Article Text

Research paper
Grammatical comprehension deficits in non-fluent/agrammatic primary progressive aphasia
  1. Dorothy Charles,
  2. Christopher Olm,
  3. John Powers,
  4. Sharon Ash,
  5. David J Irwin,
  6. Corey T McMillan,
  7. Katya Rascovsky,
  8. Murray Grossman
  1. Department of Neurology and Penn FTD Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
  1. Correspondence to Dr Murray Grossman, Department of Neurology—2 Gibson, Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania, 3400 Spruce St, Philadelphia, PA 19104-4283, USA; mgrossma{at}mail.med.upenn.edu

Abstract

Importance Grammatical comprehension difficulty is an essential supporting feature of the non-fluent/agrammatic variant of primary progressive aphasia (naPPA), but well-controlled clinical measures of grammatical comprehension are unavailable.

Objective To develop a measure of grammatical comprehension and examine this comparatively in PPA variants and behavioural-variant frontotemporal degeneration (bvFTD) and to assess the neuroanatomic basis for these deficits with volumetric grey matter atrophy and whole-brain fractional anisotropy (FA) in white matter tracts.

Design Case–control study.

Setting Academic medical centre.

Participants 39 patients with variants of PPA (naPPA=12, lvPPA=15 and svPPA=12), 27 bvFTD patients without aphasia and 12 healthy controls.

Main outcome measure Grammatical comprehension accuracy.

Results Patients with naPPA had selective difficulty understanding cleft sentence structures, while all PPA variants and patients with bvFTD were impaired with sentences containing a centre-embedded subordinate clause. Patients with bvFTD were also impaired understanding sentences involving short-term memory. Linear regressions related grammatical comprehension difficulty in naPPA to left anterior-superior temporal atrophy and reduced FA in corpus callosum and inferior frontal-occipital fasciculus. Difficulty with centre-embedded sentences in other PPA variants was related to other brain regions.

Conclusions and relevance These findings emphasise a distinct grammatical comprehension deficit in naPPA and associate this with interruption of a frontal-temporal neural network.

  • Aphasia
  • Behavioural Disorder
  • Brain Mapping
  • Dementia

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Introduction

Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) includes non-fluent/agrammatic (naPPA), semantic (svPPA) and logopenic (lvPPA) variants. Each has characteristic language deficits.1 Quantifying these abnormalities for disease-modifying treatment trials provides useful screening for histopathologic abnormalities.2 Effortful speech and limited grammatical expression, necessary features of naPPA, have been well documented in naPPA.3–6 Grammatical comprehension difficulty also is an important supporting feature of naPPA,1 but comparative studies of grammatical comprehension in small series are uncommon7–11 and anatomic assessments are rare.7 ,11

Grammatical comprehension involves long-distance syntactically specified relationships between words in a sentence. Patients with naPPA are impaired answering a simple question about a grammatically complex sentence,7 matching a complex sentence to one of several pictures,8 and arranging printed words into a question consistent with the content of a picture.12 While sensitive, these approaches have not proven specific and have not identified qualitatively distinct impairments in naPPA.7 ,8 ,11 Since grammatical comprehension of sentences may also involve working memory, it is important to dissociate these factors, particularly in patients who have working memory limitations.13 ,14 Non-aphasic patients with behavioural-variant frontotemporal degeneration (bvFTD) have executive deficits that interfere with narrative comprehension,15 but it is unclear whether this compromises grammatical comprehension.

Here, we examined grammatical comprehension difficulty in PPA variants and patients with bvFTD with a new clinical measure, and we related performance to regional grey matter (GM) and white matter (WM) abnormalities. Impaired grammatical comprehension has been related to left inferior frontal and anterior-superior temporal atrophy in two small series.7 ,11 We expected grammatically complex sentences with a cleft structure to be sensitive to grammatical difficulty in naPPA, and that this would be related to abnormalities in a GM–WM network involving inferior frontal and anterior-superior temporal regions.

Methods

Participants

We studied 66 individuals recruited by experienced cognitive neurologists (DJI and MG) from the Department of Neurology at the University of Pennsylvania. This included 39 with variants of PPA (naPPA=12, lvPPA=15 and svPPA=12) and 27 non-aphasics with bvFTD. Two independent reviewers subclassified participants according to published criteria,1 based on medical history, neurologic examination and a detailed mental status evaluation. We excluded individuals with vascular disease, hydrocephalus, primary psychiatric disorder and medical illness that may impair cognition. Participants were age- and education-matched with 12 healthy, elderly controls. Clinical and demographic information is summarised in table 1. Subjects were further characterised by performance on neuropsychological measures of executive functioning, semantic memory and episodic memory. These measures are described in table 1, where performance is summarised.

Table 1

Mean (±SD) demographic and clinical characteristics of healthy seniors and patients*

Behavioural materials

Sentence comprehension

We developed a two-alternative, forced-choice sentence-picture matching task. This is composed of 72 randomly ordered sentences and two colour pictures accompanying each sentence. The content of the sentences and the pictures included familiar, animate objects engaging in familiar activities. Forty-eight experimental sentences were equally divided into three factors with two levels of each factor, as illustrated in table 2. The structure factor included cleft (two propositions) and centre-embedded (three proposition) sentences; the length factor included short (nine words) or long (12 words, lengthened by a prepositional phrase strategically placed between grammatically linked sentence constituents) sentences; and the relativisation factor included subject-relative (agent-initial) and object-relative (patient-initial) sentences. Twenty-four control sentences did not contain a grammatical manipulation but were length-matched to experimental stimuli: half included a single main verb and the remainder were compound sentences concatenated by the conjunction ‘and’; half of each of these was short and half long. We found no statistical differences between these control versions and combined these for analyses reported below.

Table 2

Examples of sentences used in the sentence comprehension task

Participants were shown a page containing two simple, coloured illustrations, one above the other. Both pictures illustrated the same two characters performing an action named in the associated sentence, with the characters switching thematic roles (agent and patient) in the two pictures (figure 1). The actors were semantically reversible and could perform the described action equally to each other. Adjectives in the sentences did not bias responses. Participants heard an oral sentence presented naturalistically and were asked to indicate the corresponding picture. We repeated a sentence once if requested by the participant. The top image was correct on half of each stimulus type. A practice session introduced participants to the materials and procedures, and all participants seemed to understand the task.

Figure 1

Sample task illustration. Note: 1. Example of a two-choice illustration used in the sentence task presented to subjects. Subjects indicated which of the two panels correctly illustrated an orally presented sentence. The sample shown here corresponds to the long, object-relative cleft sentence, ‘It was the unpredictable man with good intentions that the woman poked’.

Behavioural statistical analysis

Control participants performed near ceiling, so non-parametric statistics were used, including Friedman tests to determine interactions of group with structure, length and relativisation factors and Wilcoxon tests to analyse each group's comprehension performance more closely. All analyses were conducted using a two-tailed α of 0.05.

Imaging methods

We related sentence comprehension performance directly to T1 and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) MRI scans available in 46 participants (naPPA, n=6; lvPPA, n=10; svPPA, n=8; and bvFTD, n=22).

GM atrophy

Details of imaging methods for GM density, reported previously,16 are provided in online supplementary appendix 1. Volumetric T1 images were collected at 3 T with 1 mm isotropic voxels. We performed voxel-wise comparisons between patients and controls with two-sample t-tests using a false discovery rate (FDR)-corrected height threshold. Since GM disease varies depending on underlying pathology17 and the number of participants in a group, we minimised these potential sources of bias by selecting statistical thresholds that equated the volume of atrophy across groups (naPPA: q<0.025; lvPPA: q<0.001; svPPA: q<0.0005; bvFTD: q<0.00001). We identified clusters surviving an extent threshold of 50 adjacent voxels. Linear regression related GM density to impaired language performance at a height threshold of p<0.05 (uncorrected), an extent threshold of 10 voxels and a peak voxel level in each cluster of p<0.001 unless otherwise noted.

WM fractional anisotropy

Details of the imaging methods for WM fractional anisotropy (FA), reported previously,16 are provided in online supplementary appendix 1. Briefly, diffusion-weighted images were acquired at 3 T with either a 30-direction or 12-direction acquisition sequence. To minimise potential bias associated with a DTI sequence, we included a nuisance covariate for DTI sequence in all analyses. To identify areas of reduced FA, we performed two-sample t-tests comparing patients and controls at a height threshold of q<0.01 (FDR-corrected) and an extent threshold of 200 adjacent voxels. Linear regression-related reduced FA to impaired language performance at a height threshold of p<0.005 (uncorrected), an extent threshold of 50 voxels and a peak voxel level of p<0.001.

Results

Behavioural results

A Friedman test analysing all groups’ scores for structure, length and relativisation factors demonstrated an interaction effect (χ2(6)=372.2; p<0.0001). Figure 2 illustrates comprehension accuracy for each factor. Interaction effects for each group and factor are presented below.

Figure 2

Mean (+SE) percent correct sentence comprehension performance. Notes: (1) (A) Percentage of correct control, cleft, and centre-embedded sentences by diagnosis. (B) Percentage of correct short and long sentences by diagnosis. (C) Percentage of correct subject- and object-relative sentences by diagnosis. For these graphs, error bars represent the SE of the mean. (2) * denotes a significant difference in mean scores (see text for details). In (A), cleft and centre-embedded differences are not shown. (3) NCs, normal controls; naPPA, non-fluent/agrammatic primary progressive aphasia (PPA); lvPPA, logopenic variant PPA; svPPA, semantic variant PPA; bvFTD, behavioural variant frontotemporal degeneration.

Structure

A Friedman test demonstrated a group–structure interaction (χ2(2)=43.1; p<0.001). Follow-up Wilcoxon tests in naPPA demonstrated worse performance with cleft than control sentences (Z=−1.992; p=0.046). Only naPPA showed this difference, and this was not evident in other groups including lvPPA (Z=−1.063, p=0.28). The deficit with cleft sentences in naPPA was equally evident for short and long versions (p>0.10) and subject- and object-relative versions (p>0.10). This could not be attributed easily to non-specific cognitive difficulty since sentence comprehension did not correlate with MMSE in naPPA, although MMSE was correlated with comprehension in other patient groups (all Spearman correlations p<0.025). All patient groups were worse with centre-embedded than control sentences (all contrasts p<0.05), and all groups except svPPA demonstrated lower scores on centre-embedded than cleft sentences (Z≥−2.533; p≤0.011). All groups showed worse performance for object-relative than subject-relative versions of centre-embedded sentences (all contrasts p<0.01), and all groups were equally impaired for long and short versions of centre-embedded sentences.

Length

While a Friedman test did not show an interaction between group and length (χ2(1)=1.67; p=0.197), we examined this factor more closely because of hypotheses that short-term memory may affect performance. A Wilcoxon test demonstrated difficulty with long compared to short sentences only in bvFTD (Z=−2.226; p=0.026). In bvFTD, sentence comprehension correlated with measures of executive functioning, including FAS fluency, category naming fluency and reverse digit span (all Spearman correlations p<0.001). A length effect was not evident in other groups.

Relativisation

Although a Friedman test demonstrated a group×relativisation interaction effect (χ2(1)=45.6; p<0.001), this was due to controls’ near-ceiling performance on object-relative sentences. Mann–Whitney tests showed worse performance on object relatives in all patient groups compared with controls (all contrasts p<0.005), and all groups had worse performance for object relatives than subject-relatives (all contrasts p<0.05).

Imaging results

Significantly reduced GM density and regressions relating GM density to comprehension are illustrated in figure 3. Figure 3 also shows significantly reduced FA in WM tracts. Peak coordinates for GM atrophy and reduced FA in WM are summarised in online supplementary appendix 2. Regression analyses relating comprehension performance to GM atrophy and reduced FA in WM are summarised in table 3.

Table 3

Regressions of language performance with grey matter atrophy and reduced white matter fractional anisotropy

Figure 3

Grey matter atrophy and regressions relating behaviour to atrophy, and reduced white matter fractional anisotropy. Note: (1) Images on the left illustrate whole brain renderings of grey matter atrophy, and coloured areas show the specific anatomic distribution of significant atrophy compared to matched controls. Anatomic locations of the corresponding clusters are provided in online supplementary appendix 2. Areas in colours other than green illustrate loci of significant regressions relating performance to grey matter atrophy. Images on the right illustrate reduced fractional anisotropy on red-green-blue (RGB) diffusion tensor images (diffusion orientation: red=left-right, green=anterior-posterior, blue=superior-inferior). Ghost areas indicate anatomic regions and orange areas within these ghosts indicate areas of significantly reduced fractional anisotropy compared to matched controls. Anatomic locations of the corresponding clusters are provided in online supplementary appendix 2. (A): non-fluent/agrammatic primary progressive aphasia; (B): logopenic variant primary progressive aphasia; (C): semantic variant primary progressive aphasia; (D): behavioural variant frontotemporal degeneration.

GM atrophy in naPPA was centred in left inferior frontal, insula and anterior-superior temporal regions, extending to right frontal regions. Regression analyses related impaired comprehension for cleft and centre-embedded sentences to GM atrophy in left anterior-superior temporal cortex. Reduced FA was evident in left superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) and inferior frontal-occipital fasciculus (IFO) as well as corpus callosum (CC), fornix and uncinate fasciculus (UNC). Regressions related performance with cleft sentences to reduced FA in left anterior CC and related performance with centre-embedded sentences to left IFO.

In lvPPA, GM atrophy was centred in left posterior-superior temporal and inferior parietal regions, extending to left frontal areas and minimally to the right hemisphere. Regression analyses related impaired centre-embedded comprehension to GM atrophy in left posterior-superior temporal and inferior parietal regions. Reduced FA was seen in left IFO, left inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), fornix and CC. Regressions related impaired centre-embedded sentence comprehension to reduced FA in left IFO, left ILF and CC.

In svPPA, GM atrophy was most prominent in left anterior temporal regions, extending posteriorly into left temporal areas and involving the right anterior temporal lobe. Regression analyses related impaired centre-embedded comprehension to GM atrophy in left mid-lateral temporal regions. Reduced FA in svPPA was seen in left ILF, UNC and CC. Impaired centre-embedded comprehension was related to reduced FA in left ILF.

In bvFTD, GM atrophy was evident throughout frontal and anterior temporal lobes bilaterally. Regression analyses related impaired centre-embedded comprehension to frontal and superior temporal GM atrophy bilaterally. Impaired long sentence comprehension also was related to bilateral frontal and superior temporal GM atrophy. Reduced FA was seen in multiple WM regions bilaterally. Impaired centre-embedded comprehension was related to reduced FA in CC, bilateral SLF and left IFO. Difficulty with long sentences was related to reduced FA in bilateral CC.

Discussion

Grammatical comprehension plays a crucial role in the diagnosis of naPPA.18 This study developed a new method to assess grammatical comprehension deficits in naPPA and help characterise this difficulty in other PPA variants as well as bvFTD. We found a deficit understanding cleft sentences only in naPPA, and this was related to interruption of a frontal-temporal neural network for sentence processing. All FTD subgroups were significantly impaired with centre-embedded sentences involving greater resource demands, and patients with bvFTD were compromised with the short-term memory component of sentences. Linear regression analyses related impairments with these sentence materials to other anatomic regions.

Development of the grammatical task

naPPA, also known as progressive non-fluent aphasia, has been associated with a grammatical comprehension deficit since its earliest description.19 This observation has led to its inclusion as an important supplementary feature of naPPA in a recent consensus report.1 Unfortunately, several confounds have limited the informativeness of this feature in prior assessments. One problem has been the use of highly demanding grammatical materials to demonstrate a deficit in grammatical comprehension. Using centre-embedded subordinate phrase constructions, for example, comparative studies found a broad sentence comprehension deficit across all PPA variants.7 ,8 The present study confirmed this impairment in a broad spectrum of FTD. More importantly, cleft sentences were selectively impaired in naPPA and were not significantly impaired in other patient groups. These sentences were less difficult statistically than centre-embedded sentences. This may reflect that cleft sentences have fewer resource demands because they contain only two propositions, while centre-embedded sentences contain three. While non-specific cognitive difficulty may have contributed to comprehension impairments in lvPPA, svPPA and bvFTD, we did not find a correlation between MMSE and comprehension performance in naPPA.

A second problem that may have confounded prior attempts to identify a measure specific for naPPA is related to task demands. Challenging tasks used to ascertain sentence comprehension difficulty have included forming a wh- question using an anagram task,12 or holding a target sentence in mind while answering a question about the target sentence.7 To minimise task-related demands, several studies have demonstrated selective comprehension impairments in naPPA with online measures.10 ,20 However, it is difficult to develop an online task that can be administered in a clinical setting. Using a balanced approach that minimises task-related demands and considers the practical constraints of task administration by asking patients to relate a sentence to one of two pictures (also see ref. 6), we identified a selective deficit for cleft sentences in naPPA. The findings presented in this paper demonstrate the efficacy of the task, even in patients with mild disease, and we are continuing to administer this test to a larger PPA cohort in order to confirm the findings of this study.

Grammatical comprehension deficits in naPPA

Patients with naPPA have disease centred in left inferior frontal and adjacent anterior-superior temporal regions.11 ,16 ,21 ,22 Grammatical expression deficits have been related to this anatomic distribution of disease.5 However, studies directly relating sentence comprehension to regional GM atrophy in naPPA have been very rare. One study associated impaired sentence comprehension with posterior-inferior frontal and anterior-superior temporal regions of the left hemisphere.7 In mixed groups of neurodegenerative patients, grammatical comprehension was related to left inferior frontal atrophy.23 ,24 The present study found that difficulty understanding cleft sentences in naPPA is related to left anterior-superior temporal GM atrophy. Although we had a small number of imaging datasets, this was essentially the same anatomic distribution related to impairments with centre-embedded sentences, confirming the reliability of cleft sentences for detecting grammatical comprehension deficits in naPPA. Future work will have to confirm this anatomic locus with larger numbers of patients. Previous work has suggested that this anterior-superior temporal region is part of a ventral WM projection stream25 important for processing grammatical information in naPPA.16 Consistent with this view, we observed reduced FA in left IFO in naPPA, as described previously,26 and difficulty with centre-embedded sentences also was related to reduced FA in this tract. We observed reduced FA in left SLF in naPPA as well, as observed previously.16 ,26 ,27 Although regression analyses did not implicate this tract directly in naPPA sentence processing difficulty, reduced FA in the nearby anterior CC and corona radiata was related to comprehension of cleft sentences. Much evidence in the stroke literature implicates callosal dysfunction in performance on grammatical measures in Broca's aphasia,28–30 and other studies have related grammatical comprehension difficulty in naPPA directly to SLF.24 The discrepancy between findings of WM involvement in Broca's aphasia and naPPA may have been due to different groups of patients and different sentence materials.

Sentence comprehension deficits in bvFTD

Patients with bvFTD were significantly impaired with centre-embedded sentences. We also used a fully penetrated design to assess an effect for length. Only patients with bvFTD showed a length effect as well. Despite the absence of an obvious aphasia, language-related deficits have been reported on comprehension measures in bvFTD involving narrative15 and discourse.31 Resource-related limitations were implicated in these deficits, and the present study found a correlation between impaired sentence comprehension and executive resource limitations in bvFTD as well. Additional work is needed to assess more precisely the role of short-term memory deficits during sentence processing in bvFTD.

bvFTD patients’ difficulty with centre-embedded sentences was related to extensive atrophy in several frontal and temporal regions. Since most of the statistically significant regressions were in the right hemisphere, we suspect that this comprehension deficit in bvFTD was not primarily linguistic in nature, but was related instead to the resource limitations that contributed to their comprehension deficits. Consistent with this view, many of the same GM regions implicated in subjects’ grammatical comprehension deficit were associated with their difficulty understanding lengthy sentences. Among these were prefrontal areas important for strategic organisation, the anterior cingulate region important for attention and initiation and posterior-superior temporal and inferior parietal regions important for short-term memory. Consistent with the bilateral nature of the brain regions implicated in the patients’ grammatical comprehension difficulty, extensive correlations related comprehension performance to WM disease in CC and corona radiata. Moreover, similar WM regions were implicated in grammatical and resource-related aspects of sentence comprehension. This suggests a common source of impairment across both aspects of sentences. Additional work correlating measures of executive functioning with WM disease is needed to confirm these observations.

Sentence comprehension deficits in lvPPA and svPPA

We found a deficit for centre-embedded sentences in lvPPA. While a preliminary report described a deficit for lengthy sentences in lvPPA,8 we did not find this. This discrepancy may have been due in part to differences in the materials used in these studies. We found left temporal-parietal atrophy in lvPPA, as described elsewhere.14 ,32 Atrophy in this GM distribution was related directly to difficulty understanding centre-embedded sentences. We also found that reduced FA was related to grammatical comprehension difficulty in IFO and CC, as we found in naPPA. These observations suggest an alternate explanation for sentence comprehension difficulty in lvPPA that is independent of a short-term memory deficit, and instead involves interruption of a large-scale, frontal-temporal sentence processing network. This will require evaluation by additional studies.

Poor comprehension of centre-embedded sentences was found in svPPA as well. While previous work associated sentence comprehension difficulty with a fundamental deficit in lexical comprehension,7 ,33 we did not find that sentence comprehension correlates with the Pyramid and Palm Tree assessment of lexical and object comprehension. Nevertheless, impaired centre-embedded comprehension was related to GM atrophy in left mid-lateral temporal cortex and to reduced FA in the associated left ILF. This network has been implicated in lexical processing in svPPA.34 Although beyond the scope of the present study, assessments of other aspects of lexical processing are needed before dismissing the association of grammatical comprehension with lexical processing.

References

Supplementary materials

  • Supplementary Data

    This web only file has been produced by the BMJ Publishing Group from an electronic file supplied by the author(s) and has not been edited for content.

    Files in this Data Supplement:

Footnotes

  • Contributors DC: Drafting, language analysis, statistical analysis. CO: Acquisition, statistical analysis. JP: Acquisition, imaging statistical analysis. SA: Language analysis. DI: Acquisition. CM: Imaging analysis. KR: Statistical analysis. MG: Drafting, study concept, imaging analysis, statistical analysis, funding.

  • Funding This work was supported in part by National Institutes of Health (AG017586, NS044266, AG032953, AG038490, NS053488, AG015116).

  • Competing interests None.

  • Ethics approval University of Pennsylvania Institutional Review Board.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.