Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Emergent modular neural control drives coordinated motor actions

Abstract

A remarkable feature of motor control is the ability to coordinate movements across distinct body parts into a consistent, skilled action. To reach and grasp an object, ‘gross’ arm and ‘fine’ dexterous movements must be coordinated as a single action. How the nervous system achieves this coordination is currently unknown. One possibility is that, with training, gross and fine movements are co-optimized to produce a coordinated action; alternatively, gross and fine movements may be modularly refined to function together. To address this question, we recorded neural activity in the primary motor cortex and dorsolateral striatum during reach-to-grasp skill learning in rats. During learning, the refinement of fine and gross movements was behaviorally and neurally dissociable. Furthermore, inactivation of the primary motor cortex and dorsolateral striatum had distinct effects on skilled fine and gross movements. Our results indicate that skilled movement coordination is achieved through emergent modular neural control.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Refinement of skilled fine and gross movements is dissociable during reach-to-grasp skill learning.
Fig. 2: Coordinated movement-related activity emerges across M1 and DLS during skill learning.
Fig. 3: Coordinated spiking activity emerges across M1 and DLS during skill learning.
Fig. 4: Coordinated M1 and DLS activity is specifically linked to skilled gross movements.
Fig. 5: M1 and DLS inactivation have differential effects on skilled fine and gross movements.
Fig. 6: DLS inactivation decreases movement-related low-frequency M1 activity.
Fig. 7: Persistent disruption of skilled fine movements after M1 lesion.
Fig. 8: Skilled fine movement representation in M1.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data used for the analyses that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Code availability

The code used for the analyses that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

References

  1. Wang, X. et al. Deconstruction of corticospinal circuits for goal-directed motor skills. Cell 171, 440–455.e14 (2017).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Whishaw, I. Q. An endpoint, descriptive, and kinematic comparison of skilled reaching in mice (Mus musculus) with rats (Rattus norvegicus). Behav. Brain Res. 78, 101–111 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Diedrichsen, J., Shadmehr, R. & Ivry, R. B. The coordination of movement: optimal feedback control and beyond. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 31–39 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Todorov, E. & Jordan, M. I. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 1226–1235 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Kargo, W. J. & Nitz, D. A. Improvements in the signal-to-noise ratio of motor cortex cells distinguish early versus late phases of motor skill learning. J. Neurosci. 24, 5560–5569 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Li, Q. et al. Refinement of learned skilled movement representation in motor cortex deep output layer. Nat. Commun. 8, 15834 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Ramanathan, D. S., Gulati, T. & Ganguly, K. Sleep-dependent reactivation of ensembles in motor cortex promotes skill consolidation. PLoS Biol. 13, e1002263 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Santos, F. J., Oliveira, R. F., Jin, X. & Costa, R. M. Corticostriatal dynamics encode the refinement of specific behavioral variability during skill learning. eLife 4, e09423 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Kawai, R. et al. Motor cortex is required for learning but not for executing a motor skill. Neuron 86, 800–812 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Costa, R. M., Cohen, D. & Nicolelis, M. A. L. Differential corticostriatal plasticity during fast and slow motor skill learning in mice. Curr. Biol. 14, 1124–1134 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Kupferschmidt, D. A., Juczewski, K., Cui, G., Johnson, K. A. & Lovinger, D. M. Parallel, but dissociable, processing in discrete corticostriatal inputs encodes skill learning. Neuron 96, 476–489.e5 (2017).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Yin, H. H. et al. Dynamic reorganization of striatal circuits during the acquisition and consolidation of a skill. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 333–341 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Rueda-Orozco, P. E. & Robbe, D. The striatum multiplexes contextual and kinematic information to constrain motor habits execution. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 453–460 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Hintiryan, H. et al. The mouse cortico-striatal projectome. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 1100–1114 (2016).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Wong, C. C., Ramanathan, D. S., Gulati, T., Won, S. J. & Ganguly, K. An automated behavioral box to assess forelimb function in rats. J. Neurosci. Methods 246, 30–37 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Lalla, L., Rueda Orozco, P. E., Jurado-Parras, M. T., Brovelli, A. & Robbe, D. Local or not local: investigating the nature of striatal theta oscillations in behaving rats. eNeuro 4, ENEURO.0128-17.2017 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Hall, T. M., de Carvalho, F. & Jackson, A. A common structure underlies low-frequency cortical dynamics in movement, sleep, and sedation. Neuron 83, 1185–1199 (2014).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Riehle, A., Wirtssohn, S., Grün, S. & Brochier, T. Mapping the spatio-temporal structure of motor cortical LFP and spiking activities during reach-to-grasp movements. Front. Neural Circuits 7, 48 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Turner, R. S. & Desmurget, M. Basal ganglia contributions to motor control: a vigorous tutor. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 704–716 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Mazzoni, P., Hristova, A. & Krakauer, J. W. Why don’t we move faster? Parkinson’s disease, movement vigor, and implicit motivation. J. Neurosci. 27, 7105–7116 (2007).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Panigrahi, B. et al. Dopamine is required for the neural representation and control of movement vigor. Cell 162, 1418–1430 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Dudman, J. T. & Krakauer, J. W. The basal ganglia: from motor commands to the control of vigor. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 37, 158–166 (2016).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Yttri, E. A. & Dudman, J. T. A proposed circuit computation in basal ganglia: history-dependent gain. Mov. Disord. 33, 704–716 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Whishaw, I. Q., Zeeb, F., Erickson, C. & McDonald, R. J. Neurotoxic lesions of the caudate-putamen on a reaching for food task in the rat: acute sensorimotor neglect and chronic qualitative motor impairment follow lateral lesions and improved success follows medial lesions. Neuroscience 146, 86–97 (2007).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Yu, B. M. et al. Gaussian-process factor analysis for low-dimensional single-trial analysis of neural population activity. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 614–635 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Peters, A. J., Liu, H. & Komiyama, T. Learning in the rodent motor cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 77–97 (2017).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Alaverdashvili, M. & Whishaw, I. Q. Motor cortex stroke impairs individual digit movement in skilled reaching by the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 311–322 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Guo, J. Z. et al. Cortex commands the performance of skilled movement. eLife 4, e10774 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Lemon, R. N. Descending pathways in motor control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 195–218 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Miri, A. et al. Behaviorally selective engagement of short-latency effector pathways by motor cortex. Neuron 95, 683–696.e11 (2017).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Ueno, M. et al. Corticospinal circuits from the sensory and motor cortices differentially regulate skilled movements through distinct spinal interneurons. Cell Rep. 23, 1286–1300.e7 (2018).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Lawrence, D. G. & Kuypers, H. G. The functional organization of the motor system in the monkey. I. The effects of bilateral pyramidal lesions. Brain 91, 1–14 (1968).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Lawrence, D. G. & Kuypers, H. G. The functional organization of the motor system in the monkey. II. The effects of lesions of the descending brain-stem pathways. Brain 91, 15–36 (1968).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Otchy, T. M. et al. Acute off-target effects of neural circuit manipulations. Nature 528, 358–363 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Díaz-Hernández, E. et al. The thalamostriatal projections contribute to the initiation and execution of a sequence of movements. Neuron 100, 739–752.e5 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Murray, J. M. & Escola, G. S. Learning multiple variable-speed sequences in striatum via cortical tutoring. eLife 6, e26084 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Dudman, J. T. & Gerfen, C. R. in The Rat Nervous System (ed. Paxinos, G.) Ch. 17, 391–440 (Academic Press, 2015).

  38. Oldenburg, I. A. & Sabatini, B. L. Antagonistic but not symmetric regulation of primary motor cortex by basal ganglia direct and indirect pathways. Neuron 86, 1174–1181 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Koralek, A. C., Jin, X., Long, J. D. II, Costa, R. M. & Carmena, J. M. Corticostriatal plasticity is necessary for learning intentional neuroprosthetic skills. Nature 483, 331–335 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. DeLong, M. R., Crutcher, M. D. & Georgopoulos, A. P. Primate globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus: functional organization. J. Neurophysiol. 53, 530–543 (1985).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Baker, K. B. et al. Somatotopic organization in the internal segment of the globus pallidus in Parkinson’s disease. Exp. Neurol. 222, 219–225 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Shmuelof, L., Krakauer, J. W. & Mazzoni, P. How is a motor skill learned? Change and invariance at the levels of task success and trajectory control. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 578–594 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Hikosaka, O., Yamamoto, S., Yasuda, M. & Kim, H. F. Why skill matters. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 434–441 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Park, S.-W., Marino, H., Charles, S. K., Sternad, D. & Hogan, N. Moving slowly is hard for humans: limitations of dynamic primitives. J. Neurophysiol. 118, 69–83 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Harris, A. Z. & Gordon, J. A. Long-range neural synchrony in behavior. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 38, 171–194 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Fries, P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 474–480 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Fries, P. Rhythms for cognition: communication through coherence. Neuron 88, 220–235 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Ramanathan, D. S. et al. Low-frequency cortical activity is a neuromodulatory target that tracks recovery after stroke. Nat. Med. 24, 1257–1267 (2018).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Churchland, M. M. et al. Neural population dynamics during reaching. Nature 487, 51–56 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Sussillo, D., Churchland, M. M., Kaufman, M. T. & Shenoy, K. V. A neural network that finds a naturalistic solution for the production of muscle activity. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1025–1033 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Delorme, A. & Makeig, S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Bokil, H., Andrews, P., Kulkarni, J. E., Mehta, S. & Mitra, P. P. Chronux: a platform for analyzing neural signals. J. Neurosci. Methods 192, 146–151 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Cowley, B. R. et al. DataHigh: graphical user interface for visualizing and interacting with high-dimensional neural activity. J. Neural Eng. 10, 066012 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Aarts, E., Verhage, M., Veenvliet, J. V., Dolan, C. V. & van der Sluis, S. A solution to dependency: using multilevel analysis to accommodate nested data. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 491–496 (2014).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Research was supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs, Veterans Health Administration (VA Merit no. 1I01RX001640 to K.G.; Career Development Award no. 7IK2BX003308 to D.S.R.), the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (grant nos. 5K02NS093014 and R01MH111871 to K.G.), the American Heart & Stroke Association (predoctoral fellowship no. 17PRE33410530/2016 to S.M.L.), A*STAR Singapore (fellowship to L.G.) and a Career Award for Medical Scientists from the Burroughs Wellcome Fund to D.S.R. (no. 1015644). and to K.G. (no 1009855).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

S.M.L., D.S.R. and K.G. designed the experiments. S.M.L. and D.S.R. carried out the electrophysiology experiments. S.M.L. carried out the acute inactivation experiments. S.M.L. and L.G. carried out the chronic lesion experiments. S.M.L. and S.J.W. performed the histology. S.M.L. carried out the analyses. S.M.L. and K.G. wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karunesh Ganguly.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Journal peer review information: Nature Neuroscience thanks Xin Jin, David Robbe, and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Fig. 1 Localization of electrodes.

a. Illustration of M1 and DLS recording sites. b. Quantification of electrolytic lesion sites marking electrode locations for four learning animals.

Supplementary Fig. 2 Corticostriatal projections.

a. Anterograde labeling of projections from M1 showing projections to the DLS. Representative image from four tracing experiments (n = 4 animals). b. Retrograde labeling of projections to the DLS showing strong inputs from cortex, including M1. Representative image from four tracing experiments (n = 4 animals).

Supplementary Fig. 3 Gross movement metrics and success rate do not covary on days five through eight of reach-to-grasp skill learning.

a. Scatterplots of mean movement duration, sub-movement timing variability and success rate for non-overlapping ten trial bins between days five and eight of training across animals (n = 123 bins across 4 animals; values are z-scored within each animal; Pearson’s r).

Supplementary Fig. 4 M1 and DLS LFP channel pairs with high coherence have non-zero phase lag.

a. Example mean movement onset-locked LFP signals from M1 and DLS (top) and extracted phase (bottom). b. Histogram of phase difference between M1 and DLS LFP signals from 250ms before movement onset to 500ms after movement onset for example M1-DLS LFP pair (grey shaded region in a). c. Peak of phase difference histogram (as in b) for all high coherence (coherence value >0.2) pairs of M1-DLS channels on day eight.

Supplementary Fig. 5 LFP power and coherence increase for behaviorally-matched trials early and late in reach-to-grasp skill learning.

a. Differences in M1 3-6Hz LFP power, DLS 3-6Hz LFP power, and M1-DLS 3-6Hz LFP coherence between behaviorally-matched ‘fast’ trials with a duration between 200-400ms on days one and two (Early Days) and days seven and eight (Late Days). Grey lines represent mean values from individual animals (n = 4 animals) and black lines represent mean ± s.e.m. across animals. P values from mixed-effects models. b. Same as a for behaviorally-matched high forelimb trajectory correlation trials (individual trial trajectory with correlation value >0.9 to mean session trajectory).

Supplementary Fig. 6 Peak frequency of M1-DLS LFP coherence covaries with movement duration on day eight.

a. Example mean M1 and DLS LFP signals for slowest third of trials (top) and fastest third of trials (bottom) on day eight for example animal. b. Difference in 3-6Hz M1-DLS LFP coherence between slowest third of trials (Slow Trials) and fastest third of trials (Fast Trials; normalized to mean coherence for slowest third of trials) on day one and day eight (n = 4 animals). Grey lines represent mean values from individual animals and black lines represent mean ± s.e.m. across animals. P values from mixed-effects models. c. Difference in peak M1-DLS frequency between 3-6Hz for slowest third of trials and fastest third of trials on day one and day eight (n = 4 animals). Grey lines represent mean values from individual animals and black lines represent mean ± s.e.m. across animals. P values from mixed-effects models.

Supplementary Fig. 7 Details of M1 and DLS spiking activity.

a. Example waveforms from array spanning M1 and DLS for one session. b. Total recorded units and total task-related units across days in M1. c. Total recorded units and total task-related units across days in DLS. d. Histogram of firing rates for all units across all sessions, vertical lines denote median for each region. e. PETHs of all M1 units time-locked to movement onset on day one (left) and day eight (right). f. PETHs of all DLS units time-locked to movement onset on day one (left) and day eight (right). g. M1 unit activity during reaching. h. DLS unit activity during reaching. i. PETHs of all phase-locked and not phase-locked M1 units on day eight. j. PETHs of all phase-locked and not phase-locked DLS units on day eight.

Supplementary Fig. 8 Body posture during reaching before and after DLS inactivation.

a. Example of top camera view and quantification of body axis/posture during reaching. b. Example body posture for all trials at the time of movement onset before DLS muscimol infusion (Baseline Session) and after DLS muscimol infusion (DLS Inactivation Session). Grey lines represent individual trials and red line represents mean position across session. c. Difference in posture variability and lateral bias for trials before DLS muscimol infusion and after DLS muscimol infusion (n = 3 sessions across 3 animals). Grey lines represent mean values from individual animals and black lines represent mean ± s.e.m. across animals. P values from mixed-effects models.

Supplementary Fig. 9 Localization and behavioral effects of excitotoxic DLS lesions.

a. DLS excitotoxic lesion localization (immunolabeling: DAPI in blue; NeuN in red). b. Differences in reach duration, sub-movement timing variability, and success rate between trials before DLS lesion (Pre Lesion) and trials two weeks after DLS excitotoxic lesion (Post Lesion). Grey lines represent mean values from individual animals (n = 3 animals) and black lines represent mean ± s.e.m. across animals. P values from mixed-effects models.

Supplementary Fig. 10 Localization of muscimol infusion.

a. Localization of muscimol infusion into DLS. Representative image from three localizations (n = 3 animals).

Supplementary Fig. 11 Localization of M1 photothrombotic lesion.

a. Example M1 photothrombotic lesion. Representative image from five localizations (n = 5 animals).

Supplementary Fig. 12 Increased GPFA neural trajectory consistency during reach-to-grasp skill learning.

a. GPFA neural trajectories for M1 (top) and DLS (bottom) on day one and day eight from example animal. b. Difference in consistency of GPFA trajectories between early days (days 1–4) and late days (days 5–8) in M1 (top) and DLS (bottom). Grey dots represent individual sessions (Early Days: n = 12 sessions across 4 animals; Late Days: n = 10 sessions across 4 animals) and black lines represent mean ± s.e.m. across sessions.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Figs. 1–12.

Reporting Summary

Supplementary Video 1

Video of fine movements involved in grasping the pellet during trials before DLS lesion and 2 weeks post-DLS lesion.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lemke, S.M., Ramanathan, D.S., Guo, L. et al. Emergent modular neural control drives coordinated motor actions. Nat Neurosci 22, 1122–1131 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0407-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0407-2

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing