Skip to main content

Midazolam, hippocampal function, and transitive inference: Reply to Greene

Abstract

The transitive inference (TI) task assesses the ability to generalize learned knowledge to new contexts, and is thought to depend on the hippocampus (Dusek & Eichenbaum, 1997). Animals or humans learn in separate trials to choose stimulus A over B, B over C, C over D and D over E, via reinforcement feedback. Transitive responding based on the hierarchical structure A > B > C > D > E is then tested with the novel BD pair. We and others have argued that successful BD performance by animals – and even humans in some implicit studies – can be explained by simple reinforcement learning processes which do not depend critically on the hippocampus, but rather on the striatal dopamine system. We recently showed that the benzodiazepene midazolam, which is thought to disrupt hippocampal function, profoundly impaired human memory recall performance but actually enhanced implicit TI performance (Frank, O'Reilly & Curran, 2006). We posited that midazolam biased participants to recruit striatum during learning due to dysfunctional hippocampal processing, and that this change actually supported generalization of reinforcement values. Greene (2007) questions the validity of our pharmacological assumptions and argues that our conclusions are unfounded. Here we stand by our original hypothesis, which remains the most parsimonious account of the data, and is grounded by multiple lines of evidence.

Background

Our interpretation of our findings was based on the following premises. When humans are prevented from becoming explicitly aware of the hierarchy they can still perform better than chance at the novel "inference" test using an implicit reinforcement learning system [1]. Several mathematical and neural models show that when trained with the TI task procedure, stimulus B develops a higher associative strength then stimulus D, and that transitive responding can be achieved simply by comparing these implicit values [15]. Thus although under some circumstances the hippocampus can play a subtle modulatory role in setting up these values [4] (and is likely critical for genuine, explicit logical inference), neural models suggest that the elemental associative learning process itself does not depend on the hippocampus, but rather on the striatal dopamine system [6, 7]. Moreover, the striatal and hippocampal systems often compete, such that disruption of one system can lead to enhanced performance of tasks that depend on the other (for review: [8, 9]).

We therefore posited that midazolam would disrupt the hippocampus [1018], thereby removing its normal inhibitory interaction with the striatum, and allowing that system to dominate. The resulting impaired hippocampal learning but enhanced implicit reinforcement encoding led to (i) impaired explicit memory recall; (ii) a pattern of learning in the initial training phase of the TI task that is a characteristic signature of associative models: greater performance on the end anchor pairs AB and DE, which can be solved purely on the basis of reinforcement values, and worse performance on the conjunctive inner pairs BC and CD; (iii) substantially enhanced generalization of reinforcement values in the transitive test [19]. These results support predictions from more abstract mathematical models in which conjunctive learning is detrimental to subsequent transitive responding [5]. Finally, in another probabilistic learning task associated with the striatal dopamine system [7], midazolam led to spared performance and only caused deficits in the very first few trials of the task [19], when the hippocampus is usually most active in such tasks [20], consistent with other accounts on the role of the hippocampus in the early acquisition of probabilistic learning [21].

In his critique, Greene argued that our theory entails three critical assumptions that are "required but not met" [22] (pp. 1–2):

  1. 1.

    that other areas critical to TI are not affected by midazolam administration, so only hippocampal deactivation can explain the effect;

  2. 2.

    that midazolam deactivates the hippocampus so that it no longer functions in a mnemonic capacity; and

  3. 3.

    that midazolam influences explicit but not implicit memory.

It is reasonable to question the assumptions of our logic. Nevertheless, on the whole we believe this criticism is misguided. First, there is ample evidence to suggest that hippocampal function is dramatically impaired by midazolam administration, and critically, no indication that the striatal reinforcement learning system – strongly predicted to support reinforcement-based generalization in this task – is negatively affected. Second, while it is certainly possible that areas other than the hippocampus are affected by the systemic drug, it is entirely unclear why this would result in an enhancement in TI performance as was observed [19]. In contrast, our theoretical framework clearly accounts for the effect, and is supported by other diagnostic converging data in our study. Third, the notion that midazolam impairs explicit memory substantially more than implicit memory, while well supported by psychological data, is actually not a critical assumption of our logic, which depends more specifically on striatal-dependent implicit associative learning rather than on implicit learning in general.

Discussion

Below we elaborate these and other issues in response to Greene's three criticisms, and identify some new avenues for research to more directly address the question.

Assumption 1: other areas critical to TI account for the effect

Greene takes issue with our suggestion that midazolam improved TI performance by deactivating the hippocampus [19], stating that "it is entirely possible that the effects of midazolam on the TI task are attributable to deactivations of areas other than the hippocampus". He cites PET evidence that midazolam inactivates some of the very same frontal and parietal regions that have been shown to be activated during explicit TI performance in other studies [23, 24]. We are puzzled by this point, as it is difficult to imagine how deactivation of a region that is normally activated during TI would lead to enhanced performance. Admitting possible additional frontal cortical effects of midazolam, our key point was that thus far no studies implicate striatal deactivation following midazolam. Other evidence from Parkinson's patients and dopamine medication manipulations support predictions from computational models of the basal ganglia, which suggest that this system is involved in learning reinforcement associations in the TI task [7]. These data provide converging evidence for a role of striatal DA system in learning implicit associative values in the TI task.

We note here that Greene is undeniably correct that by administering a systemic drug one cannot know for certain that either the observed behavioral decrements in recall (amnesia) and the associated improvement in TI, are related specifically to hippocampal deactivation. However, our central hypothesis is that the critical brain system supporting reinforcement-based learning in the TI and other feedback learning tasks is the striatal dopamine system, and to the best of our knowledge there is no evidence to suggest this system is inhibited by midazolam, either at the neural level or in learning tasks that depend on this system. Moreover, there is some evidence in rats that the two main peptide markers of activity in the direct and indirect pathways of the basal ganglia (dynorphin and enkephalin; [25]) are actually substantially enhanced under systemic midazolam administration [26, 27], as are striatal dopamine levels [28]. These effects could conceivably result from the removal of the normal inhibitory interaction with hippocampus, which have been observed under a variety of conditions in both animals and humans [8, 9]. As one recent (non-pharmacological) example in humans, when dual task conditions were introduced to interfere with explicit memory, procedural learning normally correlated with hippocampal activation and associated declarative knowledge proceeded instead in a habitual fashion that correlated with striatal activation [29].

Assumption 2: midazolam obliterates hippocampal function

Greene also questions whether midazolam disrupts hippocampal function at all. That is, despite the dense expression of benzodiazepene receptors in the hippocampus [30], some human neuroimaging studies fail to detect hippocampal deactivation under midazolam. While one study did show a dose-dependent hippocampal effect [15], others using low doses did not. However, the effects of drugs on BOLD activity are not straightforward. First, BOLD is a very indirect and imprecise measure of neural activity, whereas direct administration of midazolam in the hippocampus show robust disruptions of both neural activity and plasticity [1013]. These findings are not restricted to local injections into the hippocampus. For example, systemic midazolam administration has been shown to decrease hippocampal cholinergic activity [14]. While this action should clearly interfere with hippocampal function and memory encoding, it is much less clear what effect, if any, this would have on BOLD activity – it could simply add noise to the hippocampal system. Indeed, this hippocampal noise hypothesis has been modeled to account for various detrimental memory encoding effects of midazolam [17]). It is similarly unclear how midazolam's inhibitory effects on hippocampal long term potentiation [18] – the central mechanism thought to give rise to hippocampal memories – would translate into BOLD.

Furthermore, Greene also asserts that our theory requires midazolam to "deactivate the hippocampus to the point that it no longer functions in a mnemonic capacity". This is clearly not the case – while the low dose used in our study robustly impaired memory encoding/recall, this memory was far from obliterated altogether. These findings are similar to other studies purporting that midazolam impairs hippocampal function and substantially degrades, but does not eliminate, episodic memory [16, 17, 31]. In contrast, hippocampal amnesics were substantially impaired at learning the training pairs, particularly the inner pairs that require conjunctive encoding, never reaching training criteria [32]. Thus although these amnesics were impaired at the TI probe test, these data are not meaningful given that they never acquired the relevant premises in the first place. In contrast, participants on midazolam were impaired initially at the conjunctive inner pairs relative to control subjects, but this deficit was subtle enough that they were able to eventually overcome it. Other studies show that both humans and animals with hippocampal damage are completely unimpaired at re-combining learned elemental reinforcement values to support generalization of learned behavior to novel situations [33, 34].

Assumption 3: midazolam impairs explicit but not implicit memory

Finally, Greene claims that our argument requires midazolam to affect explicit but not implicit memory, but that there are a few studies showing some impairments in implicit memory. We note that first, there is substantial evidence to suggest that midazolam has a far greater proportional impairing effect on explicit than implicit memory [3538]. Other recent studies showed that the same experimental procedures (including midazolam dose) used in our study selectively abolished event-related brain potentials associated with recall, which had been previously linked to hippocampal function, without affecting other memory components [31]. However, the need for midazolam to spare implicit memories of all types is actually not a requirement of our theory at all. Indeed, we posit that midazolam impairs the conjunctive encoding of stimulus features in the hippocampus, and while this function is critical for several aspects of explicit memory, it can also support some aspects of implicit memory [39]. Thus again our argument relies on the notion that midazolam has not been shown to impair striatal-dependent memories, i.e. in so-called procedural learning tasks.

Future studies

To help further resolve this controversy, midazolam effects on hippocampal activity ought to be examined in situations for which the hippocampus is thought to be critical according to the same computational principles used to motivate our original hypothesis. It would be particularly informative to explore the effects of midazolam on a contrast between conjunctive and elemental memory encoding in the TI or other task showing hippocampal sensitivity to conjunctions [40]. Indeed, Greene's own study [41] showed hippocampal activation for the conjunctive "inner pairs" in the TI task, and midazolam impaired acquisition of these same pairs in our study [19], and impaired conjunctive encoding in other tasks [39].

Conclusion

Our original study was motivated by an explicit theoretical and neurocomputational framework for why this pattern should be observed, and is consistent with other psychological and mathematical analysis. In contrast, while legitimately questioning some of our assumptions, Greene provides no alternative explanation for the finding that midazolam substantially improved TI performance in our study. Until such an interpretation arises, with supporting data, the most parsimonious explanation of our data is that motivated by a priori theoretical predictions. Greene's commentary does reaffirm that alternative opinions are valid and that as is always the case, further research is necessary.

Abbreviations

TI:

transitive inference

References

  1. Frank MJ, Rudy JW, Levy WB, O'Reilly RC: When Logic Fails: Implicit Transitive Inference in Humans. Mem Cognit. 2005, 33: 742-50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. von Fersen L, Wynne CDL, Delius JD, Staddon JER: Transitive inference in pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Proc. 1991, 17: 334-341. 10.1037/0097-7403.17.3.334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Wynne CD: A minimal model of transitive inference. Models of Action. Edited by: Wynne CD, Staddon JE. 1998, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 269-307.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Frank MJ, Rudy JW, O'Reilly RC: Transitivity, flexibility, conjunctive representations and the hippocampus: II. A Computational analysis. Hippocampus. 2003, 13: 341-54. 10.1002/hipo.10084.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Siemann M, Delius JD: Algebraic learning and neural network models for transitive and non-transitive responding. Eur J Cogn Psychol. 1998, 10: 307-334. 10.1080/713752279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Frank MJ: Dynamic dopamine modulation in the basal ganglia: A neurocomputational account of cognitive deficits in medicated and non-medicated Parkinsonism. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005, 17: 51-72. 10.1162/0898929052880093.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Frank MJ, Seeberger LC, O'Reilly RC: By carrot or by stick: Cognitive reinforcement learning in Parkinsonism. Science. 2004, 306: 1940-3. 10.1126/science.1102941.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Poldrack RA, Packard MG: Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies. Neuropsychologia. 2003, 41: 245-251. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00157-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Atallah HE, Frank MJ, O'Reilly RC: Hippocampus cortex and basal ganglia: insights from computational models of complementary learning systems. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004, 82 (3): 253-267. 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.06.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Rovira C, Ben-Ari Y: Developmental study of benzodiazepene effects on monosynaptic GABAA-mediated IPSPs of rat hippocampal neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1993, 70 (3): 1076-1085.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Poncer JC, Durr R, Gahwiler BH, Thompson SM: Modulation of synaptic GABAA receptor function by benzodiazepenes in area CA3 of rat hippocampal slice cultures. Neuropharmacology. 1996, 35: 1169-79. 10.1016/S0028-3908(96)00055-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Kristiansen U, Lambert JD: Benzodiazepene and barbituate ligands modulate responses of cultured hippocampal neurones to the GABAA receptor partial agonist, 4-PIOL. Neuropharmacology. 1996, 35: 1181-91. 10.1016/S0028-3908(96)00070-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Kobayashi S, Fujito Y, Matsuyama K, Aoki M: Differential effects of midazolam on inhibitory postsynaptic currents in CA1 pyramidal cells and dentate gyrus granule cells of rat hippocampal slices. Brain Res. 2004, 1003: 176-82. 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.01.015.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Imperato A, Dazzi L, Obinu MC, Gessa GL, Biggio G: Inhibition of hippocampal acetylcholine release by benzodiazepines: antagonism by flumazenil. Eur J Pharmacol. 1993, 238: 135-137. 10.1016/0014-2999(93)90518-M.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Reinsel RA, Veselis RA, Dnistrian AM, Feshchenko VA, Beattle BJ, Duff MR: Midazolam decreases cerebral blood flow in the left prefrontal cortex in a dose-dependent fashion. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2000, 3: 117-27. 10.1017/S1461145700001814.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Merritt P, Hirshman E, Zamani S, Hsu J, Berrigan M: Episodic representations support early semantic learning: evidence from midazolam induced amnesia. Brain Cogn. 2006, 61 (2): 219-223. 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.12.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Malmberg KJ, Zeelenberg R, Shiffrin RM: Turning up the noise or turning down the volume? On the nature of the impairment of episodic recognition memory by midazolam. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2004, 30 (2): 540-549. 10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.540.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Evans MS, Viola-McCabe KE: Midazolam inhibits long-term potentiation through modulation of GABA-A receptors. Neuropharmacology. 1996, 35: 347-57. 10.1016/0028-3908(95)00182-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Frank MJ, OReilly RC, Curran T: When memory fails, intuition reigns: Midazolam enhances implicit inference in humans. Psychol Sci. 2006, 17: 700-707. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01769.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Poldrack RA, Clark J, PareBlagoev EJ, Shohamy D, Moyano JC, Myers C, Gluck MA: Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature. 2001, 414: 546-549. 10.1038/35107080.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Shohamy D, Myers CE, Kalanithi J, Gluck MA: Basal ganglia and dopamine contributions to probabilistic category learning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007

    Google Scholar 

  22. Greene AJ: Implicit transitive inference and the human hippocampus: does intravenous midazolam function as a reversible hippocampal lesion?. Behav Brain Funct. 2007, 3: 51-10.1186/1744-9081-3-51.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Nagode JC, Pardo JV: Human hippocampal activation during transitive inference. Neuroreport. 2002, 13: 939-944. 10.1097/00001756-200205240-00008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Acuna BD, Eliassen JC, Donoghue JP, Sanes JN: Frontal and Parietal Lobe Activation during Transitive Inference in Humans. Cereb Cortex. 2002, 12: 1312-1321. 10.1093/cercor/12.12.1312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Gerfen CR, Wilson C: The basal ganglia. Handbook of chemical neuroanatomy. Integrated systems of the CNS. Edited by: Swanson L, Bjorkland A, Hokfelt T. 1996, Amsterdam: Elsevier, 12: 371-468.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Rattan AK, Tejwani GA: Effect of chronic treatment with morphine, midazolam and both together on dynorphin(1–13) levels in the rat. Brain Res. 1997, 754 (1–2): 239-244. 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00084-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Tejwani GA, Rattan AK: Met-enkephalin alteration in the rat during chronic injection of morphine and/or midazolam. Brain Res. 1997, 775 (1–2): 119-126. 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00875-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Gil E, Colado I, Lopez F, Fernandez-Briera A, Fernandez-Lopez A, Calvo P: Effects of chronic treatment with ethanol and withdrawal of ethanol on levels of dopamine, 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid and homovanillic acid in the striatum of the rat. Influence of benzodiazepines, barbiturate and somatostatin. Neuropharmacology. 1992, 31 (11): 1151-1156. 10.1016/0028-3908(92)90011-D.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Foerde K, Knowlton BJ, Poldrack RA: Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006, 103 (31): 11778-11783. 10.1073/pnas.0602659103.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Montpied P, Martin BM, Cottingham SL, Stubblefield BK, Ginns EI, Paul SM: Regional distribution of the GABAA/benzodiazepene receptor (alpha subunit) mRNA in rat brain. J Neurochem. 1988, 51: 1651-4. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1988.tb01137.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Curran T, DeBuse C, Woroch B, Hirshman E: Combined pharmacological and electrophysiological dissociation of familiarity and recollection. J Neurosci. 2006, 26 (7): 1979-1985. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5370-05.2006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Smith C, Squire LR: Declarative memory, awareness, and transitive inference. J Neurosci. 2005, 25 (44): 10138-10146. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2731-05.2005.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Driscoll I, Sutherland RJ, Prusky GT, Rudy JW: Damage to the Hippocampal Formation Does Not Disrupt Representational Flexibility as Measured by a Novelty Transfer Test. Behav Neurosci. 2004, 118 (6): 1427-1432. 10.1037/0735-7044.118.6.1427.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Bayley PJ, Frascino JC, Squire LR: Robust habit learning in the absence of awareness and independent of the medial temporal lobe. Nature. 2005, 436: 550-3. 10.1038/nature03857.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Hirshman E, Passannante A, Arndt J: Midazolam Amnesia and Conceptual Processing in Implicit Memory. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2001, 130: 453-465. 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.453.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Arndt J, Passannante A, Hirshman E: The effect of midazolam on implicit and explicit memory in category exemplar production and category cued recall. Memory. 2004, 12: 158-73. 10.1080/09658210244000270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Thomas-Anterion C, Koenig O, Navez M, Laurent B: Midazolam effects on implicit and explicit memory processes in healthy subjects. Psychopharmacology. 1999, 145: 139-43. 10.1007/s002130051042.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Hirshman E, Fisher J, Henthorn T, Arndt J, Passannante A: Midazolam amnesia and dual-process models of the word-frequency mirror effect. J Mem Lang. 2002, 47: 499-516. 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00017-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Park H, Quinlan J, Thornton E, Reder LM: The effect of midazolam on visual search: Implications for understanding amnesia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004, 101: 17879-83. 10.1073/pnas.0408075101.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Giovanello KS, Schnyer DM, Verfaeilie M: A critical role for the anterior hippocampus in relational memory: evidence from an fMRI study comparing associative and item recognition. Hippocampus. 2004, 14: 5-8. 10.1002/hipo.10182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Greene AJ, Gross WL, Elsinger CL, Rao SM: An FMRI analysis of the human hippocampus: inference, context, and task awareness. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006, 18 (7): 1156-1173. 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1156.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael J Frank.

Additional information

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

M.J.F., R.C.O. and T.C wrote the paper.

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Frank, M.J., O'Reilly, R.C. & Curran, T. Midazolam, hippocampal function, and transitive inference: Reply to Greene. Behav Brain Funct 4, 5 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-4-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-4-5

Keywords