Skip to main content

CASE REPORT article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 20 June 2016
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00307

Alien Hand, Restless Brain: Salience Network and Interhemispheric Connectivity Disruption Parallel Emergence and Extinction of Diagonistic Dyspraxia

Ben Ridley1,2* Marion Beltramone3 Jonathan Wirsich1,2,4 Arnaud Le Troter1,2 Eve Tramoni3,4 Sandrine Aubert5 Sophie Achard6,7 Jean-Philippe Ranjeva1,2 Maxime Guye1,2 Olivier Felician3,4
  • 1Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, CRMBM UMR 7339, Marseille, France
  • 2APHM, Hôpitaux de la Timone, CEMEREM, Marseille, France
  • 3APHM, Hôpitaux de la Timone, Service de Neurologie et Neuropsychologie, Marseille, France
  • 4Aix Marseille Université, Inserm, INS, Institut de Neurosciences des Systèmes, Marseille, France
  • 5AP–HM, Hôpitaux de la Timone & Hôpital Henri-Gastaut, Service de Neurophysiologie Clinique, Marseille, France
  • 6GIPSA-Lab F-38000, University Grenoble Alpes, Grenoble, France
  • 7GIPSA-Lab, F-38000, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Grenoble, France

Diagonistic dyspraxia (DD) is by far the most spectacular manifestation reported by sufferers of acute corpus callosum (CC) injury (so-called “split-brain”). In this form of alien hand syndrome, one hand acts at cross purposes with the other “against the patient’s will”. Although recent models view DD as a disorder of motor control, there is still little information regarding its neural underpinnings, due to widespread connectivity changes produced by CC insult, and the obstacle that non-volitional movements represent for task-based functional neuroimaging studies. Here, we studied patient AM, the first report of DD in patient with complete developmental CC agenesis. This unique case also offers the opportunity to study the resting-state connectomics of DD in the absence of diffuse changes subsequent to CC injury or surgery. AM developed DD following status epilepticus (SE) which resolved over a 2-year period. Whole brain functional connectivity (FC) was compared (Crawford-Howell [CH]) to 16 controls during the period of acute DD symptoms (Time 1) and after remission (Time 2). Whole brain graph theoretical models were also constructed and topological efficiency examined. At Time 1, disrupted FC was observed in inter-hemispheric and intra-hemispheric right edges, involving frontal superior and midline structures. Graph analysis indicated disruption of the efficiency of salience and right frontoparietal (FP) networks. At Time 2, after remission of diagnostic dyspraxia symptoms, FC and salience network changes had resolved. In sum, longitudinal analysis of connectivity in AM indicates that DD behaviors could result from disruption of systems that support the experience and control of volitional movements and the ability to generate appropriate behavioral responses to salient stimuli. This also raises the possibility that changes to large-scale functional architecture revealed by resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (rs-fMRI) may provide relevant information on the evolution of behavioral syndromes in addition to that provided by structural and task-based functional imaging.

Introduction

Acute damage to the corpus callosum (CC) may generate a cluster of clear-cut inter-hemispheric disconnection (IHD) “split-brain” symptoms reflecting the inability to convey sensory information to contralateral motor or linguistic output areas (Tomasch, 1954; Sperry, 1968a). In rare cases it may also produce diagonistic dyspraxia (DD; Akelaitis, 1945), a fascinating neuropsychological syndrome involving involuntary inter-manual conflicts where one hand acts at cross purposes with the other. DD has remained the most representative “alien hand” syndrome, a term encompassing a variety of seemingly goal-directed movements occurring after a brain insult, performed without the intention of the actor (Scepkowski and Cronin-Golomb, 2003; Biran and Chatterjee, 2004; Berlucchi, 2012). Although DD has been observed in cases of acquired CC damage, to our knowledge it has never been described in developmentally-based CC dysgenesis.

Agenesis of the CC (AgCC), occurring in 1/4000 live births (Paul et al., 2007), may be partial or complete; may be found in isolation (primary AgCC) or as part of a wider developmental disorder (Paul et al., 2007); and is commonly associated with epilepsy (Taylor and David, 1998). In contrast to acquired lesions of the CC, primary AgCC has an overall limited and subtle impact on cognition and subjects with AgCC do not display clinically relevant IHD symptoms (Sperry, 1968b). To account for this puzzling fact—known as “Sperry’s paradox” (Sperry, 1968a)—compensatory structural pathways have been suggested. Neuroimaging has recently provided support for macrostructural changes in AgCC, evidencing intra- and interhemispheric white matter tracts providing bilateral links via the posterior and anterior commissures (Tovar-Moll et al., 2014). These are likely to compose, at least partly, the set of compensatory pathways preserving inter-hemispheric transfer. These observations are corroborated by a resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) study showing preserved functional connectivity (FC) between homotopic cortices in AgCC subjects (Tyszka et al., 2011). Additionally, intrinsic connectivity networks (ICNs) in the AgCC group were similar to those identified in controls, suggesting that global functional architecture remains largely preserved in AgCC. Paralleling the clinically-defined Sperry’s paradox, these findings stand in sharp contrast with the drastic disruption of inter-hemispheric FC observed following surgical interruption of the CC (Johnston et al., 2008).

Here, we report on an epileptic AgCC patient who developed pervasive DD behaviors following status epilepticus (SE), with gradual improvement over 2-years. Patient AM offers a rare opportunity to longitudinally study the functional underpinning of DD, without the major impact produced by acute callosal section/lesions on connectomics. By examining rs-fMRI connectivity via edgewise comparison of FC and graph theoretical modeling, we demonstrate large scale disorganization and reorganization paralleling the emergence and extinction of IHD and DD symptoms.

Case Report

Patient History

Patient AM (45 years at first scan) is an ambidextrous man with a long-standing history of complex partial seizures in the setting of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. AM was first referred to our center in 2011. He had been treated by carbamazepine (800 mg/day) and phenobarbital (100 mg/day) over the past two decades and partial seizures had occurred on a monthly basis. Awake EEG demonstrated left temporal slowing and spikes. Brain MRI revealed left hippocampal sclerosis, along with complete AgCC and posterior commissure, but an intact anterior commissure. A first neuropsychological assessment was undertaken in September 2011. On the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III), he demonstrated low to average global cognitive functioning (full IQ = 74), with mild dissociation between verbal and performance scales (respectively VIQ = 80 and PIQ = 70).

In March 2012, AM was admitted for SE of unknown duration, that left him with a right-sided motor weakness (“Todd’s paralysis”) that resolved over a 2-week period. Routine structural brain MRI was unchanged. Lamotrigine was added to his previous medication regimen (up to 400 mg/day). One month later, and despite full resolution of epileptic seizures, he started noticing a series of embarrassing and distressing behaviors, described as unpredictable and uncontrollable, and occurring on a daily basis. The most relevant examples being: (i) walking in his neighborhood, his right hand would suddenly grab a corner post rendering him unable to turn; AM would then remain stuck to the post for several minutes, desperately turning around, until his hand permitted disengagement; he would then trap his right hand in his jacket to keep walking; (ii) while driving his car, his right hand would suddenly turn the steering wheel to the left, despite intending to make a right-turn; he observed similar behaviors with his right foot, which would maintain pressure on the accelerator pedal whilst intending to slow down; (iii) while putting on his trousers with his left hand, his right hand would pull them off; the same behaviors could also occur while attempting to don a pair of socks or a sweat shirt, and several times for each piece of clothing; and (iv) his right hand would “play tricks” on him, spiriting away his wallet from the back pocket of his pants, and refusing to give it back to him.

Neuropsychological Evaluations

AM underwent a detailed assessment in September 2012 (6 months after SE onset). Full IQ was 69, with stable verbal IQ (VIQ = 80) but slightly decreased performance IQ (PIQ = 63). While the DD symptoms constituted the main interferent with AM’s quality of life, additional neuropsychological testing revealed further subtle manifestations of IHD. Right-sided constructional apraxia, right ideomotor apraxia and right visual anomia were evidenced in addition to the right-handed DD described above. Only rightward constructional apraxia is concordant with expectations of IHD in right-handed subjects, and the pattern of deficits in Patient AM suggests a bi-hemispheric organization of language, gestural and constructional skills, with right hemisphere prevalence. With the exception of enduring ideomotor problems in terms of tool-use pantomimes with his right hand, follow-up indicated significant amelioration (September 2013) and extinction (September 2014) of all neuropsychological symptoms (see Supplementary Figure 1). In parallel, DD behaviors were reported as very occasional in September 2013 and had totally resolved over the 6 months preceding the final evaluation in September 2014. At this time, awake EEG was within normal range aside from occasional left temporal slowing and spikes (see Supplementary Figure 2).

Procedure

MRI Acquisition

Patient AM was scanned in December 2012 (Time 1—acute phase) and February 2015 (Time 2—remission). Sixteen control (male, right-handed, mean age: 41 ± 16 years) cross-sectional data-sets were used for comparison. All controls underwent a pre-scan medical interview and had no history of neurological or psychiatric illness, substance abuse or psychotropic medication. Participants gave informed consent to take part in this study, with local Ethics Committee approval (Comité de Protection des Personnes Sud Méditerranée 1).

A 3T Verio scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a 12 channel receiver coil was used to obtain structural high resolution T1-weighted images magnetisation-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MP-RAGE); repetition time (TR) = 1900 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.2 ms, inversion time (TI) = 900 ms, 1 × 1 × 1 mm voxels) and 15 min of functional gradient echo planar images (250 volumes, TR = 3600 ms, TE = 28 ms, flip angle = 90°, 50 axial slices interleaved, 2.5 × 2.5 mm, 122 × 122 matrix). Participants were asked to close their eyes and not to think about anything in particular in a taskless “resting-state” condition.

fMRI Processing and Graph Construction

For a full description of processing and graph construction please see Ridley et al. (2015). Briefly, after realignment and slice-timing (SPM8, Wellcome Trust Centre, London, UK), regional masks in subject-space (FLIRT, FMRIB Software Library1) based on the AAL template (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002) were used to obtain functional time-series from which nuisance regressors obtained from regions of interest (ROIs) in white matter and ventricles and motion parameters (three planes) were removed.

A wavelet transform (MODWT) was used to obtain the range 0.035–0.07 Hz (wavelet scale 2; Achard et al., 2006). The resulting data was used to populate an 84 × 84 correlation matrix. Each cell—or edge—in this correlation matrix represents the Pearson correlation of wavelet coefficient time courses between two non-cerebellar regions of the AAL, and allowed us to compare the strength of correlation between each set of two regions in Patient AM vs. the same regional pairs in controls in an edgewise fashion.

We additionally applied graph theoretical analysis, a complementary technique which can provide information on multiscale, parallel and distributed features of patterns of connectivity (Sporns, 2014). For each individual, three adjacency matrices were created with the highest 10% (349), 20% (697), and 30% (1046) significant FDR-controlled Pearson correlation coefficients between regions, which were used to construct unweighted brain graphs. Degree as well as global and local efficiency were derived using Brainwaver (Version 3.0.2, The R Foundation for Statistical Computing; for detailed review see Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). Briefly, degree is the number of edges connecting to a node and is a broad measure of it importance/centrality (Bernhardt et al., 2013). Efficiency is the inversion of the mean shortest path length (Li,j) between a given node and a given set of other nodes (Onias et al., 2014), including the rest of the network (Global Efficiency, Eglob) or just the immediate neighbors of a node (Local Efficiency, Eloc). In healthy cortex a balance between a region’s ability to subserve its specialized function in collaboration with related structures in the immediate neighborhood (segregation/local integration) and its ability to integrate long-range information from distributed regions (global integration) is thought to be optimal (Achard and Bullmore, 2007).

Control Comparison Sample and Statistical Methodology

To compare AM to controls we used the Crawford-Howell (CH) modified two-tailed t-test specifically designed for case-control comparisons (Crawford and Garthwaite, 2012). The CH-test evaluates the null hypothesis that a case is drawn from the control distribution and the p-value additionally serves as a point-estimate of abnormality of the patient’s score, indicating the proportion of controls more extreme, and both applications were utilized here (see Figure 1). See Crawford and Garthwaite (2012) for formal proof of this dual role, and Monte Carlo simulations demonstrating better control of Type I errors and greater robustness to violations of normality than other case-control approaches. Edgewise comparisons were Bonferroni-corrected at p < 1.43 × 10−5.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Edgewise functional connectivity (FC) changes in Patient AM. (A) Significantly reduced edges (blue lines) in Patient AM compared to controls at Time 1 using the Crawford-Howell (CH) test. Differences were considered significant at a Bonferroni-corrected level of p < 1.43 × 10−5. Red nodes are used to highlight the regions joined by a significantly changed link, and are reproduced for comparison in all figures. (B) Abnormality map at Time 1 (left) and Time 2 (right), making use of the p value generated by the CH test as a point estimate of abnormality to scale edges in size and color (larger, more yellow being more abnormal), indicating the proportion of controls with FC values as extreme as those found for each edge in the patient. Note the bilateral frontal normalization vs. retention of frontoparietal (FP) abnormalities at Time 2. Abbreviations used: Sup, superior; Mid, Middle; Inf, inferior; Supp, Supplementary; Tri, triangular; L, Left; R, Right. Brain networks were visualized with the BrainNet Viewer (Xia et al., 2013).

Graph theoretical metrics were also compared at whole brain and hemispheric scales as well as in 10 canonical ICNs. See Supplementary Figure 3 for network partitions.

The CH-test was also used to confirm Patient AM and controls did not differ in terms of age and motion parameters (Supplementary Information 1).

Results

Edgewise FC

During the acute phase of DD symptoms (Time 1), edgewise analysis indicated a network of significantly reduced (Bonferroni-corrected) interhemispheric connections and intrahemispheric connections in the right (but not left) hemisphere (Figure 1A, Table 1), which abated at the second scan (Time 2). Only a single edge between the left middle and inferior occipital gyri (t(15) = −8, p < 1.43 × 10−5) was found to be significantly reduced relative to controls.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Significantly reduced edges in Patient AM vs. controls at Time 1.

The p-value yielded by the CH test was also used in its role as a point estimate of abnormality, as depicted in Figure 1B. At Time 1, prominent areas of abnormality are indicated fronto-centrally in both hemispheres and fronto-parietally in the right hemisphere. At Time 2, the scan demonstrates the spatial stability of these areas of abnormality, while indicating substantial “improvement”.

Graph Theoretical Analysis

Results from graph analysis (Table 2) at Time 1 indicate disturbances to Eglob, Eloc and in the average number of edges (degree) within the right hemisphere. At Time 2, global and local efficiency disturbances have abated, though enduring degree differences are evident at some sparsities.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Scales/Networks showing significant differences in graph theoretical indices.

At the Intrinsic Connectivity Network scale (Figure 2, Table 2) at Time 1, all metrics were disrupted within the right fronto-parietal network, and Eglob and Eloc within the salience network. At Time 2, the salience network had recovered in terms of topological efficiency and no longer exhibited significantly extreme graph metric estimates relative to controls while the right fronto-parietal network demonstrated enduring disturbances.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Significant changes in graph theoretical indices in intrinsic connectivity networks (ICNs). Differences, considered significant at p < 0.05 indicated by black asterisks, at Time 1 and Time 2 for the topological efficiency of connections between nodes and the entire brain network (Eglob top), with immediate neighbors (Eloc, middle) and the number of edges connecting to a node (Degree, bottom). Gray squares represent graph indices in AM and dark diamonds represent mean graph indices in controls. X-axis indicates connection sparsity as a percentage of all possible connections. Note that y-axis minima and maxima are different across metrics. Abbreviations used: Aud, Auditory; DMN, Default Mode Network, SM, Sensorimotor; Vis, Visual; Prime, Primary; Sec, Secondary; FP, Frontoparietal; Dors, Dorsal; Atten, Attention.

Discussion

Patient AM represents a unique case of DD in a developmentally acallosal patient, highlighting a novel complication of comorbid AgCC and epilepsy, as well as serving as a test case for the use of resting-state analyses in a little-understood disorder.

According to recent theoretical accounts, “alien” movements occur because affordances supplied by the environment reflexively generate motor primitives that are not inhibited by an intended action (Frith et al., 2000; Biran et al., 2006). The patient is aware that an action is produced (via sensory feedback), but the actual action is performed despite no intention and no postulation (feed-forward intention) of a predicted bodily change. Failure of inhibitory signals arising from the supplementary motor area (SMA) acting on a sensorimotor (SM) fronto-parietal system involved in the production and coordination of motor primitives has also been posited, either because of defective inhibition (e.g., lesions of the SMA) or an impairment of its inter-hemispheric transfer (e.g., lesions of the CC; Frith et al., 2000; Biran et al., 2006). Other accounts stress a form of “split-brain” phenomena: movements are purposeful and reasonable given each hemisphere’s competences, but the “intention” is inaccessible and thus “alien” due to failed interhemispheric integration (Verleger et al., 2011).

Activation-fMRI studies have attempted to delineate the neural basis of alien hand syndromes (Assal et al., 2007; Schaefer et al., 2010). Collectively, they suggest that both intentional and non-intentional movements involve a common set of regions, principally primary motor, supplementary motor and pre-motor areas, but differ in the activation of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). However, conclusions regarding the role of the IFG could not be drawn, since the activation studies are in apparent disagreement: associating the IFG with either intentional (Assal et al., 2007) or non-intentional movements (Schaefer et al., 2010).

Given its minimal demands on the individual being scanned, a resting-state approach offers an adjunct to traditional imaging approaches that need not rely on specific symptoms or tasks that may be difficult in DD (Scepkowski and Cronin-Golomb, 2003; Berlucchi, 2012). Our results (Figure 1A) implicate inter-hemispheric disruption between a set of frontal regions identified in the above activation studies (Assal et al., 2007; Schaefer et al., 2010) in both intentional and non-intentional movements (superior mesial, pre-central and SMA regions), as well as those that distinguish them (i.e., the IFG). Additionally, graph analysis implicates reduced efficiency in the salience network (Figure 2). The salience network is a recently described large-scale network that comprises frontoinsular regions and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), along with limbic and subcortical structures (Seeley et al., 2007). Comparisons between intrinsic connectivity and task-based co-activation derived from many thousands of functional datasets indicate the salience network encompasses a wide range of processes including integration of bodily-related information and conflict/error monitoring (Smith et al., 2009), positioning it remarkably well for the transitional role between extero-/interoception and cognition originally posited by Seeley et al. (2007): as a sensory integrator and filter capable of “tagging” information as relevant or irrelevant for higher-level “executive” networks. Concordantly, salience network disruption observed in frontotemporal dementia is thought to be specifically implicated in disinhibited behaviors (Farb et al., 2013; Zhou and Seeley, 2014). Thus, our results may reflect a disruption of a system involved in the ability to select the relevant sensory stimuli, and/or inhibit non-intended actions.

Furthermore, patients experience non-volitional movements as not only unplanned but also as distinctly “other” or “alien” (Scepkowski and Cronin-Golomb, 2003; Biran and Chatterjee, 2004; Biran et al., 2006). Both edgewise and ICN results indicate disruption of an inter-hemispheric network that task-based neuroimaging implicates in distinguishing self/other-generated actions, including dorsomedial frontal cortices and one of the salience network’s key nodes (the insular region; Farrer and Frith, 2002; Farrer et al., 2003; Sperduti et al., 2011). Interestingly, corticobasal degeneration can also result in volitional experience and movement aberrations falling under the rubric of “alien hand” (c.f. Schaefer et al., 2016). In a group of patients suffering from this condition, Wolpe et al. (2014) revealed modified FC of the pre-SMA with a set of fronto-parietal regions that included the anterior component of the salience network.

Taken together, the normalization of connectivity and network organization with remission of DD may reflect a regained ability to detect and disregard irrelevant SM programs primed by environmental affordances, and a restored sense of authorship in self-generated actions. Enduring modifications within a right hemisphere could reflect remaining difficulties in tool-use pantomimes (Supplementary Figure 1), consistent with the role of FP regions in complex actions relying on stored information such as tool use (Wheaton and Hallett, 2007).

We suggest that disconnection symptoms and DD were likely prompted by SE, unbalancing the inter-hemispheric transfer that may have previously transited via an intact anterior commissure which subserved the patient’s previously “normal” behavioral profile (Franz, 2012; Winter and Franz, 2014). Comorbities in patient AM, while potential sources of vulnerability seem unlikely to account for the acute onset and trajectory described here. Ongoing seizure activity cannot explain AM’s DD, since seizures have fully resolved. Despite distributed network effects of epilepsy (Bettus et al., 2009, 2010; Guye et al., 2010; Ridley et al., 2015), changes involving the regions identified here are for the most part identified in children and young adults and represent developmental and non-acute processes (Ibrahim et al., 2014; Luo et al., 2014; Li et al., 2015; Wei et al., 2015). Likewise, despite drastic and life-long structural modifications (Kasprian et al., 2013; Jakab et al., 2015) AgCC it is not equivalent to callosotomy (Owen et al., 2013), and recent evidence suggests acallosal brains can support a largely “normal” cognitive and intrinsic FC repertoire in the form of preserved homotopic connectivity and bilaterally symmetric ICN architecture (Tyszka et al., 2011; Tovar-Moll et al., 2014). This conclusion is bolstered by the fact that partitioning at gross anatomical scales (Supplementary Figure 4) does not yield equivalent graph metric changes, underlining the relevance of the functional network partitions employed here.

However, despite the forgoing, larger homogenous samples—including separate longitudinal control, AgCC and epilepsy patient groups—are certainly necessary to address the limitations of this study and confirm Patient AM as a general model of alien hand syndromes. Furthermore, the test-retest reliability of connectomics metrics places a lower limit on the interpretability of data, though the stability of our data over time as suggested by Figure 1B militate against this being the main determinant of our results. Future work will also need to speak to known sources of variance in connectomics, including age, gender, handedness, hemispheric dominance, medication, and vigilance state (Bettus et al., 2009; Liu et al., 2009; Tian et al., 2011; Vlooswijk et al., 2011; Tagliazucchi and Laufs, 2014).

Conclusion

Our data suggest that the salience network and interhemispheric connectivity play a role in supporting the experience and control of volitional movements and the ability to select the most relevant among internal and extrapersonal stimuli in order to produce appropriate behavior. Bearing methodological and sampling caveats in mind, the current work represents a promising first indication that rs-fMRI is relevant to the understanding of alien hand syndromes over time and, more broadly, of neuropsychiatric manifestations that may benefit in the same way from an adjunct to structural and task-based functional imaging. In this spirit, we hope that AM’s case will serve to marshal resources in pursuit of a better understanding of this difficult and rare disorder.

Author Contributions

BR, OF, MG, J-PR, MB and JW contributed to the design of the work, acquisition analysis and interpretation of data. ALeT, ET, SaA and SoA contributed to the acquisition, analysis and interpretation of data. All authors revised, contributed to and approved the final manuscript. All co-authors agree on all aspects of the work and ensure that questions related to the accuracy and integrity of any part of the submitted work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Funding

The funding sources (“PHRC-I 2013” EPI-SODIUM (grant number 2014-27)) had no involvement in study design; collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00307/abstract

References

Achard, S., and Bullmore, E. (2007). Efficiency and cost of economical brain functional networks. PLoS Comput. Biol. 3:e17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Achard, S., Salvador, R., Whitcher, B., Suckling, J., and Bullmore, E. (2006). A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs. J. Neurosci. 26, 63–72. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3874-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akelaitis, A. (1945). Studies on the corpus callosum. IV. Diagonistic dyspraxia in epileptics following partial and complete section of the corpus callosum. Am. J. Psychiatry 101, 594–599. doi: 10.1176/ajp.101.5.594

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assal, F., Schwartz, S., and Vuilleumier, P. (2007). Moving with or without will: functional neural correlates of alien hand syndrome. Ann. Neurol. 62, 301–306. doi: 10.1002/ana.21173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berlucchi, G. (2012). Frontal callosal disconnection syndromes. Cortex 48, 36–45. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernhardt, B. C., Hong, S., Bernasconi, A., and Bernasconi, N. (2013). Imaging structural and functional brain networks in temporal lobe epilepsy. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:624. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00624

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bettus, G., Bartolomei, F., Confort-Gouny, S., Guedj, E., Chauvel, P., Cozzone, P. J., et al. (2010). Role of resting state functional connectivity MRI in presurgical investigation of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 81, 1147–1154. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2009.191460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bettus, G., Guedj, E., Joyeux, F., Confort-Gouny, S., Soulier, E., Laguitton, V., et al. (2009). Decreased basal fMRI functional connectivity in epileptogenic networks and contralateral compensatory mechanisms. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 1580–1591. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biran, I., and Chatterjee, A. (2004). Alien hand syndrome. Arch. Neurol. 61, 292–294. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.2.292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biran, I., Giovannetti, T., Buxbaum, L., and Chatterjee, A. (2006). The alien hand syndrome: what makes the alien hand alien? Cogn. Neuropsychol. 23, 563–582. doi: 10.1080/02643290500180282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., and Garthwaite, P. H. (2012). Single-case research in neuropsychology: a comparison of five forms of t-test for comparing a case to controls. Cortex 48, 1009–1016. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farb, N. A. S., Grady, C. L., Strother, S., Tang-Wai, D. F., Masellis, M., Black, S., et al. (2013). Abnormal network connectivity in frontotemporal dementia: evidence for prefrontal isolation. Cortex 49, 1856–1873. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrer, C., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Frith, C. D., Decety, J., and Jeannerod, M. (2003). Modulating the experience of agency: a positron emission tomography study. Neuroimage 18, 324–333. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00041-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrer, C., and Frith, C. D. (2002). Experiencing oneself vs. another person as being the cause of an action: the neural correlates of the experience of agency. Neuroimage 15, 596–603. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franz, L. (2012). The allocation of attention to learning of goal-directed actions: a cognitive neuroscience framework focusing on the basal ganglia. Front. Psychol. 3:535. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. J., and Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 355, 1771–1788. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2000.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guye, M., Bettus, G., Bartolomei, F., and Cozzone, P. J. (2010). Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional connectivity MRI in normal and pathological brain networks. MAGMA 23, 409–421. doi: 10.1007/s10334-010-0205-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ibrahim, G. M., Morgan, B. R., Lee, W., Smith, M. L., Donner, E. J., Wang, F., et al. (2014). Impaired development of intrinsic connectivity networks in children with medically intractable localization-related epilepsy. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 5686–5700. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakab, A., Kasprian, G., Schwartz, E., Gruber, G. M., Mitter, C., Prayer, D., et al. (2015). Disrupted developmental organization of the structural connectome in fetuses with corpus callosum agenesis. Neuroimage 111, 277–288. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, J. M., Vaishnavi, S. N., Smyth, M. D., Zhang, D., He, B. J., Zempel, J. M., et al. (2008). Loss of resting interhemispheric functional connectivity after complete section of the corpus callosum. J. Neurosci. 28, 6453–6458. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0573-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasprian, G., Brugger, P. C., Schöpf, V., Mitter, C., Weber, M., Hainfellner, J. A., et al. (2013). Assessing prenatal white matter connectivity in commissural agenesis. Brain 136, 168–179. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Q., Cao, W., Liao, X., Chen, Z., Yang, T., Gong, Q., et al. (2015). Altered resting state functional network connectivity in children absence epilepsy. J. Neurol. Sci. 354, 79–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2015.04.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, H., Stufflebeam, S. M., Sepulcre, J., Hedden, T., and Buckner, R. L. (2009). Evidence from intrinsic activity that asymmetry of the human brain is controlled by multiple factors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 20499–20503. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908073106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, C., An, D., Yao, D., and Gotman, J. (2014). Patient-specific connectivity pattern of epileptic network in frontal lobe epilepsy. Neuroimage Clin. 4, 668–675. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onias, H., Viol, A., Palhano-Fontes, F., Andrade, K. C., Sturzbecher, M., Viswanathan, G., et al. (2014). Brain complex network analysis by means of resting state fMRI and graph analysis: will it be helpful in clinical epilepsy? Epilepsy Behav. 38, 71–80. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owen, J. P., Li, Y.-O., Ziv, E., Strominger, Z., Gold, J., Bukhpun, P., et al. (2013). The structural connectome of the human brain in agenesis of the corpus callosum. Neuroimage 70, 340–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, L. K., Brown, W. S., Adolphs, R., Tyszka, J. M., Richards, L. J., Mukherjee, P., et al. (2007). Agenesis of the corpus callosum: genetic, developmental and functional aspects of connectivity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 287–299. doi: 10.1038/nrn2107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridley, B. G. Y., Rousseau, C., Wirsich, J., Le Troter, A., Soulier, E., Confort-Gouny, S., et al. (2015). Nodal approach reveals differential impact of lateralized focal epilepsies on hub reorganization. Neuroimage 118, 39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubinov, M., and Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage 52, 1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scepkowski, L. A., and Cronin-Golomb, A. (2003). The alien hand: cases, categorizations and anatomical correlates. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 2, 261–277. doi: 10.1177/1534582303260119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer, M., Denke, C., Apostolova, I., Heinze, H.-J., and Galazky, I. (2016). A case of right Alien hand syndrome coexisting with right-sided tactile extinction. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:105. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer, M., Heinze, H.-J., and Galazky, I. (2010). Alien hand syndrome: neural correlates of movements without conscious will. PLoS One 5:e15010. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., Miller, K. L., Glahn, D. C., Fox, P. M., Mackay, C. E., et al. (2009). Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 13040–13045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperduti, M., Delaveau, P., Fossati, P., and Nadel, J. (2011). Different brain structures related to self- and external-agency attribution: a brief review and meta-analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 216, 151–157. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0298-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperry, R. W. (1968a). Hemisphere deconnection and unity in conscious awareness. Am. Psychol. 23, 723–733. doi: 10.1037/h0026839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperry, R. W. (1968b). Plasticity of neural maturation. Dev. Biol. 2, 306–327.

Google Scholar

Sporns, O. (2014). Contributions and challenges for network models in cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 652–660. doi: 10.1038/nn.3690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tagliazucchi, E., and Laufs, H. (2014). Decoding wakefulness levels from typical fMRI resting-state data reveals reliable drifts between wakefulness and sleep. Neuron 82, 695–708. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, M., and David, A. S. (1998). Agenesis of the corpus callosum: a United Kingdom series of 56 cases. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 64, 131–134. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.64.1.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tian, L., Wang, J., Yan, C., and He, Y. (2011). Hemisphere- and gender-related differences in small-world brain networks: a resting-state functional MRI study. Neuroimage 54, 191–202. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasch, J. (1954). Size, distribution and number of fibres in the human corpus callosum. Anat. Rec. 119, 119–135. doi: 10.1002/ar.1091190109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tovar-Moll, F., Monteiro, M., Andrade, J., Bramati, I. E., Vianna-Barbosa, R., Marins, T., et al. (2014). Structural and functional brain rewiring clarifies preserved interhemispheric transfer in humans born without the corpus callosum. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 111, 7843–7848. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1400806111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyszka, J. M., Kennedy, D. P., Adolphs, R., and Paul, L. K. (2011). Intact bilateral resting-state networks in the absence of the corpus callosum. J. Neurosci. 31, 15154–15162. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1453-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verleger, R., Binkofski, F., Friedrich, M., Sedlmeier, P., and Kömpf, D. (2011). Anarchic-hand syndrome: ERP reflections of lost control over the right hemisphere. Brain Cogn. 77, 138–150. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlooswijk, M. C. G., Vaessen, M. J., Jansen, J. F. A., de Krom, M. C. F. T. M., Majoie, H. J. M., Hofman, P. A. M., et al. (2011). Loss of network efficiency associated with cognitive decline in chronic epilepsy. Neurology 77, 938–944. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31822cfc2f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, H. L., An, J., Zeng, L. L., Shen, H., Qiu, S. J., and Hu, D. W. (2015). Altered functional connectivity among default, attention and control networks in idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 46, 118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.03.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheaton, L. A., and Hallett, M. (2007). Ideomotor apraxia: a review. J. Neurol. Sci. 260, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2007.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winter, T. J., and Franz, E. A. (2014). Implication of the anterior commissure in the allocation of attention to action. Front. Psychol. 5:432. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpe, N., Moore, J. W., Rae, C. L., Rittman, T., Altena, E., Haggard, P., et al. (2014). The medial frontal-prefrontal network for altered awareness and control of action in corticobasal syndrome. Brain 137, 208–220. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xia, M., Wang, J., and He, Y. (2013). BrainNet viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS One 8:e68910. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, J., and Seeley, W. W. (2014). Network dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia: implications for psychiatry. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 565–573. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: alien hand, callosal agenesis, disconnection syndrome, graph theory, resting-state, functional connectivity, epilepsy

Citation: Ridley B, Beltramone M, Wirsich J, Le Troter A, Tramoni E, Aubert S, Achard S, Ranjeva J-P, Guye M and Felician O (2016) Alien Hand, Restless Brain: Salience Network and Interhemispheric Connectivity Disruption Parallel Emergence and Extinction of Diagonistic Dyspraxia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:307. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00307

Received: 28 April 2016; Accepted: 06 June 2016;
Published: 20 June 2016.

Edited by:

Rachael D. Seidler, University of Michigan, USA

Reviewed by:

Márk Molnár, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary
Kathy Ruddy, ETH Zurich, Switzerland

Copyright © 2016 Ridley, Beltramone, Wirsich, Le Troter, Tramoni, Aubert, Achard, Ranjeva, Guye and Felician. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ben Ridley, ben.ridley@univ-amu.fr

Download