Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 26 November 2014
Sec. Learning and Memory
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00408

Medial prefrontal cortex dopamine controls the persistent storage of aversive memories

  • 1Laboratorio de Memoria, Facultad de Medicina, Instituto de Biología Celular y Neurociencia, Universidad de Buenos Aires-CONICET, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 2Grupo de Neurociencias de Sistemas, IFIBIO Houssay, Departamento de Fisiología, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 3Memory Research Laboratory, Brain Institute, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte (UFRN), Natal, Brazil
  • 4Departamento de Fisiología, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina

Medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is essential for initial memory processing and expression but its involvement in persistent memory storage has seldom been studied. Using the hippocampus dependent inhibitory avoidance learning task and the hippocampus-independent conditioned taste aversion paradigm together with specific dopamine receptor agonists and antagonists we found that persistence but not formation of long-term aversive memories requires dopamine D1/D5 receptors activation in mPFC immediately after training and, depending on the task, between 6 and 12 h later. Our results indicate that besides its well-known participation in retrieval and early consolidation, mPFC also modulates the endurance of long-lasting aversive memories regardless of whether formation of the aversive mnemonic trace requires the participation of the hippocampus.

Introduction

Medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) plays a critical role in remote memory retrieval as well as in the consolidation and recall of recent memories (Runyan et al., 2004; Frankland and Bontempi, 2005; Gonzalez et al., 2013). mPFC dopamine (DA) signaling has been involved in cognitive, emotional and motivational processes (Seamans et al., 1998; Pezze et al., 2003; Laviolette et al., 2005; Lauzon et al., 2009). The ventral tegmental area (VTA) is the primary source of DA afferents to the mPFC (Lammel et al., 2012). These projections are activated by aversive stimuli (Lammel et al., 2012) and it has been shown that different kinds of aversive experiences increase DA levels within this cortex (Abercrombie et al., 1989; Horvitz, 2000; Bassareo et al., 2002), suggesting that DA signaling in mPFC may play a critical role in the lasting storage of memories for fearful or noxious stimuli. Previously, we showed that maintenance of fear memory is modulated by the VTA through the late posttraining activation of DA D1/D5 receptors in the dorsal hippocampus (Rossato et al., 2009). This activation triggers a late consolidation phase that requires gene expression and protein synthesis, and selectively promotes persistence, but does not affect formation of hippocampus-dependent aversive memories (Bekinschtein et al., 2007; Rossato et al., 2009). However, little is known about the involvement of mPFC in late memory processing and even less regarding its participation in memory storage. We found that activation of D1/D5 receptors in mPFC immediately and late after learning is critical for maintenance of the long-term memory (LTM) trace induced by both hippocampus-dependent and hippocampus-independent aversion-motivated learning tasks, indicating that mPFC regulates memory durability regardless of the aversive properties of the stored information.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Experiments were conducted in male Wistar rats (UBA, Argentina and UFRN, Brazil) weighting 220–250 g. Animals were housed five to a cage and kept at a constant temperature of 23°C, with water and food ad libitum, under a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 a.m.). Experimental procedures were performed in accordance with the USA National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committees of the University of Buenos Aires (CICUAL).

Surgery

Animals were anesthetized with a mix of ketamine (85 mg/kg) and xylazine (10 mg/kg), and placed on a stereotaxic frame. The skull was exposed and leveled (flat skull, lambda and bregma at the same elevation). 22-G guide cannulas were bilaterally implanted, aimed to the mPFC: AP +3.20 mm/LL ±0.75 mm/DV −3.20 mm or the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus: AP −3.90 mm/LL ±3.00 mm/DV −3.00 mm (from Bregma; Paxinos and Watson, 1997) (Figure S1). Cannulas were fixed to the skull with dental acrylic. Immediately after surgery, animals were injected with a single dose of meloxicam (0.2 mg/kg) as analgesic. Animals were allowed to recover from surgery for 5–7 days before any experimental manipulation.

Inhibitory Avoidance Task

After recovery from surgery, animals were handled once a day for 2 days and then trained in the inhibitory avoidance task (IA) as described previously (Bekinschtein et al., 2007). Briefly the apparatus was a 50 × 25 × 25 cm acrylic box whose floor was a grid made of 1 mm-caliber steel bars. The left end of the grid was covered with a 7 cm-wide, 5 cm-high wooden platform. For training, animals were placed on the platform and as they stepped down onto the grid received a single 3-s, 0.8 mA scrambled footshock (strong training) or a 3-s, 0.4 mA scrambled footshock (weak training). Rats were tested for retention 2, 7, or 14 days after training, depending on the experiment. All animals were tested only once. In the test sessions the footshock was omitted. The time spent on the platform during this session was taken as an indicator of retention.

Conditioned Taste Aversion Task

After recovery from surgery, animals were trained in the conditioned taste aversion (CTA) task as described previously (Ballarini et al., 2009). Briefly, animals were deprived of water for 24 h and then habituated to drink water from a graduated tube for 20 min each day for 3 days. In the training session, water was substituted with a 0.1% saccharin (Sigma-Aldrich) solution and 30 min later the animals were injected i.p. with 0.4 M LiCl (Sigma-Aldrich, 7.5 ml/kg) that produces a CTA memory lasting for at least 20 days. During the tests session, water was again replaced by a 0.1% saccharin solution. Training and test sessions lasted 20 min. Rats were tested for retention 3 days (early LTM) or 20 days (remote LTM) after training. All animals were tested only once. Saccharin consumption was calculated as follow: consumption in the test session × 100/consumption in the training session.

Drug Administration

Infusions were delivered through an injector cannula extending 1 mm beyond the tip of the guide cannula. The volume infused was 1 μl per side and the infusion rate was 0.5 μl/min. The injector was left in place for an additional minute after infusion to allow diffusion and to prevent reflux. Doses were as follow: emetine 50 μg/μl, muscimol 0.1 μg/μl, SCH23390 hydrochloride 1.5 μg/μl or SKF38393 hydrochloride 12.5 μg/μl (Sigma Aldrich, St Louis, MO). Drugs were dissolved in sterile 0.9% saline, except for SKF38393 hydrochloride which was dissolved in 10% DMSO. The doses utilized were determined based on previous studies showing the effect of each compound on learning or behavioral performance (Majchrzak and Di Scala, 2000; Lima et al., 2009; Kramar et al., 2014).

Histology

Cannula placement was verified as previously described (Tomaiuolo et al., 2014). Animals were killed by decapitation at the end of the experiment, brains were fixed in 4% PFA for 2 days and sliced in 100 μm coronal sections to corroborate the injection site (Figure S1). Rats found to have misplaced guide cannula were excluded from behavioral analysis.

Data Analysis

Data were analyzed by unpaired Student's t-test or One-Way ANOVA followed by Newman–Keuls multiple comparison tests, as appropriate. IA data are expressed as mean ± SEM of training or test session step-down latency. CTA data are expressed as mean percentage ± SEM relative to the training session.

Results

IA LTM duration can be adjusted by modifying the footshock strength at the moment of training. While a weak footshock (0.4 mA) induces a short-lasting IA LTM enduring no more than 2–3 days, a strong footshock (0.8 mA) produces a persistent aversive memory trace lasting over 14 days [Figure 1A; F(6, 110) = 14.19, p < 0.0001; TR vs. 0.4 mA 2-days test, **p < 0. 01; TR vs. 0.8 mA 2-, 7-, or 14-days test, **p < 0.001; n = 10–20 rats per group]. Given that VTA dopamine neurons signal aversion, saliency and novelty (Lammel et al., 2008, 2011; Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010) and a subset of VTA DA neurons projecting to mPFC are activated by aversive stimuli (Abercrombie et al., 1989; Bassareo et al., 2002; Brischoux et al., 2009; Lammel et al., 2011, 2012), we investigated whether D1/D5 DA receptors in mPFC are involved in IA LTM processing. We performed intra-mPFC infusions of the D1/D5 receptor antagonist SCH23390 (SCH) immediately after strong IA training. SCH impaired retention when memory was assessed 7 days after training, without affecting the 2-day memory expression (Figure 1B, **p < 0.01, n = 11–12). Conversely, intra-mPFC infusion of the D1/D5 receptor agonist SKF38393 (SKF) immediately after weak IA training enhanced memory retention 7 days, but not 2 days posttraining (Figure 1C, *p < 0.05, n = 12–13). These results indicate that D1/D5 DA receptor signaling in the mPFC is required around training to establish a persistent LTM. Then, we examined the effect of blocking mPFC D1/D5 DA receptors late after IA training on memory maintenance. Intra-mPFC administration of SCH 12 h after strong IA training impaired memory retention 7 days, but not 2 days later (Figure 2A, **p < 0.01, n = 7–8). Conversely, SKF specifically increased IA LTM persistence (Figure 2B, **p < 0.01, n = 7–8). These results indicate that D1/D5 receptors in the mPFC are required late after training for persistent IA LTM, but not for IA LTM formation. D1/D5 receptors modulate the late protein synthesis-dependent phase of LTP in the hippocampus (Huang and Kandel, 1995; Navakkode et al., 2007). We found that bilateral intra-mPFC infusion of the protein synthesis inhibitor emetine (EME) 12 h after training impaired IA LTM 7 days after training. No effect on retention was observed when LTM was tested 2 days posttraining (Figure 2C, *p < 0.05, n = 10–16). Therefore, protein synthesis in the mPFC is required late after training to maintain IA LTM persistence. Late posttraining neural activity in the mPFC is also necessary for the persistence of IA LTM storage since bilateral infusions of the GABAA receptor agonist muscimol (MUS) in this cortex impaired memory retention when tested 7 days, but not 2 days after training (Figure 2D, *p < 0.05, n = 6–12). Previously, we demonstrated that persistent LTM depends on late but not early posttraining activation of hippocampal D1/D5 receptors regulated by the VTA (Rossato et al., 2009). After establishing that normal functionality of mPFC dopamine signaling at the moment of training controls the duration of IA LTM, we next investigated the possible interplay between mPFC and the hippocampus to maintain IA memory storage. We found that intra-CA1 infusion of SKF 12 h after strong IA training reversed the amnesic effect of the immediate posttraining intra-mPFC administration of SCH, suggesting that there is a functional link between the early posttraining activation of D1/D5 DA receptors in mPFC and the DA-dependent late consolidation phase in the hippocampus [Figure 3, F(3, 35) = 4.113, p = 0.0134; post-hoc comparisons: VEH/VEH vs. SCH/VEH, *p < 0.05; SCH/VEH vs. SCH/SKF, *p < 0.05; SCH/VEH vs. VEH/SKF, *p < 0.05; VEH/VEH vs. SCH/SKF, VEH/SKF vs. SCH/SKF, or VEH/VEH vs. VEH/SKF, ns; n = 8–11 rats per group]. CTA is a rapid and robust model for aversive memory in which rats acquire aversion to a novel taste when this taste is associated with a digestive malaise (Rosenblum et al., 1993; Bermúdez-Rattoni et al., 2004). This learning task requires the functional participation of the insular cortex and the amygdala but not of the hippocampus (Rosenblum et al., 1993; Guzmán-Ramos et al., 2010). CTA memory becomes sensible to protein synthesis inhibition in the insular cortex soon after training and again 5–7 h later (Rosenblum et al., 1993; Berman and Dudai, 2001; Moguel-González et al., 2008; Martínez-Moreno et al., 2011), suggesting that early and late consolidation phases occur in the insular cortex to form CTA LTM. To assess whether mPFC dopaminergic activity is involved in the persistent storage of hippocampus-independent aversive memories, we studied the effect of mPFC D1/D5 receptors blockade on CTA memory maintenance. Intra-mPFC infusion of SCH immediately after training did not affect CTA memory evaluated 3 days later (Figure 4A, n = 12–14), but impaired retention when CTA memory was evaluated 20 days posttraining (Figure 4B, **p < 0.01, n = 12–13). Similarly, intra-mPFC infusion of SCH 6 h after training did not affect CTA memory evaluated 3 days later (Figure 4C, n = 8), but impaired retention when CTA memory was evaluated 20 days posttraining (Figure 4D, *p < 0.05, n = 8–10). Therefore, normal maintenance of long-lasting CTA memory requires an early and a late phase of DA signaling in the mPFC. No differences among SCH or vehicle groups were found for baseline water intake or saccharin consumption during CTA training (Table S1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. D1/D5 receptors activity in mPFC immediately after training determines LTM persistence. (A) Animals were trained in IA using a weak (0.4 mA) or a strong (0.8 mA) footshock as unconditioned stimulus. Memory retention was evaluated 2, 7, or 14 days posttraining. (B) Animals were trained in IA using a strong footshock and immediately after that received bilateral intra-mPFC infusions of vehicle (VEH) or SCH23390 (SCH). (C) Animals were trained in IA using a weak footshock and immediately after that received bilateral intra-mPFC infusions of VEH or SKF38393 (SKF). Memory retention was evaluated 2 or 7 days after training. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001; TR, training.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. D1/D5 receptors activity, protein synthesis and neural activity in mPFC 12 h after training determine LTM persistence. (A) Animals were trained in IA with a strong footshock and 12 h later received bilateral intra-mPFC infusions of vehicle (VEH) or SCH23390 (SCH). (B) Animals were trained in IA with a weak footshock and 12 h later received bilateral intra-mPFC infusions of VEH or SKF38393 (SKF). (C) Animals were trained in IA with a strong footshock and 12 h later received bilateral intra-mPFC infusions of VEH or emetine (EME). (D) Animals were trained in IA with a strong footshock and 12 h later received bilateral intra-mPFC infusions of VEH or muscimol (MUS). Memory retention was evaluated 2 or 7 days after training. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01; TR, training.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Late posttraining activation of hippocampal D1/D5 receptors rescues the memory deficit caused by early blockade of D1/D5 receptors in mPFC. Animals were trained in IA with a strong footshock and immediately after that received bilateral intra-mPFC infusions of vehicle (VEH) or SCH23390 (SCH) plus bilateral intra-CA1 hippocampal infusions of VEH or SKF38393 (SKF) 12 h later. Memory retention was evaluated 7 days after training. *p < 0.05; TR, training.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. D1/D5 receptors activity in mPFC early and late after training determines CTA LTM persistence. Animals were trained in CTA and immediately posttraining received bilateral intra-mPFC infusions of vehicle (VEH) or SCH23390 (SCH). Memory retention was evaluated 3 days (A) or 20 days (B) after training. Animals were trained in CTA and 6 h after that received bilateral intra-mPFC infusions of VEH or SCH. Memory retention was evaluated 3 days (C) or 20 days (D) after training. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. TR, training.

Discussion

The main finding of the present study is that mPFC D1/D5 receptors regulate the long-lasting storage of hippocampus-dependent and hippocampus-independent aversive memories. We demonstrated that early posttraining D1/D5 receptor activity in mPFC is necessary and sufficient for memory persistence but not for memory formation (Figures 1B,C). This is consistent with results showing that mPFC D1/D5 receptors are not involved in encoding an olfactory fear conditioning memory (Lauzon et al., 2009). In contrast to our findings, Runyan and Dash described that intra-mPFC administration of SCH before fear conditioning impairs LTM retention (Runyan and Dash, 2004). However, more recent findings suggest that learning reactive fear responses (contextual fear conditioning) engages different brain circuitry than learning active fear responses (inhibitory avoidance task) (Yang and Liang, 2014), suggesting that memory processing could differ between tasks. Also, distinct experimental procedures such as time of drug administration (before vs. after training) or intrinsic characteristics of the tasks (multi-trial vs. single trial) could explain the differences observed. Recently, we demonstrated that protein synthesis in mPFC around training is necessary for IA LTM consolidation (Gonzalez et al., 2013). This finding impedes the analysis of the effects of early posttraining inhibition of protein synthesis in the mPFC on memory persistence. The late consolidation phase involved in memory persistence requires activation of D1/D5 receptors, neural activity and protein synthesis in mPFC late after training (Figure 2), similarly to what happens in the hippocampus (Bekinschtein et al., 2007; Rossato et al., 2009). Based on recent findings (Lima et al., 2013) and those in Figure 3, we suggest that dopaminergic inputs to the hippocampus are a final common point for neocortical influences on IA memory persistence driven by VTA-mPFC connections at the time of training and 12 h thereafter. The acute mPFC manipulations described here specifically affect memory persistence without altering memory formation, indicating that this cortex is part of the circuitry involved in the maintenance of the information over time together with the hippocampus and the VTA, as previously described by our groups (Bekinschtein et al., 2007; Rossato et al., 2009) and confirmed by others (Parfitt et al., 2011, 2012; Werenicz et al., 2012).

The role of the mPFC in memory processing has attracted much attention in recent years. Based on anatomical and behavioral data it has been postulated that the rodent prelimbic cortex and the ventral anterior cingulated cortex are homologous to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in primates (Vertes, 2006; Kesner and Churchwell, 2011). From pioneering studies on working memory (Goldman and Rosvold, 1970; Brito and Brito, 1990; Kesner and Churchwell, 2011) to studies on its role in recent and remote memories (Runyan et al., 2004; Frankland and Bontempi, 2005; Zhang et al., 2011), the mPFC appears to fulfill a wide range of mnemonic functions (Euston et al., 2012). Our findings add a new and important function to this list: the mPFC is not only relevant for the active maintenance of information during seconds (working memory), but it is also essential for storing aversive memory traces. Without the activity of the VTA (Rossato et al., 2009) or the dopaminergic activity in the mPFC at the time of training, no long-lasting LTM can be established and only a non-persistent memory lasting a couple of days is formed. It is known that a subpopulation of VTA neurons projecting to mPFC are excited by both rewarding and aversive events, conveying information regarding the motivational salience of the experience, and/or alerting signals triggered by unexpected sensory cues (Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010; Lammel et al., 2011, 2012). How this information is integrated by mPFC neurons to influence the persistence of memory storage is not known. mPFC neurons display periods of burst firing in response to salient stimuli (Burgos-Robles et al., 2007) and burst-like activation of mPFC neurons induces a massive increase in DA neuron activity (Lodge, 2011). Several anatomical and electrophysiological studies demonstrate that mPFC regulates VTA dopaminergic activity by innervating those DA neurons that project back to the mPFC (Overton et al., 1996; Carr and Sesack, 2000; Aston-Jones et al., 2009; Lodge, 2011). In this context, it is interesting to note that intra-VTA infusion of the NMDA receptor antagonist AP5 immediately after IA training elicits a selective impairment of memory persistence without affecting memory formation (Rossato et al., 2009).

We suggest that persistence of fear memory storage depends on functional interactions between the mPFC and VTA. The early activation of mPFC dopaminergic connections, via D1/D5 receptors, is critical for maintenance of the memory trace, while mPFC and hippocampus dopaminergic VTA projections are required late after training for memory persistence. Thus, activation of the VTA-hippocampus dopamine loop (Rossato et al., 2009) seems to be a final common pathway for influences coming from different cortical and subcortical regions (Figure 3 and Lima et al., 2013) to support a late consolidation phase of hippocampus-dependent aversive memories. Also, the effects of early posttraining manipulation of mPFC dopaminergic signaling indicate that this circuit is recruited before the hippocampus in the process to support memory persistence. Besides, we demonstrated that dopamine signaling in mPFC controls the persistence of an aversive no-fear related memory. Long-lasting storage of CTA LTM requires early and late posttraining activation of D1/D5 receptors in mPFC. Supporting the participation of this cortex in CTA memory processing, a recent work has shown that prelimbic activity synchronized with the insular cortex and the amygdala is necessary for CTA learning (Uematsu et al., 2014). It would be interesting to conduct further experiments to study a putative interplay between the mPFC DA signaling and the late consolidation phase described in the insular cortex (Martínez-Moreno et al., 2011). It could be possible that in this case, the insular cortex is the final common pathway to support the persistent storage of this hippocampus-independent aversive memory.

In conclusion, the present results show that early and late posttraining D1/D5 dopaminergic neurotransmission in the mPFC plays a key role in the persistent storage of different types of aversive memories. This is independent of the dynamics of CTA and IA memory consolidation and of the participation or lack thereof of the hippocampus in this process. Since catecholamine deregulation in the prefrontal cortex has been related to many psychiatric disorders (Arnsten, 2007; Gamo and Arnsten, 2011), we propose that disturbances in the D1-like receptor circuitry may be underlying the abnormal persistent storage of aversive memories, such as that observed in phobias or PTSD. Further research is needed to fully understand this phenomenon and use it as a potential therapeutic target to control the aberrant overexpression of aversive memories.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Fabricio Ballarini, Fernando Garagoli, and Guido Dorman for technical help. This study was supported by grants from Agencia Nacional de Promoción Científica y Tecnológica (ANPCyT, Argentina) to Jorge H. Medina; Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq, Brazil) and Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio Grande do Norte (FAPERN, Brazil) to Martín Cammarota; and International Brain Research Organization (IBRO) to Jorge H. Medina and Martín Cammarota.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00408/abstract

Figure S1 | Histological analysis of cannula placement. Top: Representative microphotograph showing the position of guide cannulas in mPFC (A) and the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus (B). Scale bar 1 mm. Bottom: Schematic illustration of the injection sites in the intended areas: mPFC, rat brain section at rostrocaudal plane +3.20 (A) and CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus, rat brain section at rostrocaudal plane −3.80 (B) from Bregma taken from the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (1997).

References

Abercrombie, E. D., Keefe, K. A., DiFrischia, D. S., and Zigmond, M. J. (1989). Differential effect of stress on in vivo dopamine release in striatum, nucleus accumbens, and medial frontal cortex. J. Neurochem. 52, 1655–1658. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1989.tb09224.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnsten, A. F. (2007). Catecholamine and second messenger influences on prefrontal cortical networks of “representational knowledge”: a rational bridge between genetics and the symptoms of mental illness. Cereb. Cortex 17(Suppl. 1), i6–i15. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm033

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aston-Jones, G., Smith, R. J., Moorman, D. E., and Richardson, K. A. (2009). Role of lateral hypothalamic orexin neurons in reward processing and addiction. Neuropharmacology 56(Suppl. 1), 112–121. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.060

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballarini, F., Moncada, D., Martinez, M. C., Alen, N., and Viola, H. (2009). Behavioral tagging is a general mechanism of long-term memory formation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 14599–14604. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907078106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassareo, V., De Luca, M. A., and Di Chiara, G. (2002). Differential expression of motivational stimulus properties by dopamine in nucleus accumbens shell versus core and prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 22, 4709–4719.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Bekinschtein, P., Cammarota, M., Igaz, L. M., Bevilaqua, L. R., Izquierdo, I., and Medina, J. H. (2007). Persistence of long-term memory storage requires a late protein synthesis- and BDNF- dependent phase in the hippocampus. Neuron 53, 261–277. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, D. E., and Dudai, Y. (2001). Memory extinction, learning anew, and learning the new: dissociations in the molecular machinery of learning in cortex. Science 291, 2417–2419. doi: 10.1126/science.1058165

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bermúdez-Rattoni, F., Ramírez-Lugo, L., Gutiérrez, R., and Miranda, M. I. (2004). Molecular signals into the insular cortex and amygdala during aversive gustatory memory formation. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 24, 25–36. doi: 10.1023/B:CEMN.0000012722.45805.c8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brischoux, F., Chakraborty, S., Brierley, D. I., and Ungless, M. A. (2009). Phasic excitation of dopamine neurons in ventral VTA by noxious stimuli. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 4894–4899. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811507106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brito, L. S., and Brito, G. N. (1990). Locomotor activity and one-way active avoidance after intrahippocampal injection of neurotransmitter antagonists. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 23, 1015–1019.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Bromberg-Martin, E. S., Matsumoto, M., and Hikosaka, O. (2010). Dopamine in motivational control: rewarding, aversive, and alerting. Neuron 68, 815–834. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgos-Robles, A., Vidal-Gonzalez, I., Santini, E., and Quirk, G. J. (2007). Consolidation of fear extinction requires NMDA receptor-dependent bursting in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron 53, 871–880. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, D. B., and Sesack, S. R. (2000). Projections from the rat prefrontal cortex to the ventral tegmental area: target specificity in the synaptic associations with mesoaccumbens and mesocortical neurons. J. Neurosci. 20, 3864–3873.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Euston, D. R., Gruber, A. J., and McNaughton, B. L. (2012). The role of medial prefrontal cortex in memory and decision making. Neuron 76, 1057–1070. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankland, P. W., and Bontempi, B. (2005). The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 119–130. doi: 10.1038/nrn1607

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gamo, N. J., and Arnsten, A. F. (2011). Molecular modulation of prefrontal cortex: rational development of treatments for psychiatric disorders. Behav. Neurosci. 125, 282–296. doi: 10.1037/a0023165

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman, P. S., and Rosvold, H. E. (1970). Localization of function within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the rhesus monkey. Exp. Neurol. 27, 291–304. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(70)90222-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez, C., Kramar, C., Garagoli, F., Rossato, J. I., Weisstaub, N., Cammarota, M., et al. (2013). Medial prefrontal cortex is a crucial node of a rapid learning system that retrieves recent and remote memories. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 103, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.04.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guzmán-Ramos, K., Osorio-Gómz, D., Moreno-Castilla, P., and Bermúdez-Rattoni, F. (2010). Off-line concomitant release of dopamine and glutamate involvement in taste memory consolidation. J. Neurochem. 114, 226–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06758.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horvitz, J. C. (2000). Mesolimbocortical and nigrostriatal dopamine responses to salient non-reward events. Neuroscience 96, 651–656. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00019-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y. Y., and Kandel, E. R. (1995). D1/D5 receptor agonists induce a protein synthesis-dependent late potentiation in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 2446–2450. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.7.2446

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kesner, R. P., and Churchwell, J. C. (2011). An analysis of rat prefrontal cortex in mediating executive function. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 417–431. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.07.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramar, C. P., Chefer, V. I., Wise, R. A., Medina, J. H., and Barbano, M. F. (2014). Dopamine in the dorsal hippocampus impairs the late consolidation of cocaine-associated memory. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 1645–1653. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.11

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lammel, S., Hetzel, A., Häckel, O., Jones, I., Liss, B., and Roeper, J. (2008). Unique properties of mesoprefrontal neurons within a dual mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Neuron 57, 760–773. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lammel, S., Ion, D. I., Roeper, J., and Malenka, R. C. (2011). Projection-specific modulation of dopamine neuron synapses by aversive and rewarding stimuli. Neuron 70, 855–862. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lammel, S., Lim, B. K., Ran, C., Huang, K. W., Betley, M. J., Tye, K. M., et al. (2012). Input-specific control of reward and aversion in the ventral tegmental area. Nature 491, 212–217. doi: 10.1038/nature11527

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauzon, N. M., Bishop, S. F., and Laviolette, S. R. (2009). Dopamine D1 versus D4 receptors differentially modulate the encoding of salient versus nonsalient emotional information in the medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 29, 4836–4845. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0178-09.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laviolette, S. R., Lipski, W. J., and Grace, A. A. (2005). A subpopulation of neurons in the medial prefrontal cortex encodes emotional learning with burst and frequency codes through a dopamine D4 receptor-dependent basolateral amygdale input. J. Neurosci. 25, 6066–6075. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1168-05.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lima, R. H., Radiske, A., Köhler, C. A., Gonzalez, M. C., Bevilaqua, L. R., Rossato, J. I., et al. (2013). Nicotine modulates the long-lasting storage of fear memory. Learn. Mem. 20, 120–124. doi: 10.1101/lm.029900.112

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lima, R. H., Rossato, J. I., Furini, C. R., Bevilaqua, L. R., Izquierdo, I., and Cammarota, M. (2009). Infusion of protein synthesis inhibitors in the entorhinal cortex blocks consolidation but not reconsolidation of object recognition memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 91, 466–472. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.12.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lodge, D. J. (2011). The medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortices differentially regulate dopamine system function. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 1227–1236. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Majchrzak, M., and Di Scala, G. (2000). GABA and muscimol as reversible inactivation tools in learning and memory. Neural Plast. 7, 19–29. doi: 10.1155/NP.2000.19

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Moreno, A., Rodríguez-Durán, L. F., and Escobar, M. L. (2011). Late protein synthesis-dependent phases in CTA long-term memory: BDNF requirement. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 5:61. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00061

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moguel-González, M., Gómez-Palacio-Schjetnan, A., and Escobar, M. L. (2008). BDNF reverses the CTA memory deficits produced by inhibition of protein synthesis. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 90, 584–587. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.06.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navakkode, S., Sajikumar, S., and Frey, J. U. (2007). Synergistic requirements for the induction of dopaminergic D1/D5-receptor-mediated LTP in hippocampal slices of rat CA1 in vitro. Neuropharmacology 52, 1547–1554. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2007.02.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overton, P. G., Tong, Z. Y., and Clark, D. (1996). A pharmacological analysis of the burst events induced in midbrain dopaminergic neurons by electrical stimulation of the prefrontal cortex in the rat. J. Neural Transm. 103, 523–540. doi: 10.1007/BF01273151

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parfitt, G. M., Barbosa, Â. K., Campos, R. C., Koth, A. P., and Barros, D. M. (2012). Moderate stress enhances memory persistence: are adrenergic mechanisms involved? Behav. Neurosci. 126, 729–734. doi: 10.1037/a0029861

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parfitt, G. M., Campos, R. C., Barbosa, A. K., Koth, A. P., and Barros, D. M. (2011). Participation of hippocampal cholinergic system in memory persistence for inhibitory avoidance in rats. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 97, 183–188. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.12.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paxinos, G., and Watson, C. (1997). The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, 3rd Edn. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Pezze, M. A., Bast, T., and Feldon, J. (2003). Significance of dopamine transmission in the rat medial prefrontal cortex for conditioned fear. Cereb. Cortex 13, 371–380. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.4.371

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenblum, K., Meiri, N., and Dudai, Y. (1993). Taste memory: the role of protein synthesis in gustatory cortex. Behav. Neural Biol. 59, 49–56. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(93)91145-D

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossato, J. I., Bevilaqua, L. R., Izquierdo, I., Medina, J. H., and Cammarota, M. (2009). Dopamine controls persistence of long-term memory storage. Science 325, 1017–1020. doi: 10.1126/science.1172545

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runyan, J. D., and Dash, P. K. (2004). Intra-medial prefrontal administration of SCH-23390 attenuates ERK phosphorylation and long-term memory for trace fear conditioning in rats. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 82, 65–70. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.04.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runyan, J. D., Moore, A. N., and Dash, P. K. (2004). A role for prefrontal cortex in memory storage for trace fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 24, 1288–1295. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4880-03.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seamans, J. K., Floresco, S. B., and Phillips, A. G. (1998). D1 receptor modulation of hippocampal–prefrontal cortical circuits integrating spatial memory with executive functions in the rat. J. Neurosci. 18, 1613–1621.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomaiuolo, M., Gonzalez, C., Medina, J. H., and Piriz, J. (2014). Lateral habenula determines long-term storage of aversive memories. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:170. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00170

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uematsu, A., Kitamura, A., Iwatsuki, K., Uneyama, H., and Tsurugizawa, T. (2014). Correlation between activation of prelimbic cortex, the basolateral amygdala, and agranular insular cortex during taste memory formation. Cereb. Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu069. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vertes, R. P. (2006). Interactions among the medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and midline thalamus in emotional and cognitive processing in the rat. Neuroscience 142, 1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werenicz, A., Christoff, R. R., Blank, M., Jobim, P. F., Pedroso, T. R., Reolon, G. K., et al. (2012). Administration of the phosphodiesterase type 4 inhibitor rolipram into the amygdala at a specific time interval after learning increases recognition memory persistence. Learn. Mem. 19, 495–498. doi: 10.1101/lm.026997.112

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, F. C., and Liang, K. C. (2014). Interactions of the dorsal hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens in formation of fear memory: difference in inhibitory avoidance learning and contextual fear conditioning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 112, 186–194. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.07.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Fukushima, H., and Kida, S. (2011). Induction and requirement of gene expression in the anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex for the consolidation of inhibitory avoidance memory. Mol. Brain 4:4. doi: 10.1186/1756-6606-4-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: inhibitory avoidance, conditioned taste aversion, D1/D5 receptor, VTA, system consolidation

Citation: Gonzalez MC, Kramar CP, Tomaiuolo M, Katche C, Weisstaub N, Cammarota M and Medina JH (2014) Medial prefrontal cortex dopamine controls the persistent storage of aversive memories. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:408. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00408

Received: 24 September 2014; Accepted: 11 November 2014;
Published online: 26 November 2014.

Edited by:

Martin Giurfa, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique - Université Paul Sabatier-Toulouse III, France

Reviewed by:

Federico Bermudez-Rattoni, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Mexico
Jonathan Eric Ploski, University of Texas at Dallas, USA

Copyright © 2014 Gonzalez, Kramar, Tomaiuolo, Katche, Weisstaub, Cammarota and Medina. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jorge H. Medina, Laboratorio de Memoria, Facultad de Medicina, Instituto de Biología Celular y Neurociencia, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Paraguay 2155, 3° Piso, C1121ABG Buenos Aires, Argentina e-mail: jmedina@fmed.uba.ar

Download