that the association between right UF FA values and emotion moderation was mainly driven by their relationship to negative affectivity scores (β = −0.281; p = 0.001). UF FA values were not significantly associated with effortful control scores (β = −0.126; p = 0.150).
Best-fit predictors of social function were sex, corrected age at assessment and full-scale IQ (AIC value = 407.210). Higher full-scale IQ was associated with better social functioning (i.e., lower social function factor scores; β = −0.334, p < 0.001). Best-fit predictor of empathy was sex and IMD (AIC value = 346.983). Girls had higher empathy scores than boys (β = −0.319, p = 0.006). FA values of the WM tracts implicated in emotion processing were not associated with social function and empathy.
Results of regression analyses for the three outcome models (emotion moderation, social function, and empathy) including best-fit predictors remained significant after applying Bonferroni correction, correcting for the six WM tracts (adjusted p value for significance = 0.008; Table 5).
Discussion
This study found that WM diffusion characteristics in the UF assessed at term equivalent age in very preterm infants were associated with childhood emotion moderation scores, which summarize a latent factor reflecting both increased negative affectivity and enhanced effortful control. Negative affectivity refers to a reactive temperamental trait that is characterized by an overall negative outlook of oneself and the surrounding world (e.g., frustration, anger, sadness). Effortful control is conceptualized as an umbrella term referring to a child’s capacity to focus and shift attention, intentionally inhibit a response, and respond to low-intensity stimulation and reward (Rothbart et al., 2003). The two traits, negative affectivity and effortful control, could be compared with constructs used to describe adult personality, neuroticism and constraint/conscientiousness, respectively (Digman, 1990). Summary scores for negative affectivity and effortful control tend to be negatively correlated in community samples (Putnam et al., 2006), hence the factor described here as emotion moderation, which combines high negative affectivity and enhanced effortful control, may look unintuitive at first. This factor could be interpreted as reflecting an adaptive strategy, whereby children use regulatory skills to moderate the impact of reactive systems, i.e., negative emotionality (Rothbart and Bates, 1998; Nigg, 2006). In line with this hypothesis, Eisenberg and colleagues showed that negative emotionality and low regulation were maladaptive, (Eisenberg et al., 2001) while Belsky et al. (2001) found that high levels of orienting/effortful control moderated the impact of children’s negative affectivity on behavioral outcomes.
Lower FA values in the right UF assessed at term were associated with higher emotion moderation scores in childhood. When this factor was decomposed into the two variables that loaded onto it (negative affectivity and effortful control) we found that this association was driven by the relationship between right UF FA values and negative affectivity. Anatomically, the UF is sited withing the limbic system, and connects the “temporo-amygdala-orbitofrontal network” (Catani et al., 2013), which has been suggested to play a pivotal role in “affective tagging” (Von Der Heide et al., 2013), i.e., the assignment of emotional tone, such as positive and negative feelings, to the representations stored in the anterior temporal lobe (Olson et al., 2013). Diffusion characteristics of the UF have been previously implicated in a variety of psychiatric disorders (Von Der Heide et al., 2013) including anxiety disorder (Phan et al., 2009) and major depression (Cullen et al., 2010) and have also been associated with early adversity and future psychological vulnerability to stress (Hanson et al., 2015). A study in major depression investigating diffusion characteristics and resting state functional connectivity of the UF showed structural alterations together with functional orbitofrontal cortex-amygdala inhibition, suggesting this dysconnectivity pattern was mediated by “top‐down” influences from the frontal cortex to the amygdala (Zheng et al., 2018). We speculate that early diffusion characteristics of the UF may be associated with difficulties in top-down regulation, leading to children’s inability to down-regulate amygdalar activity, resulting in the tendency to attribute negative feelings to certain contexts and situations, and in the overexpression of negative affect.
Furthermore, although our results did not show a direct association between UF FA values and effortful control, and the UF has not been selectively implicated in effortful/cognitive control in the published literature (Noble et al., 2013), it may nevertheless be relevant to effortful control in the context of emotions, with a previous study showing an association between UF integrity and emotional control in children with traumatic brain injury (Johnson et al., 2011).
Results indicated a laterality effect, with only the right UF FA values being associated with emotion moderation scores. These findings are in line with previous research, which demonstrated lateralization of emotional processes. According to the valence hypothesis (Hellige, 2001), the processing of negative emotions preferentially engages the right side of the brain, whereas positive emotions are preferentially processed by the left side. This hypothesis is supported by functional MRI results showing participants are better at discriminating sad faces when visual stimuli are displayed in the left visual field (i.e., right hemisphere) and better at discriminating happy faces when displayed in the right visual field (i.e., left hemisphere; Adolphs et al., 2001; Rodway et al., 2003). The valence hypothesis is also supported by research showing that direct stimulation of the right amygdala induced unpleasant emotions, whereas stimulation of the left amygdala induced both pleasant and unpleasant emotions (Lanteaume et al., 2007). Further, differences between the two hemispheres in terms of volume and number of streamlines for UF subcomponents have been previously observed (Park et al., 2004; Hau et al., 2017), suggesting left-lateralized dorsolateral and right-lateralized ventromedial UF subcomponents.
Diffusion characteristics of any of the neonatal WM tracts studied here were not associated with social function and empathy factor scores. Social function was characterized by higher factor loadings for subscales evaluating social problems, and lower factor loadings for prosocial behavior, therefore higher social function scores reflected increased overall social difficulties. Better social function was associated with higher full-scale IQ, supporting previous literature highlighting the importance of cognitive development for successful social adaptation (Watson et al., 1999; Adolphs, 2001). These results could be important for translational research, as they suggest that an enhancement of children’s cognitive abilities could increase their understanding of social interactions (Soto-Icaza et al., 2015), leading to a more successful psychosocial adjustment. Furthermore, both social and emotional skills are thought to be intertwined with cognitive processes, with theoretical models postulating that emotions arise from evaluations of the goal relevance of a stimulus, and that other people are the most goal-relevant stimuli in one’s life (Olsson and Ochsner, 2008). Such framework could explain the inclusion of “SDQ emotional problems” in our social function factor.
Girls had higher empathy scores than boys, indicating a better ability to empathize with the experiences of others. Sex differences in empathy have been widely documented in previous research (Auyeung et al., 2009), and are thought to be reinforced by gender-based expectations of parents, teachers, and caregivers (Stern and Karraker, 1989).
Caveats of the present study include a lack of full-term control participants, which limits the generalizability of our findings only to those children born very preterm. In addition, the very preterm preschoolers who were followed-up in our study came from a relatively high socio-economic background (23.8% belonged to the least deprived IMD quintile) and had a higher mean full-scale IQ and lower “neonatal sickness index” scores compared with non-returners. Therefore, the current sample includes those children with a more favorable outcome and may not be representative of the overall ePrime sample and the wider very preterm population. Taking this into consideration, our results may be generalizable only to relatively high functioning very preterm children.
An added limitation of the current study is that dMRI was acquired using a b value of 750 s/mm2 with 32 non-collinear directions. Further, the diffusion metric FA is not suitable for modeling crossing fibers and is more susceptible to partial volume effects, limiting our ability to delineate the SLF in its entirety.
Another limitation is the omission of the cingulum in our analyses, as this tract has been associated with emotion processing and psychiatric symptomatology (Bubb et al., 2018). Cingulum tractography was not performed because TSA determines the tract skeleton by thinning the tract down to a medial surface, and therefore tube-like structures (such as the cingulum) are ill-suited for this methodology.
Results linking neonatal WM alterations with the tendency to overexpress negative emotions about oneself and the surrounding world in childhood, contributes to the growing body of research attempting to use an infant’s connectivity profile to predict its function in the future. This has been recently demonstrated by a study that used children’s connectivity fingerprints before they could read to predict their functional responses in a brain region hypothesized to be implicated in word recognition (i.e., the visual word form area) after they had learnt to read three years later (Saygin et al., 2016). The ability to predict typical and atypical patterns of emotional development would create opportunities for the early targeting of emerging emotional problems and their downstream consequences, including emotional disorders. This could be achieved through preventative therapies, such as emotion regulation training, that would help children to deal with potential stressors and enhance their mental health (World Health Organization, 2004; Brown et al., 2012).
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by Medical Research Council (United Kingdom) Grants MR/K006355/1, MR/L011530/1, and MR/S026460/1; the Wellcome/EPSRC Centre for Medical Engineering Grant WT 203148/Z/16/Z; the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council Grant BB/J014567/1; and the Action Medical Research and Dangoor Education Grant GN2606. This work uses data acquired during independent research funded by the National Institute for Health Research Programme Grants for Applied Research Programme (RP-PG-0707-10154).
- Received December 14, 2020.
- Revision received May 19, 2021.
- Accepted May 19, 2021.
- Copyright © 2021 Kanel et al.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.