There is considerable evidence that emotions play a critical modulatory role in episodic memory (Dolcos, Denkova, & Dolcos, 2012; Weymar & Hamm, 2013). For instance, when presenting emotional and neutral materials, and memory is subsequently tested, emotionally arousing items are better remembered and are more robust against forgetting than emotionally neutral items (e.g., Bradley, Greenwald, Petry, & Lang, 1992; Dolcos, LaBar, & Cabeza, 2005; Weymar, Löw, & Hamm, 2011; Wirkner, Löw, Hamm, &Weymar, 2015). Despite convincing evidence for a memory-enhancing effect of emotion, neutral cues can also activate strong emotional memories (e.g., in patients suffering from trauma or stress-related disorders) if these events were associated with an emotional context in which these items were encountered during encoding (e.g., Davachi, 2006; Ranganath, 2010). To understand how emotion interacts with these real-world situations, recent research has therefore focused on the emotion’s effects on memory for items as a part of, or in relation to other items, the so-called contextual relational binding (Chiu, Dolcos, Gonsalves, & Cohen, 2013). Here, we investigated the brain dynamics of contextual/relational memory using event-related potentials (ERPs) recorded during retrieval.

It is a robust finding in memory research that ERPs generated by correctly recognized old stimuli evoke more positive-going waveforms than those elicited by correctly detected new stimuli (for reviews, see Rugg & Curran, 2007; Weymar & Hamm, 2013). This difference in ERP positivity has been labeled the ERP old/new effect. Two ERP old/new differences have been identified reflecting different processes involved in recognition memory: An early old/new effect (300–500 ms) located over frontal brain regions has been related to a-contextual familiarity-based remembering, while centro-parietal old/new differences (>500 ms) have been associated with recollection-based remembering (i.e., retrieval of specific contextual information about the learning episode; Rugg & Curran, 2007; Weymar & Hamm, 2013). ERP studies exploring the electrophysiological correlates of long-lasting enhancing memory effects of emotion showed that emotional events, compared to neutral ones, are associated with larger old/new effects over parietal areas (e.g., Jaworek, Weymar, Löw, & Hamm, 2014; Schaefer, Pottage, & Rickart, 2011; Weymar, Löw, Melzig, & Hamm, 2009; Weymar et al., 2011; Weymar, Löw, Modess, et al., 2010a; Weymar, Löw, Schwabe, & Hamm, 2010b; Wirkner et al., 2015; Wirkner, Weymar, Löw, & Hamm, 2013), suggesting that remembering of emotional events is most likely guided by recollection, rather than familiarity (Dolcos et al., 2005).

The few available behavioral and ERP studies investigating the effect of emotion on memory for associated contextual information, however, have produced mixed results. Some researchers found better memory for contextual features of emotional items (e.g., Doerksen & Shimamura, 2001; Mather & Nesmith, 2008; Nashiro & Mather, 2011) and neutral items embedded in arousing contexts (e.g., Guillet & Arndt, 2009; Pierce & Kensinger, 2011), whereas others found impairments in memory performance (e.g., Bisby & Burgess, 2014; Nashiro & Mather, 2011; Rimmele, Davachi, Petrov, Dougal, & Phelps, 2011; Touryan, Marian, & Shimamura, 2007) or no effects (e.g., Koenig & Mecklinger, 2008; Sharot & Phelps, 2004) when retrieving emotional, relative to neutral, contextual source information (e.g., paired words, background scenes or features). Smith, Dolan, and Rugg (2004) explored ERPs related to immediate recognition of emotionally neutral objects associated with emotional and nonemotional background scenes. These authors found no ERP old/new differences between context categories in early frontal or late parietal regions (replicated by Jaeger, Johnson, Corona, & Rugg, 2009). Similarly, Maratos and Rugg (2001) also did not observe ERP old/new differences when source information (contextual pairings) was tested for neutral words encoded in emotionally arousing sentences compared to emotionally neutral sentences. Hence, compared to ERP results from episodic memory studies using single items, ERP findings related to relational memory seem to indicate no emotional modulation in contextual memory processes.

One potential contributing factor for the lack of emotional effects might be the length of the retention interval (for discussion concerning item memory, see Weymar & Hamm, 2013). Most available studies have used immediate testing but emotional advantages in single-item memory have often been augmented over time, in part by facilitating consolidation processes (e.g., Quevedo et al., 2003; McGaugh, 2004; LaBar & Cabeza, 2006; Ritchey, Dolcos, & Cabeza, 2008). For delayed intervals, greater memory accuracy (Weymar & Hamm, 2013), stronger recollective experience, and larger ERP old/new differences have been reported for emotionally arousing stimuli (Schaefer et al., 2011; Sharot, Delgado, & Phelps, 2004; Weymar et al., 2010b), especially for unpleasant events (Weymar et al., 2011). Thus, emotion might also facilitate contextual or relational binding after a longer delay (as suggested by Pierce & Kensinger, 2011). In accordance with this suggestion, in behavioral research it has been observed that recognition of emotional contextual cues compared to neutral ones is enhanced after long (such as 1 or 7 days), but not after short delays (Pierce & Kensinger, 2011; Sharot & Phelps, 2004). Recently, two ERP studies have investigated emotional contextual binding (Jaeger et al., 2009; Jaeger & Rugg, 2012) on a delayed test (24 hours). However, Jaeger and colleagues found no better memory for neutral objects encoded in emotionally unpleasant background scenes compared to those neutral objects associated with neutral scenes, indicating that beneficial effects of contexts may occur after longer consolidation periods (c.f., Pierce & Kensinger, 2011).

One other important factor to be considered in emotional relational memory is the manner in which stimuli are bound (MacKay, Shafto, Taylor, Marian, Abrams, & Dyer, 2004; Mather, 2007). According to the arousal-biased competition (ABC) theory (Mather & Sutherland, 2011), emotional arousal can lead to enhancement and impairment among neutral stimuli competing for attention, depending on high or low priority (due to bottom-up perceptual salience or top-down attentional focus). The theory further suggests that emotional arousal may enhance associative memory for features of high priority items (e.g., color or location of an item) and impair memory for neutral items, when presented at the same time with emotional items. However, if the association between emotional cues and neutral items is well integrated, memory for neutral items may improve due to the acquisition of high priority. In order to prevent competition between emotional and neutral stimuli, in the current study participants were instructed to imagine the object as a part of the scene to promote the binding between object and background (cf. Awipi & Davachi, 2008). Moreover, unlike prior studies (e.g., Smith, Dolan, et al., 2004; Jaeger & Rugg, 2012), in which objects were presented visually separated from the background scenes (e.g., surrounded with white background and yellow frame), objects were unframed and presented with transparent background to further facilitate object/context integration. Finally, objects were presented first, followed by the background scene, to avoid direct competition between emotional backgrounds and neutral objects, as it might occur when the emotional stimulus is introduced first (cf. Jaeger & Rugg, 2012).

Thus, to further investigate the role of emotion on context binding, we explored electrophysiological correlates of recognition memory for neutral events embedded in emotional contexts, taking into account various critical factors facilitating emotional context memory (i.e., delayed retention interval and event-context integration). Furthermore, some memory studies (e.g., Bradley et al., 1992; Dolcos et al., 2005; Weymar et al., 2009) found that emotionally arousing stimuli are better remembered than low arousing neutral stimuli, observing no differences in the retrieval of unpleasant and pleasant material, whereas others observed a preference in remembering negative information over positive and neutral information (e.g., for associative memory, see Pierce & Kensinger, 2011; for item memory, see Weymar et al., 2011). In order to explore the effects of arousal and emotional valence on memory binding in healthy individuals, both high arousing pleasant and unpleasant pictures were included as contexts in the present study.

The experimental procedure consisted of two phases (see Smith, Dolan, et al., 2004, for a similar design): An incidental encoding task where emotionally neutral objects were presented in the context of emotionally neutral or emotionally arousing background scenes; and a recognition memory task in which item (old vs. new) and context (pleasant vs. unpleasant vs. neutral background picture) was tested. Based on previous studies reporting memory enhancing-effects in the context of emotion (e.g., Guillet & Arndt, 2009; Pierce & Kensinger, 2011), we made a first prediction concerning behavioral performance: (a) we expected that objects encoded in emotional contexts would be better remembered than objects encoded in neutral contexts after a 1-week retention interval. Concerning associated brain dynamics we made a second prediction: (b) we expected to find larger parietal ERP old/new differences (500–700 ms) for neutral objects previously paired with emotional, relative to neutral background pictures, possibly reflecting explicit recollection.

Materials and method

Participants

Participants were 30 healthy students (26 women, 4 men; mean age = 22.7 years; 3 left-handed, 27 right-handed) from the University of Greifswald, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision, who participated for course credits or financial compensation. All participants were native German speakers. Each individual provided written informed consent for a protocol approved by the Review Board of the German Psychological Society (DGPs). Data from three participants were excluded due to excessive artifacts in the electroencephalogram (EEG) data.

Stimulus materials

A total of 288 neutral objects and 144 background scenes were used in the present experiment. Neutral objects were selected from two different standardized sets: The Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS; Brodeur, Dionne-Dostie, Montreuil, & Lepage, 2010) and the ecological adaptation of Snodgrass and Vanderwart (Moreno-Martínez & Montoro, 2012).Footnote 1 Objects belonged to a variety of different semantic categories (e.g., office supplies, electronics, household objects) and were grouped in six different sets carefully matched in terms of semantic category, familiarity, object agreement, and manipulability, according to the normative ratings of the standard samples (see BOSS and ecological adaptation of Snodgrass and Vanderwart norms). Background scenes were selected from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang, Bradley & Cuthbert, 2008), consisting of 48 pleasant (e.g., erotic, adventure, babies, small animals), 48 neutral (e.g., buildings, landscapes, neutral human faces), and 48 unpleasant (e.g., mutilation, attack, disgust, accident) pictures.Footnote 2 Mean (SD) valence and arousal ratings (respectively) for the three picture valence categories were as follows: Pleasant: 6.94 (0.63), 5.84 (0.77); neutral: 5.27 (0.82), 3.51 (0.57); and unpleasant: 2.73 (0.85), 5.93 (0.57). Pleasant, neutral and unpleasant pictures did not differ in complexity, brightness, and contrast (all ps > .23).

During encoding, neutral objects were presented superimposed on an emotional or neutral background scene. Participants were presented with one of six encoding lists (for list construction, see Jaeger et al., 2009; Smith, Dolan, et al., 2004), each consisting of 144 object/background pairs, with no more than two pictures from the same valence presented consecutively. Each neutral object set was paired with a neutral background scene in one list, an unpleasant background in another, a pleasant background scene in a third, and was available as a novel object set for the remaining three lists.

Procedure

During encoding, 144 objects were superimposed on 144 background scenes. Objects (mean vertical and horizontal visual angle of 4.96°) and background scenes (vertical visual angle of 7.73°, horizontal visual angle of 10.93°) were presented on a 20-in. computer monitor located 150 cm in front of the participant. In contrast to previous studies (e.g., Jaeger et al., 2009; Jaeger & Rugg, 2012; Smith, Dolan, et al., 2004, Smith, Henson, Dolan, & Rugg, 2004; Smith, Henson, Rugg, & Dolan, 2005), in which objects were visually separated (white background and/or yellow frame), here objects were presented with transparent background and without frame to facilitate object-scene binding. Objects were presented in one of four quadrants of the screen. The four positions of the objects were selected randomly and with the same probability for each background category. After 3,000 ms, an emotional or neutral scene was added as background. Each object and scene pairing was presented for 5,000 ms with an intertrial interval (ITI) of 3,000, 3,500, or 4,000 ms. The encoding phase was separated into two blocks in which all 144 object-scene pairings were presented and after a break of 1 minute, the exact 144 same object/scene pairings were repeated (but in randomized order). Participants were instructed to imagine that the object is a part of the scene and to indicate after object/scene offset whether the imagination was successful or not by pressing a “yes” or a “no” button on a response pad (see Fig. 1). No mention of a memory test was made (incidental encoding).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic view of the stimulus presentation during encoding and retrieval

One week after encoding, participants returned to the lab for a memory task, in which old and new objects were presented. The task took place in a sound-attenuated dimly lit room. Each participant viewed 288 objects (i.e., 144 old and 144 novel objects). Of the 144 old objects, 48 objects had been encoded in the context of an unpleasant scene, 48 objects in the context of a pleasant scene, and 48 objects in the context of a neutral scene. Each object was presented in the center of the screen (mean vertical and horizontal visual angle of 5.92°) without context for 3,000 ms with a preceding fixation cross of 500 ms.

Participants were instructed to decide whether each object had previously been seen during encoding or not. Following object offset,Footnote 3 the question “Old/New?” appeared on the screen, and participants pressed the “old” button on a keyboard if they remembered the object or else the “new” button. When the object was recognized as old, subjects were asked to remember source information cues: the location the object was presented at during encoding (i.e., which quadrant of the screen) and the background paired with it (i.e., pleasant, neutral, or unpleasant; see Fig. 1). Participants were instructed to avoid eye blinks and body movements.

Apparatus and data analysis

EEG signals were recorded continuously from 257 electrodes using an Electrical Geodesics (EGI) HydroCel high-density EEG system with NetStation software on a Macintosh computer. The EEG recording was digitized at a rate of 250 Hz, using vertex sensor (Cz) as recording reference. Scalp impedance for each sensor was kept below 30 kΩ, as recommended by manufacturer guidelines. All channels were band-pass filtered online from 0.1 to 100 Hz. Offline reduction was performed using ElectroMagnetic EncephaloGraphy Software (EMEGS; Peyk, De Cesarei, & Junghöfer, 2011) and included low-pass filtering at 40 Hz, artifact detection, sensor interpolation, baseline correction, and conversion to the average reference (Junghöfer, Elbert, Tucker, & Rockstroh, 2000). Stimulus-synchronized epochs were extracted from 100 ms before to 1,200 ms after picture onset and baseline corrected (100 ms prior to stimulus onset). Extracted epochs were corrected for eye movement and blink artifacts using the MATLAB-based toolbox BioSig (Vidaurre, Sander, & Schlögl, 2011).

For each participant, separated ERP averages were computed for each sensor and condition. Only trials with correct responses for old and new objects were included in ERP averages. Due to the low number of hits in the source memory task (location and background), only ERPs during the item recognition task (old vs. new) were analyzed. The identification of sensor clusters representative for maximal old/new effects were based on visual inspection of the waveforms and based on previous studies (Jaeger et al., 2009; Jaeger & Rugg, 2012; Maratos & Rugg, 2001; Smith, Dolan, et al., 2004; Weymar et al., 2009, 2010b; Weymar, Löw, Modess, et al. 2010a; Wirkner et al., 2013, 2015). Because recent studies observed context effects at earlier (<300 ms) stages of processing (Curran & Dien, 2003; Tsivilis, Otten, & Rugg, 2001; Voss & Paller, 2009), especially for emotionally arousing events (Jaeger et al., 2009; Jaeger & Rugg, 2012; Schaefer et al., 2011; Smith, Dolan, et al., 2004), we also examined ERP differences in an early time window. Mean ERP amplitudes were analyzed in a 180 to 220 ms and 300 to 500 ms time window over frontal brain regions (EGI HydroCel sensors 5, 6, 7, 8, 14, 15, 16, 22, 23, and 29), and in a 400 to 700 ms time window over centro-parietal brain regions (EGI HydroCel sensors 9, 45, 80, 81, 89, 90, 130, 131, 132, 186, and 257). Recognition-related ERP data for each time window were analyzed in two separate ANOVAs, including the factors Memory (old objects vs. new objects) and Context (neutral objects encoded in pleasant contexts vs. neutral contexts vs. unpleasant contexts). If there was no difference between objects encoded in pleasant and unpleasant contexts, an ANOVA with the factor Emotion (neutral objects encoded in pleasant and unpleasant contexts vs. neutral context) was performed.

For behavioral performance data, hit rate was analyzed using an ANOVA involving the factor Context (objects encoded in pleasant context vs. neutral context vs. unpleasant context). For the source task, hit rates for stimulus location (hit location/hit old object) and background recognition (hit context/hit old object) were analyzed in separate ANOVAs involving the factor Context. Again, if pleasant and unpleasant contexts did not differ, an ANOVA with the factor Emotion (objects encoded in emotional context vs. neutral context) was performed. For effects involving repeated measures, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction for nonsphericity was used when necessary.

Results

Behavioral data

Memory performance for old (item and source memory data) and new objects is listed in Table 1.

Table 1 Hit rate means (standard deviation) for new and old objects encoded in pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant contexts.

Item memory

Hit rates for objects previously paired with emotional pictures tended to be higher than for objects encoded in a neutral context, Emotion: t(29) = 1.96, p = .059, d = .19. No differences were found between hit rates for objects that had been paired with pleasant and unpleasant backgrounds, t(29) = 1.08, p = .28, d = .13, suggesting that memory for neutral objects benefits from binding with emotionally arousing contexts irrespective of their valence during encoding.

Source accuracy rates

Hit rates for remembered objects location were low and similar for emotional and neutral contexts (F < 1), indicating that emotional context did not influence memory for source location. Overall, correct source memory for background pictures was low, and did not differ from the probability of guessing, that is, from the proportion of randomly choosing a correct contextual source (.33; ps > .286). However, there were differences as a function of picture content, Context: F(2, 58) = 10.37, p < .001, ηp 2 = .26. Unpleasant, F(1, 29) = 5.13, p < .05 , ηp 2 = .15, and neutral, F(1, 29) = 19.9, p < .001, ηp 2 = .41, contexts were better classified than pleasant contexts. Recognition for correct background also differed for unpleasant and neutral scenes, F(1, 29) = 5.5, p < .05, ηp 2 =.16. The probability of committing a false alarm by mistakenly identifying a context as neutral (.53) significantly differed from the chance level, t(29) = 5.8, p < .001, and from the probability of erroneously identifying a context as pleasant, .4; t(29) = 3.6, p < .001, d = .63, and unpleasant, .4; t(29) = 3.59, p < .001, d = .63, indicating a response bias toward neutral contexts. Based on the assumption that the response bias is a good estimate of the bias operating for object recognition, Maratos and Rugg (2001) suggest that the probability of recollection of context information can be obtained from the following formula: pCorrect source = pRecollection + pGuess × (1 - pRecollection). After applying the formula, the probabilities of correct background recognition were higher for pleasant (.77) and unpleasant (.76) compared to neutral contexts (.74), in accordance with the item memory performance.

ERP encoding data

To exclude a priori object differences, we compared ERPs in response to neutral objects subsequently encoded in emotional or neutral context in the first block of the encoding session. No ERP differences were found at any time window or sensor location (F < 1).

ERP recognition data

Frontal ERP effect

Figure 2A illustrates the grand average ERPs extracted from the frontal cluster for old objects previously encoded in the context of a pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant scene and new objects.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Frontal ERP Effect. A: Grand average ERPs in response to correctly recognized new (gray dotted line) and old objects encoded in pleasant (blue line), neutral (black line), and unpleasant (red line) contexts. ERPs are averaged across electrodes within a frontal sensor cluster. B: Correlations between the mean amplitude of the frontal ERPs positivity at 180–220 ms (black line) and 300–500 ms (gray line) time window, and hit rate for objects encoded in unpleasant contexts. C: Display of the scalp topographies of the ERP differences. The upper scalp differences plot the early (180–200 ms) emotional ERP effects; the lower topographies represent the ERP old/new effect at 300–500 ms. (Color figure online)

Early ERP Emotional effect (180–220 ms)

For the early time window, no ERP old/new difference was found (Memory: F < 1). However, frontal positivity in the time window between 180 and 220 ms was modulated by context, F(2, 52) = 3.83, p < .05, ηp 2 = .13. Follow-up testing showed that objects previously paired with emotional pictures prompted a larger positivity than objects paired with neutral pictures, Emotion: F(1, 26) = 4.17, p = .05, ηp 2 = .14 (see Fig. 2C). No significant ERP differences were found between objects paired with unpleasant scenes compared to objects paired with pleasant scenes, F(1, 26) = 3.2, p = .09, ηp 2 = .09. When comparing new objects with old objects from different encoding contexts, we observed that novel objects also produced a larger ERP positivity than objects previously encoded in neutral contexts, F(1, 26) = 4.96, p < .05, ηp 2 = .16.

Early ERP Old/New effect (300–500 ms)

ERPs for old objects were modulated by Context, F(1, 52) = 3.81, p < .05, ηp 2 = .13. Enhanced positivity was found for objects previously paired with unpleasant contexts, compared to pleasant, F(1, 26) = 8.54, p < .01, ηp 2 = .25, and neutral contexts, F(1, 26) = 4.09 p = .05, ηp 2 = .14. Although the main effect of memory was not significant, Memory: F(1, 26) = 1.55, p =.22, ηp 2 = .05, only objects from unpleasant backgrounds showed a significant old/new effect: unpleasant contexts vs. new objects, F(1, 26) = 8.58, p < .01, ηp 2 = .25. Objects paired with pleasant and neutral backgrounds did not show such an old/new difference (F < 1; see Fig. 2C).

Parietal ERP effect

Figure 3A illustrates the grand average ERPs for correctly recognized neutral objects encoded in the context of a pleasant, neutral, or unpleasant scene, and for new objects from a representative centro-parietal cluster.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Parietal ERP effect. A: Grand average ERPs evoked by correctly recognized new (gray dotted line) and old objects encoded in pleasant (blue), neutral (black), and unpleasant (red) contexts. The waveforms represent the ERPs averaged across electrodes within a centro-parietal sensor cluster. B: Correlation between the mean amplitude of the parietal ERP positivity (400–700 ms) and hit rate for objects encoded in unpleasant contexts. C: Display of the scalp topographies of the ERP old/new differences (objects in emotional contexts and neutral contexts vs. new objects). (Color figure online)

Late ERP Old/New effect (400–700 ms)

As expected, correctly recognized old objects elicited a larger positive-going waveform than correctly detected new objects, Memory: F(1, 26) = 12.12, p < .005, ηp 2= .32. This old/new difference was modulated by encoding context, F(2, 52) = 3.14, p = .05, ηp 2 = .11. Specifically, objects paired with emotional backgrounds elicited a larger ERP positivity than novel objects: unpleasant contexts vs. new objects, F(1, 26) = 4.52, p < .05, ηp 2 = .15, (pleasant contexts vs. new objects): F(1, 26) = 24,74, p < .001, ηp 2 = .49. However, objects paired with neutral background scenes did not show the parietal old/new effect: neutral contexts vs. new objects, F(1, 26) = 1.53, p = .23, ηp 2 = .06 (see Fig. 3C).

Correlations between recognition performance and ERP effects

Significant correlations between hit rate performance and ERPs were only observed for unpleasant contexts in the 180 to 220 ms, 300 to 500 ms (see Fig. 2B), and 400 to 700 ms (see Fig. 3B) time window. Hit rates for objects encoded in unpleasant contexts showed a positive relation with the ERP amplitude (180–220 ms, r = .37, p = .03, one-tailed; 300–500 ms, r = .43, p < .05; 400–700 ms, r =.35, p = .035, one-tailed), suggesting that higher memory accuracy was related to larger positivity for objects encoded in the unpleasant context (see Fig. 2B, Fig. 3B). This relation was not found for objects encoded with pleasant or neutral contexts in either time windows (rs < .27, ps > .17).

Discussion

In the current study, ERPs were used to investigate long-term recognition memory for neutral objects that had been encoded in an emotionally arousing or neutral context. We found that ERPs during retrieval were sensitive to context differences at encoding at three different time windows. We observed that old objects previously paired with emotional contexts prompted an enhanced early (180–220 ms) positivity (P2) over frontal sites. Furthermore, correct recognition was associated with larger centro-parietal ERP old/new differences (400–700 ms) for objects encoded in both pleasant and unpleasant but not in neutral contexts and associated with early frontal ERP old/new differences (300–500 ms), particularly for unpleasant contexts. Our results indicate that emotion improves associative binding and engages both automatic perceptual mechanisms and explicit recognition processes at retrieval, such as familiarity and recollection processes.

Although the effect was only marginal in the current experiment, we found enhanced memory performance for neutral objects associated with emotional contexts, in comparison to neutral contexts. This finding is in line with many previous studies reporting emotional memory-enhancing effects for single items and contexts (Bradley et al., 1992; Doerksen & Shimamura, 2001; Dolcos et al., 2005; Guillet & Arndt, 2009; Jaworek et al., 2014; Maratos & Rugg, 2001; Mather & Nesmith, 2008; Nashiro & Mather, 2011; Pierce & Kensinger, 2011; Smith, Dolan, et al., 2004; Weymar et al., 2009, 2010b, 2011; Weymar Löw, Modess, et al., 2010a; Wirkner et al., 2013, 2015; but see Jaeger et al., 2009; Jaeger & Rugg. 2012), suggesting that emotional arousal improves long-term associative memory.

In ERPs, correctly recognized old items compared to correctly classified new items elicited enhanced ERP positivity, replicating a large body of previous work on memory research with short and long retention intervals (e.g., Rugg, Mark, Walla, Schloerscheidt, Birch, & Allan, 1998; see for reviews Rugg & Curran, 2007; Weymar & Hamm, 2013). As expected, larger ERP old/new differences were observed for neutral objects encoded in emotionally arousing, but not in emotionally neutral contexts over parietal regions. Because a parietal ERP old/new effect has been related to the process of contextual remembering (Wilding, 2000; Wilding & Rugg, 1996; see Rugg & Curran, 2007, for review), our finding indicates that retrieval of emotional (unpleasant and pleasant) contexts engages recollection processes. This clearly replicates previous ERP picture memory studies that found enhanced parietal ERP old/new effects for emotional pictures (e.g., Ferrari, Bradley, Codispoti, Karlsson, & Lang, 2013; Jaworek et al., 2014; Weymar et al., 2009; Weymar et al., 2010b, 2011; Wirkner et al., 2013, 2015). Interestingly, our result contrasts with prior work where comparable ERP old/new effects were found when using emotional and neutral pictures as contexts (Jaeger et al., 2009; Jaeger & Rugg, 2012; Smith, Dolan, et al., 2004). One reason for this discrepancy could be related to item-context binding processes (Mackay et al., 2004; Mather, 2007), which were facilitated methodologically in the present study. It has been observed that contextual memory increases when neutral items are well integrated with other items or contexts (Murray & Kensinger, 2012, 2013), especially when the latter are arousing (Guillet & Arndt, 2009; Pierce & Kensinger, 2011). Compared to previous studies (e.g., Jaeger & Rugg, 2012), we facilitated item-context pairing by integrating the object into the scene.

Another reason that might be critical is stimulus order presentation (Sakaki, Fryer, & Mather, 2014; Sutherland & Mather, 2012). Based on the ABC theory (Mather & Sutherland, 2011), recent studies have observed that emotionally arousing stimuli distinctly modulate memory for preceding and following neutral target stimuli (Sakaki et al., 2014; Sutherland & Mather, 2012). Thus, if emotional events are preceded by a neutral target stimulus (i.e., high priority), emotional arousal further enhances memory for highly prioritized neutral items; however, if the emotional event is followed by a neutral target stimulus, emotional arousal captures attentional resources and impairs perceptual processing of the neutral target item, hindering the memory-enhancing effects (Sakaki et al., 2014; Sutherland & Mather, 2012). Contrary to prior studies (e.g., Jaeger et al., 2009; Jaeger & Rugg, 2012), we intended to avoid competition for attention resources by presenting the object before the context image appeared.

However, most critically contributing to the emotional enhancing effects observed in the present data is probably the 1-week interval between the encoding and the retrieval session. A number of studies have demonstrated that longer retention periods facilitate consolidation processes, resulting in memory-enhancing effects (e.g., LaBar & Cabeza, 2006), especially for emotionally, compared to neutral, arousing events (e.g., Dolcos et al., 2005; Pierce & Kensinger, 2011; Weymar et al., 2009; see Weymar & Hamm, 2013, for review). In contrast to a number of previous emotional associative memory studies (e.g., Smith, Dolan, et al., 2004; Maratos & Rugg, 2001; Jaeger et al., 2009), we promoted consolidation processes by means of long retention periods.

To summarize, the ERP parietal old/new effect findings seem to indicate that emotional contexts enhance electrophysiological correlates of long-term recollection memory for integrated neutral material. In addition to the parietal ERP old/new effects, emotional context modulated frontal ERP correlates of familiarity-based recognition. Compared to pleasant and neutral contexts, objects embedded in unpleasant background scenes generated enhanced frontal ERP old/new differences between 300 and 500 ms. A number of studies propose that relational memory depends upon recollection-based mechanisms (Rugg & Curran, 2007; Wilding & Rugg, 1996). However, recent behavioral (Diana, Yonelinas, & Ranganath, 2008, 2010) and electrophysiological studies (Addante, Raganath, & Yonelinas, 2012; Diana, Van den Boom, Yonelinas, & Ranganath, 2011; Ecker, Zimmer, & Groh-Bordin, 2007a, b; Mollison & Curran, 2012; Peters & Daum, 2009; Speer & Curran, 2007; Tibon, Ben-Zvi, & Levy, 2014; Tibon, Gronau, Scheuplein, Mecklinger, & Levy, 2014; Tibon & Levy, 2014; Tsivilis et al., 2001; Zheng, Li, Xiao, Broster, & Jiang, 2015) have found that familiarity-related processes could also contribute to source recognition (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993; Yonelinas, 2002). In an attempt to shed light on the familiar-related processes embedded in source memory, Diana et al. (2008, 2010; Diana et al., 2011) investigated the impact of encoding processes on familiarity-based recognition. The authors observed that exclusively high-unitized associations––that is, item-context pairings encoded as a single unit––elicited frontal ERP old/new effects, whereas low-unitized associations only elicited recollection-related ERPs (Diana et al., 2011). In the same direction, a number of studies have observed that when item-source associations can be processed together at encoding, familiarity processes are also enhanced during recognition. Thus, when item and source information are encoded in the same modality (visual) vs. different modality (visual and auditory; Tibon, Ben-Zvi, et al., 2014; Tibon, Gronau, et al., 2014; Tibon & Levy, 2014), when the nature of the association item-source is intrinsic versus extrinsic (Ecker et al., 2007a, b; Zheng et al., 2015), or when source refers to salient contextual information (Mollison & Curran, 2012), source recognition prompts larger familiarity-related ERPs, suggesting that memory for source information is strengthened (Addante et al., 2012; Chiu et al., 2013; Curran, 2004). In the present study, no explicit instructions for unitization were given; however, as commented above, we promoted item-context binding processes at encoding. It might be that this instruction accentuated item-context unitization for unpleasant associations and thus promoted stronger familiarity-based recognition of negative source information. However, clear conclusions cannot be derived because judgments regarding the strength of the associations were not directly recorded in the present study. Future research should assess the emotional effect on strength/unitization of item-context associations in order to clarify the present finding.

Intriguingly, we found differences of context association at early stages of processing. Neutral stimuli encoded in emotional compared to neutral contexts elicited a larger early (180–220 ms) positive-going waveform over frontal sites. This early emotional effect resembles those reported by previous source memory studies (Jaeger et al., 2009; Jaeger & Rugg, 2012; Smith, Dolan, et al., 2004), suggesting that neutral stimuli acquired emotionally arousing properties. This emotion effect may relate to the transmission of emotional attributes from contexts to objects by means of associative binding mechanisms (Smith, Dolan, et al., 2004), enhancing their processing (e.g., Öhman & Mineka, 2001; Vuilleumier, 2005). A substantial body of research has suggested the existence of highly elaborated networks, involving the amygdala (Öhman & Mineka, 2001) and cortical regions (Pessoa & Adolphs, 2010; Van Gaal & Lamme, 2012) that rapidly identify motivationally relevant events (Brosch, Sander, Pourtois, & Scherer, 2008; Pourtois, Grandjean, Sander, & Vuilleumier, 2004; Steinberg, Bröckelmann, Rehbein, Dobel, & Junghöfer, 2013; Wieser et al., 2014) and further provide a rapid learning capacity (cf. Steinberg et al., 2013). Thus, our data seem to indicate that neutral objects embedded in emotional contexts have rapidly—after two pairings!—acquired long-lasting emotional relevance.

An alternative but not excluding interpretation could also link the early frontal ERP with memory-related mechanisms. The frontal P2 has been related to the activation of representations retrieved in long-term memory due to the detection of visual information (Curran & Dien, 2003; Tsivilis et al., 2001; Voss & Paller, 2009) under explicit memory testing (Curran, 1999; Voss & Paller, 2009; but see Jaeger & Rugg, 2012). Specifically, it has been observed that the frontal P2 increases when the visual input matches with efficiently stored information, suggesting that the extent of activation of representations in memory indexed by P2 might relate to the phenomenological salience of the stimuli (cf. Voss & Paller, 2009). Consistent with this idea, Schaefer et al. (2011) found that frontal P2 was modulated by the memory strength of emotional events; that is, remembered unpleasant scenes—but not neutral ones—evoked frontal P2 old/new effects; moreover, P2 amplitudes in response to unpleasant images increased with their arousal. In the current study, larger P2 was evoked by neutral objects embedded in emotional compared to neutral background scenes, suggesting that emotional contexts increase the saliency of the events bound with, facilitating their later recognition.

From a dual-process model perspective (Yonelinas, 2002), however, it remains unclear which mnemonic mechanisms underlie the P2 old/new effects. On the one hand, the spatio-temporal attributes of the P2 matches with the frontal familiarity-related old/new effects, suggesting the existence of similar processes between both components. On the other hand, the P2 old/new effect has only been found when the events where labeled as remembered but not when they were considered familiar (Voss & Paller, 2009), indicating that P2 might be related to recollection. Schaefer et al. (2011) found distinct patterns of modulation across arousal levels between P2, frontal and parietal old/new effects, suggesting that different memory subprocesses might be involved for each component (cf. Schaefer et al., 2011). We, however, did not find clear evidence that would allow us to disentangle this question. Future research should focus on elucidating the memory sub-processes underlying this frontal memory index, assessing, for instance, which recognition processes (remember vs. know) have been employed.

Surprisingly, we found that correctly identified new objects also elicited a larger P2 than old objects paired with neutral background scene. A plausible explanation of this effect could be that early frontal activity is related to the processing of novelty/relevant stimuli (Tibon, Gronau, et al., 2014b; Tsivilis et al., 2001; Voss & Paller, 2009). In this line, Ecker, Zimmer, Groh-Bordin, and Mecklinger (2007) observed that novel neutral events generated larger early frontal activity than correctly recognized old neutral stimuli. These authors suggest that the brain response difference between new and old information at early stages of processing might be associated with an enhanced attentional capture (Ecker et al., 2007a). Thus, based on previous research, we interpret this finding as a result of enhanced processing of new objects, along with decreased processing of relatively nonrelevant (associated with neutral source information) old events.

Although pleasant and unpleasant background scenes were matched in terms of emotional arousal, we found that frontal old/new effects were only present for unpleasant-related associations. We further observed that memory performance for objects paired with unpleasant background scenes predicted larger electrophysiological activity at the three ERP memory-related components (P2, frontal and parietal old/new effects). These results are in line with recent memory studies that report enhanced long-term explicit memory for unpleasant-related stimuli, compared to pleasant ones (Pierce & Kensinger, 2011; Ochsner, 2000; Weymar et al., 2011). For instance, Pierce and Kensinger investigated emotional valence and arousal on associative binding after a short (15-min) and longer (1-week) delay and observed that memory performance for unpleasant word pairs was better in comparison to pleasant and neutral word pairs after long, but not after short retention intervals, suggesting that unpleasant-related stimuli acquire a higher long-term relevance. From an evolutionary perspective, memory for stimuli related to potentially dangerous events should be consistent, salient, and stable over time in order to facilitate their detection and avoid them in the future (Dolan, 2002). Furthermore, it has been suggested that pleasant contexts (animals or nurture scenes) do not contain the same type of survival-relevant information as unpleasant contexts (accidents or attacks), making them less relevant for future purposes than negative situations (Ashby, Isen, & Turcken, 1999; Fredrickson, 1998, 2001; Ochsner, 2000). Thus, the current findings may indicate that especially unpleasant contexts facilitate associative long-term memory processes.

One further interpretation of the present results might be related to postencoding arousal effects (Anderson, Wais, & Gabrieli, 2006; Knight & Mather, 2009) that might have facilitated memory for neutral objects. Some studies have observed that presenting neutral stimuli shortly prior to emotionally arousing materials can result in enhanced memory (1-week later) for the preceding event (Anderson et al., 2006). A potential mechanism might be that stress hormones (rapid nongenomic effects of catecholamines and glucocorticoids) released in response to arousing emotional contexts might increase vigilance via interactions between the amygdala and hippocampus formation (e.g., McGaugh, 2013), and facilitate memory storage for the context itself but also for recently experienced-related events.

Although the ability to remember cues associated with potentially threatening events could protect us from dramatic consequences, when these events are extremely distressing, memories can become maladaptive and clinically relevant (Ehlers & Clark, 2000). For instance, in patients suffering from posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), events associated with traumatizing contexts can involuntarily trigger vivid distressing memories in the form of intrusive thoughts, flashbacks, or nightmares (Parson & Ressler, 2012) and, on the other hand, can result in impaired episodic memory for the traumatic event itself (e.g., Brewin, 2013; Flor & Nees, 2014; Hayes, VanElzakker, & Shin, 2012; McNally, 2008; Wilker, Elbert, & Kolassa, 2014). A prominent hypothesis of PTSD etiology suggests that trauma memories are poorly integrated into their context (e.g., time and place) during encoding, thus resulting in retrieval distortions of trauma-related information (Dolcos, 2013; Pannu Hayes et al., 2011), overgeneralization and easy triggering of physically similar cues (Ehlers & Clark, 2000), which might relate to deficits in long-term contextual binding processes. Despite its relevance, little is known about the neural processing of emotional binding mechanisms in PTSD. ERPs might be a useful tool to elucidate the long-term binding deficits in PTSD, providing new insights into the neurobiological alterations of the memory systems in trauma and stress-related disorders.

Conclusion

In summary, we found that neutral information can activate long-term memories due to their emotional associations. Neutral objects encoded in emotional contexts showed prioritized automatic perceptual and explicit mnemonic processing during recognition that was also related to long-term memory performance. Our findings highlight the importance of emotional binding mechanisms in long-term memory processes, which could have also direct clinical relevance for trauma- and stressor-related disorders.