Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Gaze Behavior in One-Handed Catching and Its Relation with Interceptive Performance: What the Eyes Can't Tell

  • Benedetta Cesqui ,

    b.cesqui@hsantalucia.it

    Affiliations Laboratory of Neuromotor Physiology, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy, Centre of Space Bio-medicine, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy

  • Maura Mezzetti,

    Affiliation Department of Economics and Finance, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy

  • Francesco Lacquaniti,

    Affiliations Laboratory of Neuromotor Physiology, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy, Centre of Space Bio-medicine, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy, Department of Systems Medicine, Neuroscience Section, University of Rome, Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy

  • Andrea d'Avella

    Affiliations Laboratory of Neuromotor Physiology, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy, Department of Biomedical Sciences and Morphological and Functional Images, University of Messina, Messina, Italy

Abstract

In ball sports, it is usually acknowledged that expert athletes track the ball more accurately than novices. However, there is also evidence that keeping the eyes on the ball is not always necessary for interception. Here we aimed at gaining new insights on the extent to which ocular pursuit performance is related to catching performance. To this end, we analyzed eye and head movements of nine subjects catching a ball projected by an actuated launching apparatus. Four different ball flight durations and two different ball arrival heights were tested and the quality of ocular pursuit was characterized by means of several timing and accuracy parameters. Catching performance differed across subjects and depended on ball flight characteristics. All subjects showed a similar sequence of eye movement events and a similar modulation of the timing of these events in relation to the characteristics of the ball trajectory. On a trial-by-trial basis there was a significant relationship only between pursuit duration and catching performance, confirming that keeping the eyes on the ball longer increases catching success probability. Ocular pursuit parameters values and their dependence on flight conditions as well as the eye and head contributions to gaze shift differed across subjects. However, the observed average individual ocular behavior and the eye-head coordination patterns were not directly related to the individual catching performance. These results suggest that several oculomotor strategies may be used to gather information on ball motion, and that factors unrelated to eye movements may underlie the observed differences in interceptive performance.

Introduction

Vision is a major source of information for fast and accurate interceptive movements like catching or hitting a flying ball. By bringing and keeping the image of a moving target on the fovea, eye movements allow gathering salient information on the ball trajectory from high acuity vision of its spatial landmarks. Such information is often critical for the control of interceptive task [15]. A growing body of studies indicates that the eyes and the hand can be guided by common sources of visual information on the properties of the ball trajectory such as speed, angular velocity, distance, rate of expansion, and, if the ball bounces, as in cricket or tennis, the angle of incidence [58]. Likewise, the same representation of the object motion is thought to underlie the control of both effectors [5, 6, 9]. The picture emerging from these studies is that targeted eye movements often precede hand movements and subserve their control. It is then unsurprising that the quality of vision can influence ball catching performance. For instance, if vision is disrupted by occlusion of parts of the ball trajectory, catching performance deteriorates with increasing duration of the occlusion [1013]. Because of substantial sensorimotor processing delays, tracking fast targets is not always possible [14, 15] and corrective saccades and peripheral vision are often exploited to overcome this limit and pick up relevant information on the ball trajectory [1, 16, 17]. Notice that processing of target information in peripheral vision may be misleading as it alters the perceived speed of the object and can introduce movement biases [18, 19]. In fact, several studies have reported decreasing catching performance with increasing ball speed [2022].

How vision contributes to the control of the interceptive action is debated [7, 8, 23, 24]. One possibility is that visual cues about the temporal and spatial characteristics of the target trajectory and prior knowledge about the target dynamics allow an accurate extrapolation of its motion. Gaze behavior may reflect the ability to anticipate changes in the visual scene and to predict the future position of the target. Another possibility is that motion execution is updated continuously based on a combination of optical cues (see [8] for a review), guiding the hand toward target interception (so called prospective control). According to this second view, eye movements may provide a measure of the quality of the visual information and efference copy used to guide the action [15, 25, 26]. Several studies have shown that target foveation allows a continuous monitoring of target speed and direction [27, 28], as well as other trajectory features [8]. Such information is crucial for the visual control of the hand [7, 27, 29] and for adjusting the initial motor plan on the way toward the catch zone [27, 28].

Irrespective of the specific strategy which is hypothesized to underlie target interception, it is often assumed that eye movements are informative of the accuracy of the control processes (whether predictive or prospective), and thus are related to the subject's skill in the interceptive task [1, 3]. In line with this expectation, several experiments investigating oculomotor strategies in different ball sports, such as cricket [3, 4, 30], baseball [1], juggling [3133], volleyball [34], soccer [16], and tennis [3538], have indeed reported differences in gaze tracking performance across subjects of different expertise. For example, Land and McLeod [3] observed that a key feature of the oculomotor behavior of cricket batsmen is represented by a saccade at the bounce point, which is crucial for the evaluation of the post-bounce trajectory. These authors reported that an expert athlete used the information acquired immediately after ball release to predict the bounce event more accurately and more in advance than an amateur player and a naive player. The expert player was then able to pursue the ball after the bounce longer than the other two players. In another study, professional baseball athletes facing fast ball trajectories kept their eyes on the ball longer and with faster smooth pursuit eye movements than less expert players [1]. Similar differences in gaze behavior between players with different expertise were also observed in volleyball [34].

However, whether accurate gaze tracking of the ball throughout its motion is required to perform successful interceptive movements is still controversial. It is worth noting that the majority of the studies on the relationship between ocular pursuit and interceptive performance have related gaze behavior only to the skill level or the expertise of the subjects, whereas the dependence of hitting/catching success in individual trials on the quality of eye tracking has not been investigated to our knowledge. In fact, there is evidence that ball tracking may vary in duration and quality across subjects of the same skill level [2, 34, 36, 37, 39] or, vice versa, the ability at picking up visual information may not be very different between expert and novice players [36, 4042]. Moreover, a recent study has shown that several cricket batsmen, taken from a population of 13 sub-elite players of the same skill level, barely foveated the ball, and yet succeeded in the task execution [2]. Similarly, it has been shown that interceptive performance is not impaired if visual acuity is degraded by myopic blur [43]. Others studies reported that novice players attempted to view the ball constantly throughout its trajectory, in contrast with expert players [35]. Recently, it has also been observed that the exact targeting of the bounce point in cricket is not mandatory for the extraction of post-bounce ball flight characteristics, if memory from previous trials or expectation of the physical properties of the ball are also exploited [4]. In summary, these contrasting results suggest that the role of accurate ball tracking for successful completion of interceptive tasks needs further investigation.

In the present study, we explored whether keeping the eyes on the ball is crucial for good performance in a one-handed catching task. In particular, we investigated the relationship between the quality of visual tracking and catching success on a trial-by-trial basis. Nine subjects were asked to catch a flying ball projected from a distance of 6 m and arriving at a reachable position without bouncing on the floor. Thus, our task did not require accurate prediction of the bounce point as observed in cricket or tennis [30, 35, 36, 44]. Different ball flight conditions specified in terms of flight duration and ball arrival height were obtained by setting different ball initial velocities with a custom made launching system [45]. This approach allowed testing ball trajectories which involved different levels of catching and tracking difficulty. In fact, the flight conditions with the highest ball speeds were quite challenging for catching and tracking [1, 22, 46]. The quality of ocular pursuit was characterized by means of several timing and accuracy parameters. Moreover, because it has been recently shown that elite cricket batters couple head rotation to ball movement [30], we also investigated the relative role of the eye and head contribution to ball tracking. Finally, because our previous study of a similar catching task had revealed a large inter-individual variability in arm kinematics [20], we wondered whether such variability could be due to differences in the subject-specific ability at gathering and using salient environmental information sources. Here we expanded and complemented this analysis by also examining whether catching performance was related to different individual gaze tracking behaviors.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Nine right handed subjects (6 males and 3 females, labeled S1 to S9), between 22 and 42 years old (30 ± 6, mean ± SD) participated in the study. Two of them were the first and last author of the study (S6 and S8 respectively). All subjects had normal or corrected to normal vision, were informed about the procedure and the aims of the study, which was approved by the Ethical Review Board of the Santa Lucia Foundation, and gave their written informed consent to participate to the experiment.

The task and apparatus

The experimental task was similar to the one reported in a previous study [20]. Briefly, participants were asked to catch a ball projected by means of a dedicated launching apparatus [45] while standing at a distance d = 6 m (Fig. 1 A). Eight different ball flight conditions obtained by the combination of four mean flight durations (T1 = 0.55 s, T2 = 0.65 s, T3 = 0.75s, T4 = 0.85 s) and two mean arrival heights, Z1 (low ball arrival height) and Z2 (high ball arrival height) were tested. In particular, nominal Z1 and Z2 were adjusted according to the shoulder height of the subject (Hsh): Z1 = Hsh-0.3m, Z1 = Hsh + 0.3m.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Experimental apparatus, ball trajectories, and marker placement.

(A) Subjects were standing at a distance of 6 m in front of a screen with a hole through which balls were projected by a launching apparatus positioned behind the screen. (B) Ball trajectories in spatial and in gaze coordinates for the different experimental conditions examined in the present study. The different T-Z condition are shown in different colors. Solid thick lines represent the Z2 conditions, thin lines represents Z1 conditions. Top Panels: horizontal (x) and vertical (z) spatial coordinates over time of the average ball trajectory for each block, and the corresponding azimuth and elevation gaze coordinates (rightmost column); the dashed line in the z(t) plot represents the subject eyes height. Bottom Panels: horizontal (x) and vertical (z) spatial velocities of the average ball trajectory for each block, and the corresponding azimuth versus elevation gaze velocities (right most column). (C) Vicon markers placement on the Eyelink helmet. (D) Vicon markers placement on the subject upper trunk.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g001

The different ball trajectories resulted from a combination of different initial values of the vertical and horizontal velocities and corresponded to different azimuth and elevation angles and angular velocities throughout the flight. Fig. 1B shows elevation angles and angular velocity traces in each flight condition, illustrating the ideal gaze paths of a stationary catcher tracking the approaching ball accurately. In the Z1 conditions, ball elevation angle first increased and then decreased. In the Z2 conditions, instead, as the ball landed behind the subject, elevation angle increased throughout the entire flight. Ball speed ranged within the [914] m/s interval. These ranges have already been tested in similar previous studies [21, 22] and they have been found to be demanding for the perceptual-motor system. Vision of the launcher orientation was impeded by a large big screen in front of the launching machine [45], hence preventing anticipation of the initial ball velocity. Finally, as several sources of random variability affected the ball acceleration and release in our launching apparatus [45], the initial velocity of the ball slightly varied across trials in the same T-Z condition. Thus, in our setup the ball arrival height had a standard deviation of approximately 20 cm [45].

Data acquisition

Prior to the beginning of the experimental session, the launching apparatus was calibrated according to a procedure described in a previous report [45]. During the experiment, the eye pupil position was recorded at 250 Hz using a head-mounted video-based binocular eye tracker (EyeLink II, SR Research, Ltd., Mississauga, Ontario, Canada). The spatial position of several markers placed on the subject’s body and head, and the position of the ball throughout its entire flight were tracked at 100 Hz using a motion capture system (Vicon-612, Vicon Motion Systems Ltd. UK). Head markers were attached on the EyeLink helmet as reported in Fig. 1C: left front head (LFHD, i.e. M3), left back head (LBHD), right front head (RFHD, i.e. M2), and right back head (RBHD, i.e. M1). Markers were also positioned on the skin overlying the epicondylus lateralis (RELB) and on the right forearm (RFRA) as shown in Fig. 1D. The wrist position (RW) was estimated by averaging the position of two additional markers (RWRU, RWRR), placed at the extremity of a stick (21 cm) taped on the subject wrist in correspondence to the mid-point between the ulnar styloid and radial styloid processes. In addition, the coordinates of the center of the launcher hole and the orientation of the screen were also estimated by means of three markers placed on the screen surface (Fig. 1A). Markers coordinates were referred to a right handed calibration frame placed on the floor at 6 m distance from the launch plane (i.e. the world coordinate frame), as shown in Fig. 1A. A consumer-grade PAL mini-DV video camera was used to film the subjects during the entire experimental session.

Experimental protocol

For each flight condition subjects performed one block of 10 trials, for a total of 8 blocks. If the ball accidentally touched the ceiling of the laboratory or the boundaries of the exit hole on the screen, the launch was repeated. The order of the blocks was randomized across subjects. Prior to the beginning of each session, a few launches with different initial conditions were carried out for task familiarization. Each trial started with an auditory cue to alert the subject of a new launch. After a time interval randomly varying between 1 and 5 seconds to avoid auditory anticipation, the experimenter inserted the ball inside the launching machine and the ball was launched.

Gaze orientation in space was estimated by combining the measurement of eye orientation in the head provided by the eye tracker, and the measurement of head position and orientation in space obtained with the Vicon motion capture system. The detailed description of the procedure is reported elsewhere [47]. Prior to the beginning of the launch session, subjects performed a calibration session required for the extraction of the geometrical parameters of the mapping between the pupil image, as measured by the eye tracker, and gaze coordinates in space. Subjects were asked to gaze at a Vicon marker located on the edge of a stick that was slowly moved by the experimenter within the subject’s field of view. The procedure was repeated at the beginning and at the end of the catching experiment. Additionally, to prevent gaze error estimation caused by accidental slippages of the tracker over the subject head, a drift correction procedure was applied [47]. With the exception of S4 and S7, all other subjects performed a drift correction trial at the end of each block, which consisted in fixating a marker located on the bottom border of the exit hole of the launching system (i.e. the REF marker in Fig. 1A). The recorded data were then used to estimate the slippage, and hence to correct the geometrical parameters extracted with the initial calibration of the system. In the case of S4 and S7 the correction was carried out by assuming that the subjects were looking at the exit hole of the launching system when the trial started.

Data analysis

Subject's catching performance was evaluated considering the percentage of successful catches. In particular, each trial was classified as caught if the ball was captured by the hand and non-caught otherwise. Kinematic data from the Vicon markers were digitally low-passed filtered (25 Hz cutoff frequency, zero-lag FIR filter) and differentiated to obtain the first- and second-order derivatives. As in a previous study [20], ball flight events were characterized by means of several parameters. Ball launch time was defined as the instant at which the ball passed through the screen. Impact time was computed as the instant at which the distance between the ball trajectory and the plane passing for the RWRU, RWRR and RFRA markers reached its minimum. Flight duration was defined as the time interval between the launch time and the impact time.

A trial was not included in the analysis if: 1) the subject initiated the movement before the ball was projected in space by the launching system (2.2 ± 1.3, mean ± SD, trials excluded within the entire session, averaged across subjects); 2) the position of the markers on the head was too noisy or was not reconstructed by the Vicon system for too many frames to allow the Vicon software to accurately interpolate its trajectory (10.7 ± 4.6 trials); 3) the ball was not reconstructed (2.7 ± 1.4 trials) because it exceeded the tracking volume of the Vicon system (i.e. about 7.5m×3m×3m in our case).

Gaze, eye, and head coordinates

The calibration parameters were used to extract the gaze-in-space orientation angles from the EyeLink-II and the Vicon head markers recordings. In addition, a diagnostic analysis was carried out for each trial to assess which drift correction trial between those recorded respectively at the beginning and at the end of each block gave the best reconstruction outcomes. This procedure ensured high accuracy and precision in the estimation of gaze angles throughout the entire experiment. In particular, for this experiment the estimation of azimuth and elevation angles had an accuracy of 0.07° on average, always better than 0.39°, and a precision of 0.49° average, and always better than 0.80°.

In the following, we will refer to gaze and eye coordinates as the azimuth (θ) and elevation (φ) angles expressed: a) with respect to the world coordinate frame centered in the eye (i.e. gaze coordinates: θ and φ); b) with respect to a head-fixed coordinate frame system centered in the eye (i.e. eye coordinates: θe and φe) [47]. The head coordinates are the azimuth and elevation angles expressed with respect to the head pose defined while the subject looks straight ahead at a far target at eye height (the primary position) by means of the position of three non-collinear points on the helmet (i.e head coordinates θh and φh).

In brief, eye coordinates express the rotation of the eye inside the orbit, head coordinates express the head position and orientation in space, and gaze coordinates take into account the head position and orientation in space and express the gaze shift with respect to the primary position. In particular:

(1)

The position of the ball, tracked with the Vicon motion capture system, was also converted in gaze coordinates (i.e., θb and φb). These angles represent the ideal gaze orientation that a catcher would have if he/she was perfectly tracking the ball.

Different filter cut-off frequencies were applied to the EyeLink-II data depending on the specific analysis being performed. For the purpose of extracting calibration and correction parameters for the estimation of gaze orientation in space, pupil coordinates recorded with the EyeLink-II system and positions of the target and head markers collected with the Vicon system were digitally low-pass filtered respectively at 25 Hz and 15 Hz cutoff frequency (zero-lag FIR filter) as specified in [47]. For the purpose of the saccade identification and analysis, the EyeLink-II raw data were filtered with a 50 Hz cutoff frequency. The calibration parameters were used to compute gaze in eye coordinates. Data were then differentiated to obtain the first- and second-order derivatives. Saccades were identified, using the left eye coordinates, based on a combination of threshold criteria for acceleration and velocity as specified in [48]. Briefly, points in the gaze acceleration trace exceeding a threshold of 1500°/s2 were first identified, and the complete saccadic trajectory was determined by finding the peaks and troughs of the acceleration within the interval in which the threshold criteria was exceeded. If the use of the acceleration threshold failed to identify a saccade that could be detected by visual inspection, a second pass was made in which a velocity threshold of 30°/s was applied. For each trial we quantified the number of saccades and, for each saccade and for both the azimuth and the elevation angles, several parameters were computed. The saccade amplitude (AS) was defined as the difference between the final and the initial orientation of the eye. The saccade onset or latency time (LTS) corresponded to the time the saccade was initiated (see above) with respect to the time of ball launch. Similar to other studies [49], saccades were divided in two groups: the first saccade was used for the characterization of the initial oculomotor response, while the remaining saccades were used to characterize the pursuit phases [15]. Hereafter, we will refer to the latter group as catch-up saccades. Finally for the purposes of the analysis of gaze tracking features (see below) the EyeLink-II raw data were low pass filtered with a 15Hz cut-off frequency in accordance to [50]. All analyses were performed on the left eye data.

Gaze tracking features

Representative examples of gaze and ball trajectories are reported in Fig. 2 for one subject (S9). Panels A and B show the azimuth and the elevation gaze coordinates of the left eye (eye, red lines) and of the ball (ball, black lines) in the T3 condition, for a low Z1 (panel A) and a high Z2 (panel B) launch. As in previous studies [35, 31, 51, 52], eye movements were characterized by a combination of smooth pursuit and saccades. Thus, the analysis carried out in the present study aimed at characterizing these two types of eye movements and their synergy during visual tracking. To this aim, eye and ball gaze coordinates were extracted using the calibration parameters as specified in [47], and then differentiated. Pursuit quality was evaluated by means of accuracy and timing parameters.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Representative example of eye and ball gaze position and velocity including saccades.

(A) Gaze coordinates of the ball (red lines) and the left eye (black lines) are shown for one trial relative to the T3Z1 condition for one subject (S9). Azimuth and elevation trajectories are plotted up to the impact event; cyan vertical dotted lines bound the saccadic intervals and define the catch-up phase tracking; blue dotted lines bound the interval of CT phase; (B) gaze coordinates of the ball and the left eye in on trial in the T3Z2 condition. (C) scatter plots of the gaze speed in azimuth against gaze speed in elevation for the same trial reported in panel A; (D) scatter plots of the gaze speed in azimuth against gaze speed in elevation for the same trial reported in panel B. Saccadic events are represented by cyan circles (saccade start) and diamond (saccade end); blue square represent the end of the catch-up phase, the magenta circle the end of the smooth pursuit phase, and black circles the impact event.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g002

Pursuit accuracy parameters quantified the gaze error during tracking. They included:

  1. the positional error:
    (2)
    where and are the eye and ball gaze orientation vectors at time t, whose components are respectively the horizontal and vertical angles in the (θ, φ) plane.
  2. the saccadic gain (GS), i.e. the actual saccade gaze shift amplitude divided by its desired value, that is the difference between the ball position (expressed in gaze coordinates) at the end and the beginning of the saccadic movement.
    (3)

Pursuit timing parameters quantified the duration of the different type of pursuit movements (i.e. smooth pursuit and catch-up saccades) throughout the ball flight, and were defined as follows:

  1. The duration of the catch-up saccade (CUS) phase, i.e. the time interval within the total ball flight duration in which saccades are likely to be triggered during ongoing tracking of the ball. The duration of the CUS phase provides a quantitative measure of the subject's ability at predicting target motion. In fact, catch-up saccades are triggered whenever the positional error accumulates during pursuit because of the lag of the smooth pursuit eye movements with respect to the target motion [15]. They are likely to be present at pursuit initiation [53]. Once the oculomotor system anticipates target motion by predicting its next state, the positional error decreases and hence the probability of observing a catch-up saccade become very low. Thus, the faster subjects are at predicting the current ball motion, the shorter the CUS phase is [15]. We also evaluated the normalized value of the CUS duration, CUSn, obtained by dividing the duration of the CUS phase by the ball flight duration.
  2. The duration of continuous tracking (CT) phase, i.e. the pursuit time interval in which catch-up saccade generation is inhibited and the ball is foveated continuously. As shown in Fig. 2 the typical eye movement patterns observed in this task include an initial CUS phase in which subjects tracked the ball with a combination of pursuit and catch-up saccade movements, followed by a continuous tracking up to a point where the ball was no longer pursued (no-tracking-phase, NT). Thus, the CT duration was computed by considering the time interval from the end of the CUS up to the last sample in the gaze trajectories before ball impact in which PE < 3°. The dark blue dashed lines in Fig. 2 (top and middle rows) bound this interval.
  3. The total duration of smooth pursuit (SP), i.e. the duration of the intervals within the CUS and CT intervals in which the target was foveated. Here we assumed that subjects could have followed the ball with an eccentricity of a few degrees to the fovea [15, 25]. Thus, the SP duration was computed eliminating the saccadic movements from the eye movement patterns and then considering the number of samples (i.e. the time interval) in which PE < 3°.

Two additional parameters were used to assess the quality of ball tracking: tracking gain (Gain) and delay (TAU). Tracking gain is often used in behavioral studies of smooth pursuit. Tracking delay quantified the timing accuracy of the pursuit. It is worth noting that the present experiment was carried out in unrestrained head movement conditions. Here, because the velocity of the target was high (Fig. 1) and exceeded the eye velocity saturation threshold (60°/s [1]), ball tracking was achieved with combined eye and head movements. Thus, the analysis was carried out on gaze coordinates instead of eye coordinates. These quality parameters were computed as follows:

  1. The tracking gain (Gain) was computed as the component of the gaze velocity vector () in the direction of the ball velocity vector (, in gaze coordinates):
    (4)
  2. The time delay (TAU) was defined as the lag of the gaze position with respect to the position of the ball. To evaluate its value we used the same approach reported in [54]. In particular for each time sample t, we minimized the error function defined as:
    (5)
    where dircos is the extent to which the gaze velocity was aligned with the velocity of the ball, and it is computed as follows:
    (6)

We allowed τ to vary within [–200, 200] ms range.

The TAU and Gain pursuit parameters were evaluated during the smooth pursuit time intervals.

Head contribution to gaze

Head movements were analyzed to assess their contribution to the gaze tracking across subjects and experimental conditions. By definition (see Equation 1), the orientation angles describing gaze in space are the sum of corresponding head and eye orientation angles [47] and any given gaze shift amplitude and direction could be obtained by infinite combinations of the two. We wondered whether the different performance levels observed across our subjects in the catching task could be related to different eye-head coordination strategies as suggested in a recent study of elite cricket players [30]. To this aim, we quantified the fraction of gaze shift due to a change in head orientation: (7) (8) where and are, respectively, the azimuth and the elevation coordinates in correspondence of the maximum gaze excursion (i.e.), while and are the azimuth and the elevation coordinates at launch. The closer to 1 is the parameter the higher is its contribution to the gaze. In the case of the eye coordinates, and parameters were computed according to the same expression substituting head with eye coordinates. It is worth noting that the and must sum to 1 according to Equation 1.

Statistical Analysis

A binary response variable Y was used to describe catching success, i.e. Yij = 1 if the i-th subject caught the ball at the j-th trial, Yij = 0otherwise. The j-th trial was characterized by a specific flight duration (Tk, k = [1, 2, 3, 4]), ball arrival height (Zh, h = [1, 2]), and repetition number within each block.

As a first analysis, the association between catching success and experimental conditions (i.e. 4 flight durations and 2 ball arrival heights) was investigated (TEST 1). To this aim, Generalized Linear Mixed Models (GLMM) were fitted to allow both fixed (i.e. the T-Z experimental conditions) and random (i.e. the subjects) effects to be appropriately accounted [55, 56]. In particular, we implemented the following model: (9) where

  • Yij* is a latent response variable, assumed to have a Gaussian distribution, linked to the response variable Yij for subject i in trial j through the probit link function, i.e. such that the expected value of the latent variable E(Yij*) is the inverse of the standard normal cumulative distribution (Φ) of the response probability:
    (10)
  • εij and μi are the error terms, which represent respectively the variability within (εij) and between subjects (μi).
  • tj and z indicate respectively flight duration and ball arrival height of the j-th trial relatively to the i-th subject; in particular tj = Tk, with k = [1, 2, 3, 4] and zj = Zh, with h = [1, 2]. Use of tj and zj as categorical variables were also investigated.

A second analysis (TEST 2) was performed to assess the dependence of several pursuit quality parameters on the ball flight characteristics, taking into account differences between individual subjects. Each of the pursuit parameters was considered the dependent variable in a Linear Mixed Model (LMM) [5759] that takes into account both fixed effects (flight duration and ball arrival height) and random effects (subject). A similar analysis was used in a previous study [20]. As the assessment of which model fits best such random effects structure is a delicate issue [58, 60, 61], we report the complete procedure adopted for each parameter in the Appendix. This analysis allowed testing whether the average values of a parameter varied across subjects and experimental conditions.

We then investigated the relation between catching performance and pursuit quality parameters. As a first explorative analysis, the following model was evaluated: (11) where the vij is the specific parameter under consideration. However, due to the collinearity between the predictors vij, tij and zij observed for the majority of the pursuit parameters evaluated in TEST 2 (see the Results section), such model could not reliably assess a dependence of the catching performance on pursuit parameters. Thus, two different models were evaluated (TEST 3 and TEST 4).

We first assessed whether catching performance could be predicted from pursuit quality parameters separately in each T-Z condition (TEST 3). In particular, we wanted to assess both the effect of pursuit quality parameters on the response variable on a trial-by-trial basis, taking into account the different average pursuit parameter of each individual, and the relation between the response variable and the individual average pursuit parameter. Data from all subjects in each experimental condition were fitted using the following Generalized Linear Mixed Model (GLMM): (12) where Yij* is the latent response variable defined above for the i-th subject and for the j-th trial in the Tk Zh experimental condition; vij is the parameter under consideration, is its average value over the trials in the Tk Zh condition of the i-th subject, and εij and μi are error terms (see above). For each subject the independent variable represents the variation of the parameter from its subject-specific mean in the Tk Zh experimental block, and thus it evaluates the relation between the ocular feature ad the catching response once the individual average distribution is taken into account; we will refer to this term as the variation term. If the βv is significant, it means that there are differences in the vij value across caught and non-caught trials. The variable instead is used to assess whether the subject-specific average value of the parameter is related to catching performance; we will refer to this term as the within-subject average term. If the γ coefficient is significant it means that the different catching performances of individual subjects depends on their specific ocular behaviors.

We then further investigated the relation between pursuit quality parameters and catching performance in all experimental conditions after removing the dependence of pursuit parameters on T and Z (TEST 4). Data from all subjects and all experimental conditions were fitted using the following GLMM [62]: (13) where rij are the residuals of the mixed model linear regression for the specific feature v (TEST 2, see also the Appendix):

(14)

This approach allows to control the partial effect of the variation of vij that affects the response variable Y directly. The maximum likelihood estimation was used to fit the mixed models in accordance to [58].

Finally, we investigated whether improvements in the eye tracking and catching performance were achieved later in each block (TEST 5), by extending the TEST 1 and TEST 2 analyses. In particular, we evaluated whether the inclusion of the trial number inside each block as a predictor ameliorates the LMM and GLMM models fit quality. To this aim, we compared the model structure extrapolated in TEST 1 or in TEST 2 separately for each parameter with a second model which included also the trial number parameter (AICntrial). For instance in the case of TEST 1 we considered the model: (15) where nj indicates the trial number of the of the j-th trial relatively to the i-th subject. Similarly in the case of TEST 2, we added the βntrialnj term, to the model that was selected for the specific parameter (see Results section). The two models were then compared according to the AIC test as described in the Appendix. Statistical analyses were performed in the R software environment (R development Core team (2011). R foundation for statistical computing, Vienna. ISBN:3-900051-07-0 URL http://www.Rproject.org) with the lme4 package (lme4: Linear mixed-effects model using S4 classes. R package version 1.0–5 http://RAN.R-project.org/package=lme4). Multiple comparisons of means (i.e. Tukey Contrasts) were also performed with multcompare package.

Results

Catching performance differs across subjects and depends on ball flight characteristics

The subjects enrolled in the experiment showed broadly varying levels of catching performance. Fig. 3 reports the percentage of caught balls in all the experimental conditions. Subjects were ordered according to their average success rate (i.e. the total number of the caught balls divided by total number of launches), from the lowest (i.e. S1, 2% of successful trials) to the highest values (S9, 80% of successful trials). For all subjects, catching success depended on ball flight characteristics. The statistical analysis (TEST 1, using GLMM) showed a significant fixed effect of the flight duration T on the response variable Y (Table 1).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Percentage of caught and non-caught trials in all the experimental T-Z conditions.

Subjects are ordered according to their average catching success rate (i.e. the total number of the caught balls divided for total number of launches) from the lowest (i.e. S1) to the highest values (S9).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g003

thumbnail
Table 1. Result from fitted Y* response function for TEST 1 analysis (first row), and from the different eye movement parameters (LMM analysis), for TEST 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.t001

In particular, catching success increased significantly as T increased (T: βT > 0, pT < 0.01), while no significant effect of ball arrival height was found in this data set (Z: pZ = 0.68). Thus, given the relation between flight duration and ball approaching speed (Fig. 1, panel B, first column and second row), success rate decreased as speed increased, in accordance with previous studies [22, 46]. Post-hoc tests revealed that there were significant differences in performance between T1 and the other flight duration conditions (p < 0.01 for all the comparison), and between T2 and T4 conditions. The T1 experimental conditions were the most challenging for all subjects: with the exception of S9, who showed a success rate slightly below 60%, all other participants caught less than 40% of the balls (Fig. 3). Tracking the ball in these conditions required the highest azimuth and high elevation gaze angular velocities. Notably, S1 and S2 were unable to catch most of the balls, independently of the speed. In particular S1 succeeded only once throughout the entire experiment, while S2 succeeded only in 14 trials.

General characteristics of gaze behavior during catching

All subjects showed a similar sequence of eye movement events. In the first part of the trial, i.e. 30–60% of the total duration, they tracked the ball with a combination of pursuit and catch-up saccades (CUS phase). Afterwards, they continuously tracked the ball without saccades (CT phase), up to a point before impact in which they no longer tracked the ball (no-tracking, NT phase). Fig. 4 illustrates all the eye movement events recorded in three subjects (S1, S6 and S9). Each row of the raster plots shows the eye movement timeline in one trial of the experimental session, including saccadic events (colored bars, with color identifying different flight conditions), foveal vision intervals (gray bars), and periods in which the subject did not track the ball (white bars). No-tracking intervals were distributed throughout the flight, and in particular: (1) at the beginning of each trial, while the subject looked at the exit hole of the launching system; (2) between consecutive catch-up saccades, whenever a positional error accumulated during pursuit and exceeded the threshold of 3° (see Methods); (3) at the end of the trial (NT phase), some instants before impact, when additional information on the ball motion presumably could no longer influence hand guidance toward the future impact point [11] due to visual processing and command transmission delays [63]. Occasionally, a saccade was also triggered at the end of the trial prior to impact (see the raster plot for S9 in Fig. 4), particularly in the case of low launches. In most cases, subjects re-directed the gaze toward the launching system, while in a few cases they looked the hand at impact.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Time course of eye movement events for three subjects of different catching skill level.

Raster plots of eye movement timeline are shown for all the trials collected during the experimental sessions. Data are aligned to the impact time. Colored bars represent saccades: the different T-Z conditions are color coded as reported in Fig. 1. The different time conditions are bounded by the dashed black lines. Gray bars represent the intervals in which the target was foveated (i.e. PE < 3°). White bars represent the intervals in which the subject was not tracking the ball. The black dots at the end of each bar indicate caught trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g004

These representative subjects give an overview of the general characteristics of gaze behavior observed in this experiment. The average (mean ± SE) values of the timing of different eye movement events in all experimental conditions (columns) is reported for each subject (rows) in Fig. 5. Overall, we observed both differences and similarities in gaze behavior across subjects. Differences were mainly related to the subject-specific distributions of the pursuit quality and timing parameters, and will be described later in this section. Similarities were found in the modulation of the timing of eye movement events (i.e. the first saccade latency, the beginning and the end of CUS and the CT phases) in relation to the ball trajectory characteristics. For instance, in all the subjects as flight duration increased, the duration of the CUS phase (striped colored bars in Fig. 5) and of the CT phase (gray bars) expanded, while the CUS phase was longer and the CT phase shorter in the Z2 conditions than in the Z1 conditions. We analyze in detail the dependence of the eye movement parameters on the ball flight conditions in the next subsection.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Eye movement event timeline for each subject averaged across trials in all the experimental conditions.

Distribution of the eye movement events for each subjects (rows) averaged across trials of each experimental condition (mean ± SE; SE represent by the black lines inside the bars). The different T-Z conditions are reported in different columns. Colored bars represent the average duration of the first saccadic movement; striped colored bars represent the average CUS duration. Gray bars represent the average CT duration. White bars at the beginning and at the end of the raster plots represent respectively the latency time LTs and the NT interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g005

Gaze behavior depends on ball flight characteristics

Ball flight duration and arrival height affected both the timing and accuracy of ocular pursuit. In our experiment longer ball flights and higher ball arrival heights corresponded also to higher initial ball elevation velocities, as shown in Fig. 6 (top row) where the relation between flight duration and elevation angular velocity at launch is reported. We performed a LMM analysis (TEST 2) to assess the relationship between eye movement parameters and flight conditions. The results of the analysis of the number of saccades, the latency and amplitude of the first saccade, the duration of CUS and SP phases, the tracking gain and delay are summarized in Table 1.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Dependence of ball flight duration and number of saccades on ball initial elevation angular velocity.

Top panel: the ball flight duration (i.e. difference between impact and launch time) averaged across the different T-Z conditions (mean ± SE) are plotted with respect to the initial elevation ball velocity at launch. The four different time conditions are indicated by different marker shapes. Data are illustrated separately for the two different arrival heights (right column: high; left column: low). Subject color coding is reported on top of the figure, and it is shaded ranging from black to red depending on subject average score (i.e. the total number of the caught balls total number of launches). Bottom panel: mean number of saccades throughout the trial.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g006

The number of saccades triggered throughout the flight (Fig. 6, bottom panel, data are illustrated separately for the two Z conditions) increased with both flight duration and ball arrival height (βT > 0, βZ > 0, pT < 0.01, pZ < 0.01). Moreover, during the CUS phase, every single catch-up saccade reduced the gaze positional error accumulated during pursuit. In this respect, Fig. 7 shows the average distribution of the saccadic gain across flight duration conditions for each subject, illustrated separately for the two ball arrival heights. On average, the gain increased at each subsequent saccade, being close to 1 at the end of the CUS phase.

thumbnail
Fig 7. Saccadic gain average distribution across flight duration conditions.

The error bar plots show the saccadic gain for respectively the first, the second, the third, and the forth saccade of each subject enrolled in the experiment. Data are averaged across the four flight duration conditions (mean ± SE), and are illustrated separately for the Z2 (top row) and Z1 (bottom row) ball arrival height conditions. Saccades triggered at end of the trial were excluded from this analysis. The numbers on top of each bar represent the percentage of occurrence of respectively the II, III, and IV saccadic event computed for each subject by pulling together data from the same Z conditions. The first saccade had an occurrence of 100%.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g007

The initial oculomotor response was characterized by the latency time (LTs1) and the amplitude (Δφ) of the first saccade (Fig. 8).

thumbnail
Fig 8. First saccade characteristics.

Top panel: first saccade latency times averaged across the different T-Z conditions (mean ± SE) are plotted with respect to the initial vertical angular ball velocity at launch. Data are illustrated separately for the two different arrival heights (right column: Z2, left column: Z1). The different subjects and the four different flight duration conditions are coded as in Fig. 6. Bottom panel: amplitude of the first saccade.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g008

The interaction term between the T and Z fixed effects was significant (p < 0.01) in the case of LTs1. In fact, depending on the ball arrival height, there was a different dependence of LTs1 on flight duration. For example, in the Z2 condition (high) LTs1 increased as the ball flight duration increased, while in the Z1 condition (low) LTs1 showed a more variable behavior (see Fig. 8 top panels). In the case of high launches subjects appeared to initiate pursuit in advance when facing faster balls (i.e. T1 and T2 conditions) with respect to slower conditions (i.e. T3 and T4 conditions). The amplitude of the first saccade was also modulated by ball flight conditions (Fig. 8 bottom panels). Subjects significantly increased the amplitude as flight duration and ball arrival height increased (βT > 0, βZ > 0, both significant). In fact, longer and higher ball trajectories presented also higher initial elevation angular velocities (Fig. 6), which in turn led to larger initial saccadic movements. For this reason data in Figs 8, 9, and 10 are plotted against the initial ball gaze elevation velocity. Differences across subjects were also observed in the distribution of these parameters. They will be described below in a dedicated subsection.

thumbnail
Fig 9. Pursuit timing parameters variation across experimental conditions.

Top row: catch-up saccade phase normalized durations (CUSn, mean ± SE for each TZ condition) are plotted with respect to the initial vertical angular ball velocity values. Data are illustrated separately for the two different arrival heights (right column: Z2, left column: Z1). Bottom row: smooth pursuit normalized duration (SPn). The different subjects and the four different flight duration conditions are coded as in Fig. 6.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g009

thumbnail
Fig 10. Pursuit accuracy parameters variation across experimental conditions.

Top row: smooth pursuit gain parameter averaged for each subject and experimental condition (mean ± SE) is plotted with respect to the initial vertical angular ball velocity at launch. Data are illustrated separately for the two different arrival heights (right column: Z2, left column: Z1). Bottom row: the eye-ball positional lag parameter (TAU). The different subjects and the four different flight duration conditions are coded as in Fig. 6.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g010

Pursuit performance was characterized by timing and accuracy parameters (see Methods). In order to assess the relationship of the CUS and SP pursuit phases on the ball flight conditions, the statistical analysis (TEST 2) was carried out on their values normalized by the ball flight duration, i.e. the CUSn and the SPn parameters (see Methods). Fig. 9 shows the scatter plot of the CUSn (top panel) and SPn (middle panels) values averaged across the trials of each experimental condition (block) for each subject. The CUSn parameter significantly increased as T and Z values increased (βT > 0, βZ > 0, pT < 0.01, pz < 0.01). Post-hoc tests revealed significant differences between T1 and all the other flight time conditions, and between T2 and T4 conditions. The SPn parameter, which assesses the total duration of continuous tracking throughout the entire flight, significantly increased as flight time increased (βT > 0, pT < 0.01), but it was lower in the case of Z2 conditions with respect to Z1Z < 0, pz < 0.01). Post-hoc tests revealed that all comparisons were statistically significant, with the exception of the T2-T3 pair. Overall pursuit duration, given by the sum of the CUS and SP, increased not only because the flight time duration increased, but also because subjects were able to track the ball for a larger fraction of the its flight interval. The quality of tracking during pursuit was assessed by mean of the Gain and the TAU parameters (average values over the SP duration) shown in Fig. 10.

The pursuit gain changed across the ball flight durations, and, in particular, it increased as T increased (βT > 0, pT < 0.01). Post-hoc tests indicated that these differences were mainly ascribed to the T1 condition which was significantly different from the others. Significant differences were also observed between low and high launches (pz < 0.05). Gain was higher in low launches than in high launches. Finally, the TAU parameter, i.e. the time lag between the ball and the gaze, was not influenced by the T and Z factors (pT = 0.41, pZ = 0.76).

In summary, in the longest flight duration conditions, the ball was foveated longer and more accurately. The T1 conditions were the hardest to track, while on average no significant differences were observed between the T3 and T4 conditions both in the SPn and CUSn durations and in the gain parameter. These results, together with the observation that also catching performance increased with longer flight durations (TEST 1), may suggest that there is a direct relation between the accuracy and the duration of ball tracking and the subject’s performance in accordance with previous studies [3]. However, both the increase in catching probability and the increase in smooth pursuit duration may be due to a common dependence on ball flight duration (and hence ball speed, Fig. 1) and not to a direct relation between the two variables. To assess the existence of such relationship we considered the dependence on catching performance on pursuit parameters separately in each flight condition (TEST 3) and in all conditions after taking into account the dependence of each parameter on flight conditions (TEST 4).

Gaze behavior in relation to catching performance

To assess the relationship between pursuit parameters and catching performance we first pooled data from all subjects together and we compared the average values of each parameter over all caught and non-caught trials in each flight condition (Fig. 11).

thumbnail
Fig 11. Eye movement parameters averages in caught and non-caught trials.

The distribution (mean ± SE across subjects) between caught (black bars) and non-caught (white bars) trials, of respectively CUSn, SPn, Gain, TAU and the head contribution parameters (azimuth θ and elevation φ) is reported for each T-Z experimental condition. The difference between the two score groups are indicated by the asterisks when present. Significance code: * the variation term in TEST 3 was significant; † the within-subject average term was significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g011

To determine the significance of the resulting differences, we fitted the dependence of the response variable Y* on both the deviation of the value of each parameter in each individual trial from its subject-specific average and on the subject-subject average with a GLMM (TEST 3, see Methods). In fact, differences could be due to variations of the pursuit parameter related to different catching success probability (i.e. the variation term in Equation 13) but also to different average values of the parameter in subjects with different average catching performance (i.e. the within-subject average term in equation 13). Differences between score groups (i.e. caught and non-caught trials) were not significant for CUSn and Gain in all the eight T-Z experimental conditions (Fig. 11, top panel, leftmost column). Similar results were observed also for TAU (with the exception of the T2Z2 condition). Significant differences between score groups were observed for SPn in the fastest ball flight conditions. In particular, the normalized duration of smooth pursuit was significantly longer in caught than in non-caught trials in the two T2 conditions and in the T1Z1 condition. Then, to assess whether the relation between the pursuit parameters and the catching performance shown separately on each flight condition (TEST 3) was valid across conditions, after taking into account the subject-specific dependence of each pursuit parameters on the flight conditions, we fitted the dependence of the response variable Y* on the residuals of the regression of the pursuit parameters on the flight conditions (TEST 4). Thus, we tested the effect of the variation of each pursuit parameter on the response variable Y* beside its subject-specific dependence on the ball flight characteristics, evaluated with the analysis carried out in TEST 2. Only the SPn parameter affected the response variable Y* as shown by the significant slope coefficient of the GLMM model (βSP = 5.34, pSP < 0.01). No significant slope coefficients were observed for the rest of the pursuit parameters analyzed in this study.

Overall these results show that catching performance was mainly related to the ability of tracking the ball for a longer time.

Different pursuit strategies across subjects

The analyses described above showed a relationship between some of the pursuit parameters and both ball flight characteristics and catching performance. For example in TEST 2 we reported that both the SPn, the CUSn, and the Gain parameters presented higher values in the case of the slower flight conditions (T3 and T4), which were indeed characterized by a larger number of successful trials with respect to the faster conditions (see TEST 1). Next, we tested whether the relationships between pursuit parameters and ball flight conditions (T and Z) significantly differed across individuals. For each parameter, we compared a linear model (LM) relating the pursuit parameter to T and Z for all subjects and a linear mixed model (LMM) including in addition a subject-specific random effect factor (TEST 2, see Methods and Appendix). The models were compared taking into account their complexity with an AIC test: if AIClmm < AIClm, the inclusion of the random factor was justified providing evidence for significant differences in pursuit strategy across subjects. Moreover, depending on the LMM model selected for the specific pursuit feature under analysis (see Appendix and Table 1), we were able to assess the contribution of the random effect to the inter-individual variability. For instance, a subject-specific intercept in the LMM model indicates that the subjects differs on the average value of a specific pursuit parameter while a subject-specific slope indicates a difference in the dependence of a pursuit parameter on a ball flight parameter (T and Z).

Subject-specific eye movement characteristics were present since the beginning of the pursuit. Significant differences across the subjects were found in the LTs1 parameter (AIClmm < AIClm, Table 1). For instance S3, S6, S7, and S8 showed a shorter LTs1 than the other subjects (Fig. 8, top row), while S1 showed a longer LTs1. Inter-individual variability was also observed in the modulation of the amplitude of the first saccade in the different flight conditions. Such difference was captured by an adjustment of the intercept and of the slope of the T factor (p < 0.01) on an individual basis. In particular, S1 showed the highest saccade amplitude range (Fig. 8, middle panels). This subject fixated the exit hole of the launching system longer and then triggered a large initial saccade arriving almost on the target as shown by the high values of the saccadic gain for the first saccade in Fig. 7 (black bar). On the contrary, subjects S8 presented the smallest saccade amplitude range across subjects. Instead of using a single saccadic displacement, S8 triggered a second smaller corrective saccade after the first saccade. In line with this strategy, the saccadic gain distribution reported in Fig. 7 showed that, in the case of S8, the gain of the first saccade was much smaller than the gain of the first saccade of the other subjects, while the gain of the second saccade was comparable to the gain of the first saccade of the other subjects. This behavior was particularly evident for high ball arrival heights and long flight durations (i.e. T2-Z2, T3, and T4 conditions).

The number of saccades triggered throughout the trial differed significantly among subjects (AIClmm < AIClm, Table 1). For instance, S2, S4, S6, S8, and S9 presented a larger number of saccadic events with respect to the rest of participants (Fig. 6 bottom panel). The opposite trend was observed in the case of S1 who showed the smallest number instead. In the case of S8, this result was related to the presence of the corrective saccades triggered at the beginning of pursuit. In the case of S9 and S6 in the Z1 conditions, it was due to the presence of an additional saccade triggered at time of impact (Fig. 4, bottom and middle panels). Finally in the case of S2 and S4 the large saccades number was explained by the longer CUS phase duration with respect to the rest of the participants (Figs. 5 and 9).

Marked difference across subjects were found at the level of the CUSn and the SPn parameters (AIClmm < AIClm, Table 1). However these difference were mainly related to S2, S3, and S4. In particular, S3 and S4 presented the opposite behavior with respect to S2, and showed the smallest CUSn and the highest SPn parameters values (see Fig. 5, bottom panels, and Fig. 9). Notably by repeating the AIC test on a subset of data excluding S2, S3, and S4, we found that the random factor was not significant anymore in the case of the SPn parameter (AIClmm = -611.65 and AIClm = -620.81).

Further evidence for inter-individual variability was provided by the analysis of the smooth pursuit, the Gain, and the TAU parameters (Table 1 and Figs. 10 and 12). For instance, S2 and S4 presented the worst tracking performance: the average values of the gain during pursuit were the lowest in all conditions; the TAU parameter, which quantify the eye-ball positional lag, was the highest. Subject S3 and S7, by contrast, tracked the ball with highest pursuit gain values that in some conditions, i.e. T1Z1, T2Z2 and T2Z4, were on average larger than 1, respectively 1.01 ± 0.23, 1.06 ± 0.05, 1.02 ± 0.03. Subject S9 presented poor tracking performance in the T1Z1 and T2Z1 conditions as shown by the lower average gain values, i.e. 0.7 ± 0.16. These ranges were comparable with those presented by subjects presenting the worst performance, i.e. S1, S2 and S3, respectively 0.76 ± 0.19 in those experimental conditions (Fig. 10). A representative example of the general patterns of the tracking gain from all the trials and the subjects in the T2 conditions is reported in Fig. 12. Finally, according to the AIC criteria, differences across subjects were not significant for the NT duration parameter (AIClmm > AIClm, Table 1). Subjects stopped looking at the ball on average 0.12 ± 0.05 s (mean ± SD) before impact.

thumbnail
Fig 12. Representative examples of the gain patterns.

Data from all the participants and the trials in the T2 conditions are shown separately for the two ball arrival conditions. Red lines represent the average patterns across trials. Gray dashed lines represent the non-caught trials pattern. Black lines represent the caught trials pattern. The gaps in the traces represent the occurrence of a saccades in all the trials. Inter-individual variability was mainly attributed to subjects S2 and S3 who showed on average, respectively, the lowest gain and highest gain values, while all other participants presented similar pursuit gain. Notably, tracking accuracy was similar between the least skilled subject (S1) and the best catcher (S9). Moreover, no marked differences were visible between caught and non-caught trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g012

In summary, we found that the dependence of all the pursuit parameters on the flight conditions was different across subjects, indicating the existence of individual pursuit strategies.

Individual gaze tracking behavior in relation to catching performance

We wondered whether the subjects-specific gaze tracking behaviors described in the previous paragraph were also associated to the individual ability at catching the ball. This information was captured by the within-subject average term in TEST 3 (see Methods). We expected that subjects with more accurate gaze tracking presented also higher catching success rates, at least for the SPn parameter in accordance to the outcomes from TEST 3 and TEST 4 reported above.

With the exception of the Gain parameter in the T4Z1 condition (TEST 3: γ = 13, pγ = 0.025) and the TAU parameter in the T1Z2 condition (γ = -31.9, pγ = 0.04), the within-subject average term in TEST 3 was not significant in the case of the all other pursuit features and ball flight conditions evaluated in this analysis. Hence, the results from the present analysis do not support the hypothesis that superior interceptive performance of a subject is due to his/her superior visual information.

Effect of practice on catching performance and gaze behavior

As subjects performed blocks of trials with the same ball flight characteristics, we assessed whether the trial number within each block affected the dependence of catching performance and ocular pursuit parameters on flight duration and arrival height. We thus extended the GLMM model of the dependence of the response variable on flight characteristics (TEST 1) and the LMM model of the dependence of ocular pursuit parameters (TEST 2) including the trial number in a block as a fixed effect (TEST 5). The inclusion of the trial number improved the GLMM model fit quality (Table 1, AIC = 632.9, AICntrial = 626.8) indicating an effect of practice on catching performance (βntrial = 0.1 and pntrial < 0.001). However, repeating the test on subsets of data including only the fastest (i.e. T1 and T2) or the slowest (i.e.T3 and T4) ball flight conditions, we observed that the effect of the trial number was significant only in the fastest conditions (T1T2: pntrial = 0.006; T3T4: pntrial = 0.246). Interestingly in the case of T1 and T2 conditions, the significant trial number effect was due only to the two subjects with the best performance (i.e. S8 and S9). After excluding these subjects the test was not significant anymore (T1T2: pntrial = 0.11).

Ocular pursuit parameters did not change throughout the block. The inclusion of the trial number in the LMM models did not improve fit quality (AIC < AICntrial) for all ocular pursuit parameters with the exception of the tracking gain (Gain: AIC = -832.79, AICntrial = -834.69) which increased within blocks (βntrial = 0.003, p = 0.04). However, a further analysis revealed that this effect on the gain parameter was ascribed mainly to S3. After excluding this subject the test was not significant anymore (Gain: AIC = -758.3, AICntrial = -755.0). Finally, the analysis was also repeated including only data from the two fastest conditions (T1 and T2), i.e. those in which predictive mechanisms were likely to be predominant with respect to on-line control of the movement [63]. In fact, one possibility is that in those cases adaptation in the gaze behavior could have helped improving catching performance (see results of TEST 5 on the GLMM model relative to the T1 and T2 conditions reported above). For all parameters except the initial saccade latency time (LT1s), the inclusion of the trial number did not improve the fit (AIC < AICntrial). The significant effect for the LT1s parameter (AIC = -1596.8, AICntrial = -1599.9) was ascribed only to S9. After excluding this subject the test was not significant anymore.

Eye-head coordination patterns and their relation with catching performance

As subjects could pursue the flying ball with different combinations of eye and head movements, we wondered if they adopted different eye and head coordination patterns and whether differences in eye-head coordination strategies were related to differences in catching performances. Fig. 13 shows the average head and eye trajectories in the T3Z1 (bottom panels) and T3Z2 (top panels) conditions for each subject (mean ± SE across trials). All the participants initiated pursuit with the head still and they used the first saccadic movement to redirect their gaze on the ball. However, different eye-head coordination patterns were observed across subjects and ball flight conditions during the remaining part of the ball flight.

thumbnail
Fig 13. Inter-individual variability in the eye-head coupling.

The average (mean ± SE across trials) ball (black lines), gaze (red lines), head (blue lines), and eye (gray lines) azimuth (θ) and the elevation (φ) trajectories in the T3 conditions are shown for all the subjects enrolled in the experiment. Top rows: Z2 condition; bottom rows; Z1 condition. Trajectories are plotted up to the impact event. Dotted lines bound the pursuit interval defined as the time windows between the end of the first saccadic event and the end of the CT phase. For the purpose of figure clarity we keep out the initial offset of both the eye and head position due to small bias with respect to the primary position defined at calibration (see Methods).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g013

In the case of the elevation coordinate φ in the Z1 condition (Fig. 13 forth row), participants showed three different behaviors. S1 and S4 initiated to counter-rotate the eye inside the orbit (i.e. decreasing the eye elevation angle while the ball elevation angle is increasing) at the end of the first saccade and simultaneously they started to rotate the head towards the ball. This strategy required the eye to rotate in the opposite direction with respect to the head to compensate for a head rotation larger that the gaze shift. In contrast, S5, S8 and S9 pursued the ball mainly with the eye and they either maintained the head almost still (S5) or presented very small head excursions (S8 and S9). Finally, S2, S3, S6 and S7 showed a third type of eye-head coordination pattern. At the end of the first saccade they pursued the ball mainly with the head and they maintained the eye at a fixed orientation inside the orbit. In the Z2 condition the head contribution to gaze shift was larger than the eye contribution. With the exception of S3 and S4 who counter-rotated the eye during tracking, and S7 who contributed both with the eye and the head to the gaze tracking, the rest of the subjects maintained the eye at a fixed orientation inside the orbit at the end of the CUS phase, and coupled the head to the ball motion (Fig. 13 second row).

Although less marked than for elevation angles, differences across subjects in the eye-head coordination patterns were also present for horizontal gaze tracking, i.e. θ angle, in the Z1 condition (Fig. 13 third row). For instance, some subject relied mainly on eye movements to track the ball, i.e. S1, S2, and S5. The rest of the participants instead tracked the ball with a combination of the eye and head movements. This latter strategy was adopted by seven of the nine subjects (S3—S9) in the Z2 condition (Fig. 13 first row), while the remaining two (S1 and S2) showed larger eye movement than head movements as in the Z1 condition.

Similar strategies were also observed in the other flight duration conditions. The statistical analysis (TEST 2) carried out to assess the dependence of the fraction of gaze shift due to head movements on flight conditions (T and Z, fixed effects) and subject (random effect) indicated that differences across participants were significant both for azimuth and elevation angles (AIClmm < AIClm for both parameters, see Table 1 where only the results relative to the parameters are shown, given the complementary relationship with parameters). Fig. 14 illustrates the average values of and parameters used to quantify the eye and head contribution to the gaze shift of each subject separately for the high (Z2) and low (Z1) arrival height conditions (mean ± SE across all flight duration conditions). In the case of vertical gaze angle φ and the Z1 conditions, S1, S2, S3, S4, and S7 presented larger values with respect to. For S1 and S4 this was also associated with the smallest values, due to the effect of the counter rotation of the eye inside the orbit (see Fig. 13 in the case of the T3 conditions). In contrast, S5, S8, and S9 showed lower values due to their higher eye contribution to gaze shift, whereas S6 used a combination of eye and head movement to track the ball. In the Z2 conditions, the head contribution was predominant in all subjects (see the large white bars in the top rightmost panels of Fig. 14). Moreover, in accordance to Fig. 13, S4 showed the smallest values due to eye counter rotation. In the case of horizontal gaze angle θ and the Z1 conditions S1, S2, S5, S6, and S8, presented the smallest values, while the other participants contributed both with eye and head to gaze shift and hence presented similar values of and. A similar contribution of eye and head to gaze shift was observed also in the case of Z2 with the exception of S1 and S2 (see the large black bars in the top leftmost panel of Fig. 14) who tracked the ball horizontally mainly with the eye movements (gray lines in Fig. 13).

thumbnail
Fig 14. Eye and head contribution to gaze parameter distribution.

Left panels: the azimuth eye (i.e. black bars) and head (i.e. white bars) contributions to gaze are shown. For each subject data are averaged across the flight duration conditions (mean ± SE) and they are illustrated separately for the two different arrival heights conditions (top row: Z2, bottom row: Z1). Right panels: the same is illustrated relatively to the elevation coordinates (i.e. and).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.g014

We then assessed the relationship between the head contribution parameters and catching performance in each individual flight condition. To this end, first we tested whether the catching performance could be predicted by the parameter (TEST3). No differences between caught and non-caught trials were found in the case of both parameters (i.e. the variation term βv: in TEST 3: p >0.05), with the exception of the in the T2Z1 condition (βv: = 1.23, p = 0.044). Interestingly we observed that in the case of the Z1 conditions and the parameter, the within-subject average term, i.e. γS, was significant in all the flight time conditions except T1, and with a negative effect (T1Z1: γS = -3.45, p = 0.13; T2Z1: γS = -3.86, p = 0.01; T3Z1: γS = -4.28, p = 0.02; T4 Z1 γS = -7.8, p = 0.02). According to Fig. 14, the head contribution to the gaze vertical shift was smaller in the case of our most skilled subjects (see the white bars in the rightmost bottom panel). Instead, no significance of the within-subject average term was observed for in the Z2 conditions. In the case of the horizontal head contribution to gaze parameter, i.e., the within-subject average term was significant in four of the total eight conditions, and presented a positive value (T1Z1: γS = 3.59, p = 0.02; T1Z2: γS = 3.25, p = 0.02; T2Z2: γS = 9.47, p = 0.01; T4 Z1 γS = 5.01, p = 0.03). This effect was mainly ascribed to S1 and S2, the catchers with lower success rates, who indeed presented a larger eye contribution to gaze values than the other subjects (see Fig. 14, black bars in the leftmost panels). It is worth noting however that differences across subjects were less marked than in the case of parameter in the Z1 conditions. We also checked whether the relationship between catching performance and the distribution observed in TEST 3 held in all experimental conditions after accounting for the dependence on ball flight conditions (i.e. TEST4). For both parameters the slope coefficient of the GLMM model was not significant (pΔH03B8 = 0.22; pΔHφ = 0.81), meaning that no differences in the parameters were present between caught and non-caught trials.

Discussion

We investigated the extent to which the accuracy in ocular pursuit is related to catching performance. We addressed two different aspects of this issue. On the one hand, we examined whether it was possible to discriminate between caught and non-caught trials according to the characteristics of eye movements, taking into account the fact that different subjects could present different gaze tracking strategies and that both catching and gaze behaviors changed depending on the experimental conditions. On the other hand, we also assessed whether individual average gaze tracking performance influenced the outcome of the catching action. We showed that all subjects presented a similar sequence of oculomotor events and a similar modulation of the timing of these events in relation to the characteristics of the ball trajectory. Gaze tracking performance was better for the slowest ball flight conditions, in which a larger number of successful trials was observed. We also found that keeping the eyes on the moving target for a longer time improved the catching performance on a trial-by-trial basis. However, large differences in ocular behaviors were also observed across our subjects and, differently from what was expected, we did not find a tight coupling between individual gaze tracking performance and catching performance. For instance, the best catchers in our experiment showed an accuracy of ball tracking similar to that of the worst players. These results raise a question as to the role of the visual feedback information underlying the control of an interceptive action. This issue is discussed below.

Gaze behavior in relation to ball flight characteristics and catching performance

The different ball flight conditions used in our study evoked distinct pursuit responses. When investigating the link between gaze and catching behavior and ball motion characteristics, we observed that, as in several previous studies [1, 22, 64], both gaze and catching deteriorated with increasing ball speed (TEST1 and TEST2). For instance, in the T1 and T2 conditions, the number of catches decreased (Fig. 3), smooth pursuit lasted less (Fig. 9), and tracking gains values were lower (Fig. 10) compared to the other experimental conditions. In these fastest conditions, there was little time for executing the catching movement, and it is likely that a preplanned movement control policy was preferred to a continuous feedback control policy [8, 65]. In the slowest conditions, instead, ball tracking was less challenging. The subjects pursued the ball for a greater portion of its flight and with a higher accuracy, and showed a higher percentage of successful catches (Fig. 5). In fact, several studies have shown that target foveation improves the perception of its trajectory features [66] and prediction [8, 26, 28].

However, when relating the interceptive performance directly to the gaze tracking accuracy, we observed that only the duration of the smooth pursuit (SPn) affected the catching performance. According to TEST4, when the pursuit lasted longer, the probability of catching the ball was higher. Moreover, the analysis carried out in TEST 3 showed that this was true in almost all the fastest conditions, i.e. T1Z1, T2Z1 T2Z2, given that in these cases the variation term was significant. We argue that, in the presence of stringent time constraints, gathering enough visual information for ball motion prediction became a crucial issue, as target foveation could be challenging. It follows that even a small improvement in pursuit duration could be highly beneficial for the interceptive outcome. The lack of a similar relationship in the other conditions could be explained by the fact that tracking was easier at lower ball speeds (i.e. T3 and T4).

Notably, no statistical differences between caught and non-caught trials were observed at the level of the other pursuit parameters (CUS, Gain, and TAU). For example, we considered the CUS phase duration a quantitative measure of the subject inability at predicting the ball trajectory, and we would have expected higher CUSn values for the non-caught score group (Fig. 11). In fact, catch-up saccades have often been described as a compensatory strategy to reduce the positional error accumulating during gaze tracking [15, 67], rather than a beneficial mechanism for motion prediction as in the case of the smooth pursuit [25, 27, 53]. However, it has been recently proposed that the extra-retinal signals used to control the smooth pursuit eye movement are also involved in the generation of catch-up saccades [68]. Hence the information of the ball motion characteristics could have been gathered not only via an efference copy signal during the smooth pursuit of the ball [26], but also during the pursuit/saccade tracking. In line with this hypothesis, all our subjects showed a marked increase of the saccadic gain at each subsequent saccade in the initial phase of the ball pursuit (Fig. 8).

To our knowledge, this is the first analysis of the relationship between ocular pursuit performance and interceptive performance on a trial-by-trial basis. The present results provided evidence that in our task the performance of an interceptive action is linked to the ability at tracking the ball for a longer time, thus confirming previous findings [1, 3, 26, 27]. In addition, the lack of significant differences observed for Gain, TAU, and CUS features between caught and non-caught trials suggests that the accuracy at keeping the eye on the ball was not critical for catching. Our best subjects (S6, S7, S8, and S9) showed tracking gain values within a wide range [0.65–0.98] (maximum and minimum average values within each caught trial observed throughout the experimental session), and time lags within a similarly wide range [1.6–98 ms] during pursuit in the case of caught trials. Thus, the analysis of the gaze tracking accuracy might not be informative of the visual signal processing which subserves motor control execution in our task, as argued by other studies [1, 3, 31, 41]. For instance, information on target motion may be gathered peripherally as suggested in [2, 17]. Recently, it has been proposed that information coming from memory from previous trials or expectation are also used to guide the action [4, 22, 51]. These results could explain previous findings from other groups which reported no decrement in the batting performance of cricket players when the quality of the visual input was perturbed by inducing myopic blur up to +2.00D [43].

Gaze behavior variability across subjects in relation to catching performance

We observed large differences in both catching performance and gaze strategies across individuals (see Table 1 and Figs. 8, 9, and 10). Subjects differed in the latency of the pursuit initiation, in the amplitude, accuracy, and number of the first saccades, in the tracking duration, pick up information strategies (see the high inter-individual variability in the CUSn and the SPn durations in Fig. 9), as well as in the eye-head coordination patterns (Figs. 1213). In this context, evidence from previous studies comparing the ocular behaviors of expert and novice players in several ball sports disciplines [1, 3, 30, 42, 69] suggested that that skilled performance relies on superior visual skills. Accordingly, we expected that subjects with a more accurate tracking (i.e. higher gain, longer pursuit time, and shorter lag) also had higher catching success rates.

At single trial level, the logistic regressions analyzed in TEST 4 was significant for the SPn parameter, meaning that longer pursuit durations corresponded to a higher probability of ball catching. At the same time, individual eye movement characteristics were not directly related to individual catching performance level, as the within-subject average term in TEST 3 was not significant in all the gaze tracking parameters analyzed in the present study. In fact, the subjects who tracked the ball longer were not necessarily the subjects with the best catching performance. For instance, S3 showed the highest tracking gains and smooth pursuit durations but he caught only 31% of the balls. Similarly, S1, who succeeded only in 2% of the cases, tracked the ball accurately with values of smooth pursuit durations, saccadic gain, and pursuit gain comparable to those of the best performing subjects (Figs. 4, 9, and 10).

The lack of a direct relationship between the ocular pursuit performance and the subject's catching ability observed in our experiment questions the generality of the conclusions drawn from previous studies investigating visuomotor coordination in interceptive tasks. High-acuity visual information has quite often been considered a prerequisite for good performance in these tasks. Our findings provide evidence that superior visual information is not a necessary condition for superior performance. For instance, more effective use of peripheral vision could explain the differences in the interceptive outcomes observed across the participants of our study. However, other non-visual factors may underlie the lack of a relationship between individual ocular and catching performances observed in our experiment. As suggested above, intercepting a moving target is not only a matter of gathering best visual information. Eye movements might be used to estimate the kinematics of the target, but other factors more related to hand-ball interaction may play a key role for the generation of the required motor plan [70]. These factors may affect the fine tuning of the grasping phase or the generation of the appropriate contact forces at the hand, and may require prior knowledge of the physical properties of the ball, such as its mass [70, 71] or size [8].

We are not suggesting that vision is not important for interception. Rather we argue that its relevance in the generation of the motor responses observed in other studies [3] also depends on the specific task constraints. For instance, discontinuities were not present in the ball paths employed in the present study. Thus, subjects were not forced to gaze at relevant points before the potential change of the ball trajectory as it happens in cricket or in juggling. In contrast, visual information acquired intermittently during the ball flight was likely sufficient to regulate the action and to guide the hand toward the impact point [3, 27, 72]. For this purpose, different gaze behaviors could be similarly effective at gathering the same amount of information on the ball motion, as the inter-individual variability in the oculomotor responses observed in the present study suggests. Recently, it has been shown that in a simulated fly-ball of a baseball video game, when asked to intercept the ball controlling a mouse, subjects were more successful when they gazed for longer periods on the cursor if the ball was visible throughout the entire flight [73]. Instead, with targets occluded near the end, subjects achieved better performance by tracking the target more accurately and by avoiding gaze shifts near interception, suggesting that this target tracking provided better trajectory predictions for the interceptive response [73]. These results further support our claims that the relation between gaze and manual interceptive outcomes depends on the task constraints.

Eye-head coordination variability across subjects in relation to catching performance

In the present study, we also investigated whether the inter-individual variability observed in pursuit and catching behaviors was related to differences in retinal and extraretinal (i.e. based on proprioception of the neck muscles due to head movements) information pick-up strategies. We characterized the eye and head coordination patterns and in particular their contribution to the total gaze shift. Recently Mann and coworkers [30] showed that professional cricket players directed the gaze to the bat at impact. To this aim, in the post-bounce trajectory of the ball they pursued it mainly with the head. In contrast, less expert players closely aligned their eyes with the ball during the entire flight. It has been suggested that, by coupling the direction of the head movement to the motion of the ball, expert players could more easily derive the arrival position and control the hitting movement in a self-centered coordinate frame. In our experiment, we also observed a large variability in the eye-head movement patterns across subjects (Fig. 13), particularly at the level of the vertical gaze shift, i.e. the parameter, in the Z1 conditions. Subjects with an overall high performance level tended to track the ball more with the eyes than with the head, and viceversa in the case of the low level subjects. With the exception of the T1 condition, the within-subject average term in TEST 3 was always statistically significant for the parameter. Surprisingly, the opposite behavior was observed with respect to the horizontal gaze shift, i.e.. Although the within-subject average term in TEST 3 was not significant in some conditions (see the Results section), there was a tendency toward using more the eye than the head for ball tracking in the case of low performance level subjects S1 and S2 (see the large black bars in the leftmost columns of Fig. 14). It is worth mentioning, however, that in the Z1 conditions differences across performance level were less marked than in the case of the parameter, given that also S5, S6, and S8 showed a larger contribution of the eye movement, i.e. the parameter, to the total gaze shift over the head (see the black bars in the bottom leftmost panels of Fig. 14). Notably, our analysis was carried out separately for the vertical and horizontal gaze angles, because we observed that the azimuth component of gaze becomes large when the ball approaches the subject in the final part of its trajectory (see Figs. 1, 2, and 13), whereas the visual angle or only one direction of the visual excursion, considered as the relevant one, were often evaluated in previous studies [1, 2, 4, 39, 42].

In sum, our results are in line with the findings of Mann and colleagues [30,43] relatively to the horizontal gaze shift, i.e. the parameter, but they differ in the vertical direction, i.e. the parameter. It is likely that in the case of low performance level subjects were less capable at maintaining head-ball coupling due to the large angular velocities observed in the final part of the ball trajectory (see Fig. 1 rightmost panels and Fig. 2). Nevertheless we believe that the different nature of the tasks employed in our study and in the study by Mann and colleagues might explain the different results we obtained for the parameter. The peculiar head movement strategies observed in professional players in [30] arose from their necessity to see the ball while hitting it with the bat, and hence these strategies concerned more the final part of the ball trajectory. However, in our experiment, subjects had to catch the ball with the hand. Proprioception instead of visual feedback was likely sufficient for limb positioning and orientation as argued in [74]. Accordingly, our subjects stopped watching the ball ~120 ms prior to contact (i.e. NT phase). Instead, the interaction with the bat in cricket (or a racquet in tennis and squash) is likely to require shifting of the gaze from the moving target to the future ball-bat contact. This mechanism may facilitate fine tuning of the arm and wrist orientation and it would be beneficial for the hitting movement execution as suggested in [30]. Moreover, no discontinuities were present in the ball trajectory in our experiment. While gathering the major amount of visual information in the post bounce is crucial for interception in the case of cricket or tennis sports due to the abrupt change of ball motion direction [35], in our experiment the smooth nature of the ball path may have reduced the importance of visual feedback at ball-hand contact for action guidance.

Overall, our results are more in line with other previous findings, which reported flexible behaviors of the eye and the head contributions to gaze both between and within subjects in a similar task [39]. These findings suggest that different information pick-up mechanisms may have been exploited by our subjects to capture information on the target motion.

Conclusions

We investigated the relation between the catching performance and the gaze tracking accuracy in a one handed catching task. On a trial-by-trial basis, we showed that the ability at tracking the ball longer increased catching probability. At the same time, we could not find a significant relationship between the subjects' average accuracy and duration of the ball visual tracking, on one hand, and the subjects' level of catching performance, on the other hand, as observed in previous studies. Instead, we showed that different gaze behaviors and eye-head coordination strategies may emerge in different individuals. These results suggest that other non-ocular factors underlie the observed variability in the sensorimotor strategies and performance outcomes observed in our experiment. One of the assumptions that has often been made in the neuroscience literature is that there are optimal behavioral and perceptual strategies toward which subjects converge when performing a complex task [75]. On the other hand, it has also been shown that goal equivalent motor control solutions are possible when task constraints are relaxed [20, 76]. Thus, a number of perceptual strategies were probably possible to gather information about ball kinematics in our experiment. For example, subjects could track the ball continuously, but rely on motion signals at some instant or on average signals across presentation time [66]. Also a different use of retinal and extraretinal cues was possible [39]. We argued that subjects’ intention, attention, and inter-individual variability in the sensitivity to different types of visual cues could have played a role in the manifestation of one strategy with respect to the others, while non ocular factors, more related to the hand-ball interaction, probably affected the success of interceptive movements. Future studies will investigate the potential interactions between these different elements in the control of interceptive movements.

Appendix

The use of the Linear Mixed Model is appropriate whenever the experimental design presents crossed fixed effects, here the different T- Z experimental conditions, and random effects, i.e. the subjects enrolled in the experiment [58, 59]. The first step of our analysis was aimed at assessing whether the inclusion of the random effect in the model, that is the use of LMM instead of a simpler Linear Model LM, was justified. To this aim, we used the same approach described in our previous study [20]. In particular we fitted the two models: (1A) (2A) where i is the i-th subject and j is the j-th trial, vij is the eye movement feature under analysis, and S0i is the offset term that accounts for the by subject adjustment of the intercept term, i.e. the deviation from the β0. To evaluate the significance of the random effects, that is the presence of inter individual variability in the parameter distribution, we used the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) for both the LMM and LM analysis [55]. The AIC evaluates the quality of the fit taking into account the complexity of the model (the lower the AIC value, the better the model fitting, see [57]). If the AIClmm resulted lower than the AIClm the inclusion of the subject factor was justified, providing evidence for inter-individual differences. The level of significance was set as p <0.05.

The second step consisted in the evaluation of the interaction between the fixed effects. If the previous test confirmed the significance of the random effects inclusion, LMM0 was compared with the following model: (3A) Otherwise the simpler linear model was evaluated and no more actions were required:

(4A)

Mixed models comparison and the p-values of the slopes coefficients were computed according to the likelihood ratio test as reported in [58]. Once the random and fixed structures were assessed, the resulting model was re-fitted according to the restricted likelihood criteria which is more conservative.

Once the structure of the fixed effect was assessed, the next step was the identification of the random effects structure. For instance, the model could include either a simpler structure that accounts only for between-subjects manipulations, hence introducing only by-subjects adjustments to the intercept as in (16), or a more complex model which also allowed for within-subject manipulations. In this latter case, the extent to which subject's effect deviates from the population fixed effect slopes was also investigated.

In accordance with several dedicated studies [25, 58, 60, 61] we adopted the "confirmatory hypothesis testing" approach to identify the best model that fits our data. Briefly, we identified a set of specific critical hypotheses on the data structure and then measured the evidence for or against them. Typically the models are built iteratively, starting from the simpler model structure and then adding more random factors incrementally. At each iteration the simpler and the more complex models are compared according to the likelihood ratio test as reported in [58].

The models evaluated for the present analysis are listed below in sequential order (in the following we will consider the interaction term not significant): (5A) where the slope offset S1i captures how much each subject deviates from the slope relative to the ball flight time factor T. By comparing the LMM2 and LMM0 we assessed both whether the inclusion of the random slope factor was appropriate, and the significance of the S1i coefficient. If this hypothesis was confirmed, LMM2 model was compared with the following: (6A) where in addition to the S1i offset, the amount each subject deviates from the slope relative to the ball arrival height factor Z was considered by the S2i coefficient. Otherwise LMM0 was compared with:

(7A)

Table 1 reports the selected models and the outcomes of the analysis for each pursuit parameter. In addition the coefficients of determination were also computed. In particular we separated the contribution of fixed and random effects by calculating both the marginal (i.e. the proportion of variance explained by the fixed factor) and the conditional (i.e. the proportion of variance explained by both the fixed and random factors) R2 parameters according to the following formulas:

(8A)(9A)

Supporting Information

S1 DataSet. A compressed archive (.zip) contains a data file and a text file with a detailed description of the data organization.

The data file is in Matlab format (.mat) and contains a data structure with, for each subject and for each trial: 1) the gaze coordinates estimated from the eye-tracker recordings as specified in the Methods; 2) the head, the body, and the ball data recorded with the Vicon Motion capture system; 3) the information about the trial characteristics (i.e. the T—Z condition, the catching score, and the launch and impact time events).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119445.s001

(ZIP)

Acknowledgments

We thank Matteo Dell' Omodarme for initial advices on the statistical analyses.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: BC AdA FL. Performed the experiments: BC. Analyzed the data: BC AdA MM. Wrote the paper: BC AdA MM FL. Designed the software used in analysis: BC AdA. Performed the statistical analysis: BC MM.

References

  1. 1. Bahill AT, LaRitz T. Why can't batters keep their eyes on the ball? American Scientist. 1984;72:249–53.
  2. 2. Croft JL, Button C, Dicks M. Visual strategies of sub-elite cricket batsmen in response to different ball velocities. Hum Mov Sci. 2010;29:751–63. pmid:20031242
  3. 3. Land MF, McLeod P. From eye movements to actions: how batsmen hit the ball. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1340–5. pmid:11100157
  4. 4. Diaz G, Cooper J, Rothkopf C, Hayhoe M. Saccades to future ball location reveal memory-based prediction in a virtual-reality interception task. J Vis. 2013;13.
  5. 5. Hayhoe MM, McKinney T, Chajka K, Pelz JB. Predictive eye movements in natural vision. Exp Brain Res. 2012;217:125–36. pmid:22183755
  6. 6. Mrotek LA. Following and intercepting scribbles: interactions between eye and hand control. Exp Brain Res. 2013;227:161–74. pmid:23552996
  7. 7. Peper L, Bootsma RJ, Mestre DR, Bakker FC. Catching balls: how to get the hand to the right place at the right time. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1994;20:591–612. pmid:8027714
  8. 8. Zago M, McIntyre J, Senot P, Lacquaniti F. Visuo-motor coordination and internal models for object interception. Exp Brain Res. 2009;192:571–604. pmid:19139857
  9. 9. de'Sperati C, Viviani P. The relationship between curvature and velocity in two-dimensional smooth pursuit eye movements. J Neurosci. 1997;17:3932–45. pmid:9133411
  10. 10. Marinovic W, Plooy AM, Tresilian JR. The utilisation of visual information in the control of rapid interceptive actions. Exp Psychol. 2009;56:265–73. pmid:19439399
  11. 11. Sharp RH, Whiting HTA. Information-processing and eye movement behaviour in a ball catching skill. Journal of human Movement Studies. 1975;1:124–31.
  12. 12. Bosco G, Delle Monache S, Lacquaniti F. Catching what we can't see: manual interception of occluded fly-ball trajectories. PLoS One. 2012;7:e49381. pmid:23166653
  13. 13. Dessing JC, Oostwoud Wijdenes L, Peper CL, Beek PJ. Adaptations of lateral hand movements to early and late visual occlusion in catching. Exp Brain Res. 2009;192:669–82. pmid:18936928
  14. 14. Schalen L. Quantification of tracking eye movements in normal subjects. Acta Otolaryngol. 1980;90:404–13. pmid:7211334
  15. 15. Orban de Xivry JJ, Lefevre P. Saccades and pursuit: two outcomes of a single sensorimotor process. J Physiol. 2007;584:11–23. pmid:17690138
  16. 16. Savelsbergh GJ, Williams AM, Van der Kamp J, Ward P. Visual search, anticipation and expertise in soccer goalkeepers. J Sports Sci. 2002;20:279–87. pmid:11999482
  17. 17. Abernethy B, Russell DG. The relationship between expertise and visual search strategy in a raquet sport. Human Movement Science. 1987;6:283–319.
  18. 18. Shapiro A, Lu ZL, Huang CB, Knight E, Ennis R. Transitions between central and peripheral vision create spatial/temporal distortions: a hypothesis concerning the perceived break of the curveball. PLoS One. 2010;5:e13296. pmid:20967247
  19. 19. Tynan PD, Sekuler R. Motion processing in peripheral vision: reaction time and perceived velocity. Vision Res. 1982;22:61–8. pmid:7101752
  20. 20. Cesqui B, d'Avella A, Portone A, Lacquaniti F. Catching a ball at the right time and place: individual factors matter. PLoS One. 2012;7:e31770. pmid:22384072
  21. 21. Mazyn LI, Savelsbergh GJ, Montagne G, Lenoir M. Planning and on-line control of catching as a function of perceptual-motor constraints. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2007;126:59–78. pmid:17239809
  22. 22. Tijtgat P, Bennett SJ, Savelsbergh GJ, De Clercq D, Lenoir M. Advance knowledge effects on kinematics of one-handed catching. Exp Brain Res. 2010;201:875–84. pmid:19949776
  23. 23. Katsumata H, Russell DM. Prospective versus predictive control in timing of hitting a falling ball. Exp Brain Res. 2012;216:499–514. pmid:22120106
  24. 24. Zago M, Bosco G, Maffei V, Iosa M, Ivanenko YP, Lacquaniti F. Internal models of target motion: expected dynamics overrides measured kinematics in timing manual interceptions. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:1620–34. pmid:14627663
  25. 25. Barnes GR. Cognitive processes involved in smooth pursuit eye movements. Brain Cogn. 2008;68:309–26. pmid:18848744
  26. 26. Spering M, Schutz AC, Braun DI, Gegenfurtner KR. Keep your eyes on the ball: smooth pursuit eye movements enhance prediction of visual motion. J Neurophysiol. 2011;105:1756–67. pmid:21289135
  27. 27. Brenner E, Smeets JB. Continuous visual control of interception. Hum Mov Sci. 2011;30:475–94. pmid:21353717
  28. 28. Wilmut K, Wann JP, Brown JH. How active gaze informs the hand in sequential pointing movements. Exp Brain Res. 2006;175:654–66. pmid:16794847
  29. 29. Brouwer AM, Middelburg T, Smeets JB, Brenner E. Hitting moving targets: a dissociation between the use of the target's speed and direction of motion. Exp Brain Res. 2003;152:368–75. pmid:12898090
  30. 30. Mann DL, Spratford W, Abernethy B. The head tracks and gaze predicts: how the world's best batters hit a ball. PLoS One. 2013;8:e58289. pmid:23516460
  31. 31. Dessing JC, Rey FP, Beek PJ. Gaze fixation improves the stability of expert juggling. Exp Brain Res. 2012;216:635–44. pmid:22143871
  32. 32. Huys R, Beek PJ. The coupling between point-of-gaze and ball movements in three-ball cascade juggling: the effects of expertise, pattern and tempo. J Sports Sci. 2002;20:171–86. pmid:11999474
  33. 33. Amazeen EL, Amazeen PG, Beek PJ. Eye movements and the selection of optical information for catching. Ecological Psychology. 2001;13:71–85.
  34. 34. Vickers JN, Adolphe RM. Gaze behaviour during a ball tracking and aiming skill. International Journal of Sports Vision. 1997;4:18–27.
  35. 35. Ripoll H, Fleurance P. What does keeping one's eye on the ball mean? Ergonomics. 1988;31:1647–54. pmid:3229411
  36. 36. Rodrigues ST, Vickers JN, Williams AM. Head, eye and arm coordination in table tennis. J Sports Sci. 2002;20:187–200. pmid:11999475
  37. 37. Singer RN, Williams AM, Frehlich SG, Janelle CM, Radlo SJ, Barba DA, et al. New frontiers in visual search: an exploratory study in live tennis situations. Res Q Exerc Sport. 1998;69:290–6. pmid:9777666
  38. 38. Abrams RA, Meyer DE, Kornblum S. Eye-hand coordination: oculomotor control in rapid aimed limb movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1990;16:248–67. pmid:2142197
  39. 39. Oudejans RR, Michaels CF, Bakker FC, Davids K. Shedding some light on catching in the dark: perceptual mechanisms for catching fly balls. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1999;25:531–42. pmid:10205865
  40. 40. McLeod P. Visual reaction time and high-speed ball games. Perception. 1987;16:49–59. pmid:3671040
  41. 41. Regan D. Vision and cricket. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2012;32:257–70. pmid:22574826
  42. 42. Abernethy B. Expertise, visual search, and information pick-up in squash. Perception. 1990;19:63–77. pmid:2336337
  43. 43. Mann DL, Ho NY, De Souza NJ, Watson DR, Taylor SJ. Is optimal vision required for the successful execution of an interceptive task? Hum Mov Sci. 2007;26:343–56. pmid:17289195
  44. 44. Land MF. On the functions of double eyes in midwater animals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2000;355:1147–50. pmid:11079387
  45. 45. d'Avella A, Cesqui B, Portone A, Lacquaniti F. A new ball launching system with controlled flight parameters for catching experiments. J Neurosci Methods. 2011;196:264–75. pmid:21256864
  46. 46. Mazyn LI, Montagne G, Savelsbergh GJ, Lenoir M. Reorganization of catching coordination under varying temporal constraints. Motor Control. 2006;10:143–59. pmid:16871010
  47. 47. Cesqui B, de Langenberg R, Lacquaniti F, d'Avella A. A novel method for measuring gaze orientation in space in unrestrained head conditions. J Vis. 2013;13. pmid:24344056
  48. 48. Bennett SJ, Barnes GR. Human ocular pursuit during the transient disappearance of a visual target. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90:2504–20. pmid:14534275
  49. 49. de Brouwer S, Missal M, Lefevre P. Role of retinal slip in the prediction of target motion during smooth and saccadic pursuit. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86:550–8. pmid:11495930
  50. 50. Mrotek LA, Flanders M, Soechting JF. Oculomotor responses to gradual changes in target direction. Exp Brain Res. 2006;172:175–92. pmid:16418846
  51. 51. Diaz G, Cooper J, Hayhoe M. Memory and prediction in natural gaze control. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2013;368:20130064. pmid:24018726
  52. 52. Lopez-Moliner J, Brenner E, Louw S, Smeets JB. Catching a gently thrown ball. Exp Brain Res. 2010;206:409–17. pmid:20862460
  53. 53. de Brouwer S, Yuksel D, Blohm G, Missal M, Lefevre P. What triggers catch-up saccades during visual tracking? J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:1646–50. pmid:11877535
  54. 54. Mrotek LA, Soechting JF. Target interception: hand-eye coordination and strategies. J Neurosci. 2007;27:7297–309. pmid:17611282
  55. 55. Moscatelli A, Mezzetti M, Lacquaniti F. Modeling psychophysical data at the population-level: the generalized linear mixed model. J Vis. 2012;12.
  56. 56. Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MH, et al. Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol. 2009;24:127–35. pmid:19185386
  57. 57. Hoffman L, Rovine MJ. Multilevel models for the experimental psychologist: foundations and illustrative examples. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:101–17. pmid:17552476
  58. 58. Barr DJ. Random effects structure for testing interactions in linear mixed-effects models. Front Psychol. 2013;4:328. pmid:23761778
  59. 59. Locker L Jr, Hoffman L, Bovaird JA. On the use of multilevel modeling as an alternative to items analysis in psycholinguistic research. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:723–30. pmid:18183884
  60. 60. Baayen RH, Davidson DJ, Bates DM. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language. 2008;59:390 412.
  61. 61. Bates DM. lmer4: Mixed-effects modeling with R 2010.
  62. 62. Willett W, Stampfer MJ. Total energy intake: implications for epidemiologic analyses. Am J Epidemiol. 1986;124:17–27. pmid:3521261
  63. 63. Tresilian JR. Hitting a moving target: perception and action in the timing of rapid interceptions. Percept Psychophys. 2005;67:129–49. pmid:15912877
  64. 64. Bahill AT, Kallman JS. Predicting final eye position halfway through a saccade. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 1983;30:781–6. pmid:6662536
  65. 65. Laurent M, Montagne G, Savelsbergh GJ. The control and coordination of one-handed catching: the effect of temporal constraints. Exp Brain Res. 1994;101:314–22. pmid:7843318
  66. 66. Spering M, Montagnini A. Do we track what we see? Common versus independent processing for motion perception and smooth pursuit eye movements: a review. Vision Res. 2011;51:836–52. pmid:20965208
  67. 67. Krauzlis RJ. Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:591–603. pmid:14762145
  68. 68. Daye PM, Blohm G, Lefevre P. Catch-up saccades in head-unrestrained conditions reveal that saccade amplitude is corrected using an internal model of target movement. J Vis. 2014;14. pmid:25540263
  69. 69. Abernethy B. Anticipation in squash: differences in advance cue utilization between expert and novice players. J Sports Sci. 1990;8:17–34. pmid:2359149
  70. 70. Lacquaniti F, Maioli C. The role of preparation in tuning anticipatory and reflex responses during catching. J Neurosci. 1989;9:134–48. pmid:2913200
  71. 71. D'Andola M, Cesqui B, Portone A, Fernandez L, Lacquaniti F, d'Avella A. Spatiotemporal characteristics of muscle patterns for ball catching. Front Comput Neurosci. 2013;7:107. pmid:23966939
  72. 72. Panchuk D, Davids K, Sakadjian A, Macmahon C, Parrington L. Did you see that? Dissociating advanced visual information and ball flight constrains perception and action processes during one-handed catching. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2013;142:394–401. pmid:23435115
  73. 73. Delle Monache S, Lacquaniti F, Bosco G. Eye movements and manual interception of ballistic trajectories: effects of law of motion perturbations and occlusions. Exp Brain Res. 2015;233:359–74. pmid:25311389
  74. 74. Schenk T, Mair B, Zihl J. The use of visual feedback and on-line target information in catching and grasping. Exp Brain Res. 2004;154:85–96. pmid:14661068
  75. 75. Yarrow K, Brown P, Krakauer JW. Inside the brain of an elite athlete: the neural processes that support high achievement in sports. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:585–96. pmid:19571792
  76. 76. Ganesh G, Haruno M, Kawato M, Burdet E. Motor memory and local minimization of error and effort, not global optimization, determine motor behavior. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104:382–90. pmid:20484533