Introduction

Studying sex differences in various diseases and disorders has become increasingly important as there are sex differences not only in the prevalence of certain diseases but also manifestation of the disease and in treatment efficacy [1,2,3,4]. We focus on the importance of studying sex differences in cognition with an eye toward a better understanding of the notable sex differences in the prevalence and severity of memory symptoms in a number of neurological diseases that favor either sex. This research is important as cognitive deficits with neurological disease show significant sex differences.

In both Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and depression, women show a greater prevalence of disease and steeper declines in memory or more severe cognitive symptoms in both disorders [1,2,3, 5]. On the other hand, men with schizophrenia or Parkinson’s Disease show greater cognitive impairments compared to women with these same diseases [6,7,8,9,10,11]. Although underlying biological mechanisms of these sex differences in cognitive impairment with neurological disease are not completely elucidated, there are sex and sex hormones differences in hippocampus structure and plasticity that may contribute to the greater vulnerability within each sex by disease type. Understanding sex differences in the contribution of hippocampal plasticity to cognition can give us important clues on the underlying mechanisms of disorders that involve disruptions to cognition and hippocampal integrity in an effort to lead to better treatments for patients with such disorders.

In this review, we focus on sex differences in hippocampal cognition with an emphasis on understanding how sex differences in hippocampal adult neurogenesis may contribute to cognition. However, while we discuss sex differences in behavior and sex differences in the structure and properties of the hippocampus, we do not mean to suggest that sex differences in structure of the hippocampus are the cause of sex differences in behavior. It is equally possible that sex differences in structure may cause or prevent sex differences in behavior [12]. Certainly, there are many types of sex differences noted, including sex differences seen in mechanism but no sex differences seen in behavior [13]. A recurring theme throughout this review though will be that we continue to be bereft of a full understanding of how males and females differ in even a basic understanding of hippocampal structure, connectivity, receptor dynamics, and function.

We concentrate on the hippocampus as it is a highly plastic structure due in part to the presence of adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus [14,15,16], fluctuations in dendritic spine/synapse density, dendritic arbourization [17], and electrophysiological plasticity with long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) [18, 19]. This plasticity is modified in a sex dependent manner either basally or may manifest after exposure to stress or other experiences. The circuitry (reviewed in [20]), and receptor characteristics of the hippocampus are well known, but as mentioned above, an important caveat is that we know little of how these characteristics may differ between males and females.

Importantly, sex differences in the hippocampus, at least in part, relate to steroid hormone manipulations and it is important to acknowledge that the hippocampus contains sex hormone receptors such as androgen receptors (AR), and estrogen receptors (ER)- α, β and G-coupled protein receptor (GPER). The relative receptor density differs by sex in a region-specific way as the CA3 and CA4 regions of adult female rats contain a greater amount of ER-β compared to male rats [21] whereas there are greater levels of AR in male compared to female rats, dependent on estrous cycle phase in the CA1 and dentate gyrus [22]. Furthermore, there are notable alterations in females in the distribution of ERα and β and progesterone receptors across the estrous cycle [23]. While beyond the scope of this review, there are notable sex differences in AR and ER distribution in the hippocampus during development [24]. Thus, it is important to note that there are sex, age, and estrous cycle phase differences in the level of mRNA and protein expression of these hormone receptors in the hippocampus.

Many of the sex differences in the hippocampus are also seen following exposure to stress, and the hippocampus contains a high concentration of glucocorticoid and mineralcorticoid receptors (MR) compared to other brain regions, which makes the hippocampus more vulnerable to chronic glucocorticoid exposure via stress [25,26,27]. Although to our knowledge no studies have directly compared the sexes, there are some hints in the literature of sex differences in glucocorticoid receptor (GR) are that women show a statistically significant reduction in GR expression in the hippocampus with age, while men show only a trend for a reduction [28]. However, it is important to note that this study had a relatively small sample size of post-mortem tissue (12 males, 14 females). Sex differences in the pattern of MR and potential changes in GR expression are seen during development in rodents [29]. Unfortunately, the work to categorize sex differences in receptor density characteristics of steroid hormone receptors and other receptors within the hippocampus is not yet complete, and we need to acknowledge that more differences in receptor density and characteristics likely exist between the sexes.

In this review, we note the accumulated evidence for sex differences in the morphology, connectivity, and physiology of the hippocampus in humans and in rodents, then, we discuss sex differences in the hippocampal adult neurogenesis focusing on basal differences and after stress and learning. Lastly, we discuss sex differences in hippocampus-dependent spatial learning and fear conditioning with a focus on strategy use and when sex differences in these tasks are expressed.

Sex differences in the morphology, activation, and connectivity of the hippocampus: findings from human studies

Men typically are reported to have larger volumes of both the left and right hippocampus than women [30] but once total brain volume or intracranial volume is used as a correction factor, there is no longer a significant sex difference [31]. Recent studies show no sex differences in hippocampal volume across development [32]. One caveat in these studies is that a number of factors that influence hippocampal volume are not often documented including early adversity (reductions are often seen in men but not in women: [33]), phase of menstrual cycle [34], parity status [35], hormone therapy [36], menopausal status [37], genotype [38], and testosterone levels in men [39]. Researchers are cautioned that these variables need to be considered to obtain an accurate understanding of sex differences in the volume of the hippocampus.

Whereas whole hippocampal volume may not show a sex difference, regional differences within the hippocampus and connectivity to the hippocampus do exist between the sexes [40, 41]. For example, women have larger posterior hippocampus than men [40], with differences between the sexes in structural covariance and functional connectivity, indicating differences in connectivity. Studies in humans indicate sex differences in functional brain connectivity [42,43,44,45]. Women have more inter-hemispheric connections compared to men, while men have stronger intra-hemispheric connection compared to women [42,43,44]. Furthermore, Filippi et al. [46] showed women have greater intra-connectivity within the temporal lobe compared to men. One study has argued that because there is no true distinct dichotomy between the sexes in terms of MRI measures of volume and connectivity that this suggest there is no true ‘male’ or ‘female’ typical brain [45]. But other researchers have argued that the lack of a dichotomy (an extreme form of sex difference) does not preclude the idea that there are patterns of connectivity, and structural differences on a continuum that relate to a male-typical or female-typical brain (see [47, 48] for more discussion). fMRI studies in language processing show women have greater bilateral activation of BOLD in the temporal and frontal lobes in comparison to men [49, 50]. Furthermore, women have greater activation in regions other than the hippocampus compared to men during a virtual water maze task, such as prefrontal cortex [51, 52]. This is particularly intriguing as it matches animal data indicating a greater involvement of the prefrontal cortex in female rats compared to male rats during the Morris water maze task [53]. These types of studies are important to consider when examining activation patterns without regard to stratifying the data by sex.

Briefly, there are sex differences in neurodegeneration noted in AD. The CA1 region of the hippocampus is one of the first sites of damage in AD [54]. Atrophy of the CA1 region in AD and other forms of dementia is associated with memory loss and spatial disorientation during early stages of the disease [55,56,57]. Sex differences exist in the areas that correlated with AD progress and neuronal density with age in the hippocampus [58]. Negative associations were seen in Alzheimer’s patients between Braak stage and CA1 neuronal density in women but not men, while these same associations were seen in the CA3 neuronal density in men but not women [58]. In addition, elevated Braak-stage resulted in decreased in CA1 neuronal density in women, but not in men, whereas elevated Braak stage resulted in decreased CA3 neuronal density in men but not in women. Interestingly, total tau, as well as, Aβ-42 levels in women were associated with a steeper decline in cognition and volume of the hippocampus compared to men [59] with more pronounced effects in the prodromal condition of mild cognitive impairment (MCI). In addition, in MCI patients with a higher risk to develop AD, women were found to show greater cognitive decline and more total tau, Aβ-42 ratios that were exacerbated by the presence of the apolipoprotein e4 allele [60]. Furthermore, studies suggest that women are more likely to show associations of progression to AD with changes in hippocampal volume and amyloid burden, while in men white matter hyperintensities are more related to disease progression [59, 61, 62]. This work is an important step to determine whether sex differences can aid in our understanding of disease progression and possible treatment strategies that may need to differ by sex.

Sex differences in morphology and electrophysiological properties of hippocampal neurons: findings from rodent studies

Sex differences exist in the morphology and electrophysiological properties of hippocampal neurons. However, researchers need to be aware that estrous cycle significantly influences a number of parameters in which sex differences are seen such as cell proliferation [63, 64], hippocampal volume [65], LTP and LTD [66, 67], CA1 apical spine density [68], and hippocampal AMPA receptor stoichiometry [69]. Thus, if estrous phase is not taken into consideration this may affect magnitude and appearance of sex differences in these measures. Studies have reported that there are sex differences in the morphology of granule neurons and CA3 pyramidal neurons [70,71,72]. Juraska et al. [72] found that male rats had greater dendritic intersections in granule neurons of the dentate gyrus compared to female rats. Galea et al. [73] indicated that female rats had greater branch points in the basal dendrites of CA3 pyramidal neurons compared to male rats and Gould et al. [74] found that females had more primary dendrites in CA3 than males, and that males had more thorny excrescences than females. Another study found more dendritic spines in the CA3 pyramidal neurons in females than in males although the sexes were not directly compared statistically in that study [70]. These results suggest basal sex differences in the morphology of granule and pyramidal neurons in the rat hippocampus (see Table 1).

Table 1 Basal sex differences in hippocampal plasticity

Gonadal hormone manipulations, estrous cycle phase and exposure to stress can also reveal sex and hormone differences in hippocampal morphology [68, 73, 75]. For example, proestrous female rats have greater apical dendritic spine density in CA1 pyramidal neurons compared to males and diestrous females [68]. Acute stress increases apical dendritic CA1 spine density in males but decreases it in females, dependent on estrous cycle phase [75]. Many of the structural differences in the dentate gyrus and CA3 regions show sex differences in response to gonadectomy, with mossy fibers innervations increased in castrated males but not in ovariectomized females [70]. In addition, castrated males have longer dendrites and larger mossy fibers, while ovariectomy decreases dendritic spine density in the CA3 region [70]. However, this study did not directly compare the effects of gonadectomy between the sexes statistically. Chronic stress decreases apical dendritic complexity in male rats, and basal dendritic complexity in female rats in the CA3 region of hippocampus ([73]; see Fig. 1). In the dentate gyrus, granule neurons have greater dendritic intersections in single-housed males than single-housed females, whereas females raised in an enriched environment have larger dendritic trees compared to males raised in an enriched environmental [76]. Intriguingly, much like work in humans, early life adversity reduces hippocampal volume, neurogenesis and impairs spatial memory in male rodents (reviewed in [77]) but separate studies indicate that females show resilience after early life adversity with little effect on the hippocampus [78]. Even though these studies did not directly examine sex differences, a number of studies have corroborated these findings (reviewed in [77]). There are sex differences in the manifestation of early life adversity on the hippocampus and amygdala [79] with males, but not females, showing more morphological changes after various forms of early life stress [79]. These results collectively suggest that sex differences in the morphology of hippocampal subregions exist under basal conditions and that other sex differences are unveiled after environmental or gonadal hormone perturbations (see Table 2).

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Photomicrograph of a coronal section of a rodent hippocampus. b Acute predator odor stress (TMT) decreases cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus of male rats, but not in female rats ([110]; modified from original with permission). c Chronic restraint stress for 21 days decreases apical dendritic complexity in male rats, the same stress decreases basal dendritic complexity in female rats in the CA3 region of hippocampus [73]. Figures are reprinted with permission from Falconer and Galea [110] and Galea et al. [73]. d Neural stem cell marker Nestin-expressing cells (red) in the dentate gyrus merged with DAPI (blue). e Ki67-expressing cells (green) in the dentate gyrus merged with DAPI (blue). f Doublecortin-expressing cells (red) in the dentate gyrus. g Double staining of NeuN (green) and BrdU (red) in the dentate gyrus 3 weeks after BrdU injection. DCX doublecortin, DG dentate gyrus, TMT trimethylthiazoline (main component of fox feces), BrdU bromodeoxyuridine

Table 2 Sex differences in hippocampal plasticity revealed after gonadectomy, stress, or learning

In addition to the morphological differences, there are sex differences in LTP in the hippocampus. Males exhibit larger early and late-LTP compared to females in the dentate gyrus, CA3 and CA1 regions [80,81,82,83]. Interestingly, proestrous females showed greater magnitude of early-LTP compared to diestrous females through the perforant path [84]. It is important to note however, that during proestrus, seizure threshold is also decreased [85, 86] and as such, it becomes more difficult to find an appropriate tetanus [66], indicating greater excitability in the female hippocampus during proestrus. Furthermore, the composition of AMPA/NMDA receptors of CA1 pyramidal neurons is different between males and females, as females show greater AMPA/NMDA ratio than males [81, 84]. Oberlander and Woolley [87] demonstrated that estradiol enhances presynaptic and postsynaptic potentials in the CA1 pyramidal neurons in both males and females. However, postsynaptic sensitivity of male CA1 pyramidal neurons is potentiated by ERβ while female CA1 pyramidal neurons are potentiated by GPER1 [87]. In addition to sex differences in the CA1 region of hippocampus, there are sex differences in the neural plasticity at DG-CA3 synapses (see [88] for review). In short, mossy fibers evoke larger population spikes in CA3 pyramidal neurons in females during proestrus and estrus relative to males, while male mossy fibers have stronger synaptic connections to CA3 neurons than females [89, 90]. However, these two studies did not compare males and female directly. These anatomical and electrophysiological findings suggest that intrahippocampal circuitry and ER mechanisms are differently organized between males and females. Furthermore, the female hippocampus is dynamic across the estrous cycle and through reproductive experience (for review, see [91]). Further research is needed to elucidate the underlying cellular mechanisms of sex differences in hippocampal physiology under basal and environmental perturbations.

Sex differences in adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus

Adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus plays important roles for some forms of learning and the stress response [92,93,94]. For instance, reduction of hippocampal adult neurogenesis leads to an impairment in trace eye blink conditioning [95], pattern separation [94], contextual fear conditioning [96, 97], proactive interference and forgetting [98,99,100], long-term, but not short-term, spatial memory [101], and reversal learning [102, 103], although sex differences were not analyzed in these studies. However, one study has noted that the ablation of adult neurogenesis did not influence memory for novel object placement in either male or female rats [104]. It is not known whether there are sex differences in the functional outcome of reducing adult neurogenesis but there are differences in neurogenesis levels after learning and stress that are discussed below.

Net increases or decreases in the amount of neurogenesis are determined by changes in cell proliferation, migration, differentiation, and survival either independently or in orchestration together. Sex differences are seen in some of these responses to manipulations as prenatal alcohol exposure decreases neurogenesis in female, but not male, rats via reduction in the ratio of new cells differentiating into neurons without affecting cell proliferation or survival, although the sexes were not directly compared across studies [105, 106]. We next discuss basal sex differences in cell proliferation [64, 107], but not survival of these new neurons (see the section 'Sex differences in basal adult neurogenesis'), and in the production and survival of adult-born neurons in response to stress or hippocampus-dependent learning [64, 108,109,110] in the sections 'Sex differences in adult neurogenesis after exposure to stress' and 'Sex differences in adult neurogenesis after learning' below.

Sex differences in basal adult neurogenesis

Some studies find basal sex differences in cell proliferation but not in survival of new neurons in the dentate gyrus. In Sprague Dawley rats and meadow voles, females have greater levels of cell proliferation compared to males that may depend on phase of estrous cycle or season [64, 107, 111]. Galea and McEwen [107] showed that female wild meadow voles have greater cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus only during the non-breeding season compared to males. Furthermore, Tanapat et al. [64] showed that female Sprague Dawley rats during proestrus have greater cell proliferation compared to non-proestrous females and males. However, some studies have failed to find sex or estrous differences in cell proliferation in mice [112, 113], but others show the estrous cycle effect in mice [114]. In contrast to cell proliferation, most studies indicate that there are no significant sex differences in the survival of new neurons in rats, mice, or voles [64, 111, 112, 115, 116] though Dalla et al. [117] found sex differences favouring male rats. Some studies do show sex differences, favouring males, in immature neurons in adulthood [118]. Sex hormones such as androgens and estrogens are potent modulators of adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus but a lengthy discussion of these effects is beyond the scope of this review. Briefly, estrogens modulate neurogenesis in females but to a lesser extent in males [116], whereas androgens modulate neurogenesis in males [119, 120] but it is not known if they modulate neurogenesis in females (for a review see [121]).

Sex differences in adult neurogenesis after exposure to stress

Stress acts on adult neurogenesis in a sex-dependent manner [110, 118]. For example, acute predator odor decreases cell proliferation in male, but not female rats [110] and this was not dependent on levels of testicular hormones in adult males [122] or ovarian hormone levels in adult females [110]. Similarly, repeated restraint stress reduced the number of proliferating stem cells in adult males, but not females [118]. However, 9 days after 12 days of restraint stress, neurogenesis was reduced in adult females, but not adult males [118], suggesting that the duration of time after stress exposure had more impact in females compared to males. Other studies find that both males and females show reduced neurogenesis to stress (footshock, water restraint) and these discrepancies are likely dependent on the nature of the stressor [114]. Together, these studies indicate the nature, duration and timing of the stressor as well as what aspect of neurogenesis is measured (proliferation, immature neurons, mature neurons) will affect the expression of sex differences on neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus.

Sex differences in adult neurogenesis after stress are dependent on the nature of the stressor but also when during the lifespan stress is given, with males more affected by short-term pre-weaning stress and females more affected by stress during puberty on adult neurogenesis. Early life stress, caused by alterations in bedding or maternal deprivation (postnatal day 2–9), reduces neurogenesis in adult male but not adult female mice [123]. However, maternal postpartum corticosterone across the entire postpartum (preweaning to weaning) decreases neurogenesis in adult female offspring but not in adult male offspring [124, 125]. In addition, 24 h of maternal deprivation on postnatal day 3 reduced DCX-expressing cells in females, but increased DCX-expression in males on postnatal day 21 [126], but it is not clear whether this difference would be seen in adulthood. Stress during adolescence (restraint stress) reduces neurogenesis in adult female, but increases neurogenesis male rats [127]. It is clear that stress can reveal sex differences in neurogenesis that depend on the timing of exposure to stress and the nature of the stressor, and for a comprehensive review on sex differences after stress on neurogenesis the reader is directed to other reviews [121, 128].

Sex differences in adult neurogenesis after learning

Adult neurogenesis is modulated by learning differently based on sex (Fig. 2), the direction of which is modified by sex differences in task performance [109, 117, 129]. Chow et al. [129] found that male rats outperformed female rats in acquiring the spatial version of the Morris water maze, which resulted in an enhancement of new neuron survival in the dentate gyrus of males but not females. Conversely, Dalla et al. [117] showed that diestrous female rats outperformed male rats in the trace eyeblink conditioning, which resulted in learning-enhanced survival of new neurons in female, but not male, rats in the ventral dentate gyrus. Yagi et al. [109] demonstrated through that males showed better performance in separating similar patterns and greater adult neurogenesis in the dorsal dentate gyrus than females. These results do not mean to suggest that females do not use the new neurons they create after learning as correlations are seen in the activity of new neurons in relation to performance in females but not in males [129]. Interestingly, male place learners with poor learning ability are more likely to show enhanced neurogenesis in response to spatial training compared to better learners [130]. Collectively, these findings indicate that task difficulty may be differently related to the neurogenic response by sex and/or that sex differences in the functional incorporation and integration of new neurons into the existing circuitry may be differently related to the degree of contribution of the hippocampus for learning.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Neurogenesis is modulated by learning differently based on sex. a Male rats separate similar patterns (adjacent arms) more efficiently than females. Male spatial strategy users had more correct responses on similar (adjacent) arms than female spatial strategy users. There were no sex differences in performance among idiothetic (response) strategy users [109]. b Male strategy users have more new neurons (BrdU/NeuN cells) in the dorsal dentate gyrus compared to all other groups [109]. c Males that underwent spatial training (hidden platform in the Morris Water Maze) have more BrdU-labelled cells in the dentate gyrus compared to female rats that underwent hidden platform training (spatial), visible platform training (Cue), or cage controls (control) [129]. Figures are modified and reprinted with permission from Chow et al. [129] and Yagi et al. [109]. GCL granule cell layer

Sex differences in hippocampus-dependent cognition

Sex differences in learning strategy

Previous studies in both humans [131,132,133] and rodents [134,135,136] demonstrate that males and females differentially rely on two different learning strategies during spatial navigation tasks (Fig. 3). Place strategy engages the hippocampus, while response or cue strategy engages the striatum [137,138,139]. Men are more likely to use geometric cues and women are more likely to use landmark cues to reach a destination [140, 141]. In rodents, female rats preferentially use response strategies when ovarian hormones are low, while male rats preferentially use the place strategy to solve the same tasks [135, 141,142,143]. Studies have found that strategy choice varied in part by testosterone levels in male rats and ovarian hormones in female rats. Removal of testicular hormones by castration slightly reduced the preference for a place learning strategy [136] and low testosterone increased the use of a response strategy while high testosterone led to a preference for a place strategy in dual-solution water maze [144]. In contrast, female rats relied more on landmark cues during non-proestrous phases (lower levels of estradiol), and a place strategy during the proestrous phase (higher estradiol levels; [145]). This is consistent with other studies showing that higher levels of estradiol are associated with a place strategy and lower levels of estradiol with response strategies in female rats [63, 134, 146, 147]. These sex differences in the preference of the two learning strategies suggest that males and females may rely on different brain regions during spatial training, and may contribute to sex differences in performance during spatial navigation.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Schematic illustrations for sex differences in spatial learning strategies in the (a) Morris Water Maze and (b) Dual T-Maze Task. Males are more likely to use geometric cues (place strategy) and females are more likely to use landmark cues (cue response strategy) or proprioceptive cues (proprioceptive response strategy) to reach a destination [135, 140,141,142,143]

Sex differences in spatial learning and memory

Meta-analyses indicate that males outperform females on tasks of spatial navigation and working memory, in both human and rodent studies [148,149,150]. For instance, Galea and Kimura [141] demonstrated that men made fewer errors to recall a learned route on a map, but intriguingly women remembered more landmarks along the route than men, indicating differential attention to cues in men versus women while route learning. In agreement with human studies, male rodents outperform female rodents in a variety of spatial navigation and working memory tasks [150]. However, a number of studies have failed to demonstrate sex differences in spatial navigation tasks. This inconsistency may be due to types of spatial tasks, strategy use, stress exposure, cues that can be used to solve the task, and/or hormone levels. As described in the previous section ('Sex differences in learning strategy'), males and females have different tendencies to rely on the hippocampus-dependent place strategy or the striatum-dependent response strategy. Indeed, a sex difference favouring males exists in the standard reference memory version of the Morris water maze task; but there is no sex difference observed during a cue competition task in which subjects can use both the place and response strategy to solve the task [147]. Furthermore, the environmental cues within the task can dictate whether sex differences in performance are seen. Men perform more accurately to find a platform location than women when more geometric spatial cues exist in the virtual water maze, whereas there is no sex difference when more landmark cues exist [151]. These findings imply that males and females use different strategies, or exhibit differential cue use, to solve the same maze. Indeed, males attend to geometric cues while females attend to landmark and visual cues to solve spatial tasks in both humans and rodents [141, 142]. Thus, it is important to ensure that cues are considered when using both males and females in spatial tasks, as these may significantly influence whether sex differences will be seen.

Reviews in neuroscience suggest that studies are underpowered to detect sex differences [152]. However, this review fails to appreciate that conditions of testing are important in the outcome of sex differences on learning such as cue availability affecting strategy use, housing, hormone levels, and temperature of the water, that all contribute to whether sex differences in spatial acquisition are seen or not. Indeed, Button et al. [152] suggests that sample sizes of 68–134 are required to demonstrate a sex difference. We suggest that the sample size needed to detect a sex difference in spatial performance is much lower if attention is paid to parameters such as cue choice. The heterogeneity in findings on sex differences in spatial ability, are more likely due to variability in protocols and experimental conditions, affecting effective strategy use rather than whether a true sex difference in spatial ability exists. Variability in cognitive testing has lead more than one researcher to suggest the use of standardized protocols/testing [153] and new efforts are underway to share and compare data via platforms such as http://mousebytes.ca/home to improve our understanding of biological variations influence learning.

While there are fewer studies, studies examining the ability to perform pattern separation indicate that males perform more accurately in challenging spatial pattern separation tasks compared to females [109]. For example, comparing two different studies where sex was not compared statistically, male mice were able to distinguish two adjacent arms in the radial 8-arm maze [92], while a different research group reported that females failed to distinguish the two adjacent arms in this same task [94]. However, both sexes performed equally well in distinguishing between distant arms in that task [92, 94]. Although these data are only suggestive, these studies were conducted using mice with different genetic backgrounds, and thus conclusive evidence for sex differences in the ability for pattern separation cannot be established using those two studies. Yagi et al. [109] found sex differences in pattern separation performance (see Fig. 2). In separate analyses of arms, we showed that males were better at pattern separation of similar, but not distinct, patterns. However, an a priori analysis revealed that male place strategy users outperformed female place strategy users when separating similar patterns during a pattern separation task, whereas there were no significant sex differences among response strategy users [109]. Although not originally reported the effect size (Cohen’s d) for the sex difference favouring males in performance on similar arms was 1.73, and 0.6 for distinct arms. This study also demonstrated that male place strategy users had greater number of adult born neurons in the dorsal dentate gyrus than all other groups. Furthermore, the number of new neurons in the ventral dentate gyrus was more strongly associated with the ability to distinguish two adjacent arms (similar patterns) in female place strategy users, although a trend existed in males and caution should be noted as the sample size was small. These results suggest that new neurons in the dorsal hippocampus are more responsive to enhancing effect of spatial learning on survival of new neurons in males. Whereas, the relationship of neurogenesis in the ventral hippocampus in females with performance suggests that female performance is more sensitive to stress and/or that different connectivity patterns may exist between males and females in the dorsal versus ventral hippocampus, an effect detected in humans [40].

Sex differences in spatial learning and memory: relationship to stress

The idea that female performance may be more sensitive to stress, may explain some sex differences seen in the Morris water maze. When rats were pre-exposed to the environment these studies failed to demonstrate the sex difference in spatial performance [150, 154]. Pre-exposure to the water maze reduces the stress levels and circulating levels of stress hormones during the testing sessions and it is possible that females perform better under a less stressful environment [155, 156]. However, studies in meadow voles that used a pre-exposure baseline session consistently showed sex differences favouring males (for review see [157]). It is important to understand that the sex differences in learning also depend on factors such as maternal care experience [158] and gonadal hormone concentrations in males and females (for review see: [91]). While beyond the scope of this review, estradiol levels in females drive spatial acquisition, and as such, lower estradiol in females is associated with better performance in both rodents and in women ([159, 160]; see [91] for review). Given that stress increases estradiol levels in females [161], it may be that this initially exposure to stress serves to increase estradiol levels, pushing the trajectory to poorer performance in females relative to males.

Studies examining learning after stressful experiences show sex differences in performance [162, 163]. Repeated restraint stress (21 days) reduces dendritic complexity in the apical or basal dendrites of CA3 pyramidal neurons of males and females, respectively [73]. However, this same stress regime results in opposing effects on spatial acquisition with impairments in males but improvements in females although the sexes were not directly compared in one study for learning outcomes (reviewed in [164]). Thus, seemingly small differences (apical vs basal) to the architecture of dendrites can lead to opposing effects on spatial memory between the sexes. This association of a reduction in branching leading to an improvement in learning outcomes, or an increase in spines related to a time of impairments in learning outcomes, is a pattern that is often seen in females. It is possible that the signal to noise ratio is different between males and females, and given the greater excitability of the hippocampus in females, female neurons may require pruning of hippocampal neurons to achieve better learning outcomes. This opposing pattern of stress on outcome in males versus females is also seen in studies examining transcriptome after stress, with female mice showing an greater increase compared to males in DNA methyltransferase 3a gene expression, which is an important regulator for synaptic plasticity, in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) after subchronic variable stress [165]. Unfortunately, there are very few studies on the ‘sexome’ done to date [166]. The few studies completed do suggest a number of differences between males and females in gene expression in the hippocampus with aging and disease [167,168,169,170] that may be related to sex differences in cognition. For instance, the number of upregulated genes with aging in the hippocampus of women is two times greater than that of men [169]. These up-regulated genes include genes regulating cell apoptosis, cytokine activity and cell metabolism, which directly or indirectly interact with cell aging and cell death. Studies examining transcriptomic analyses in the hippocampus indicate that there are sex differences [171, 172]. For example, hippocampal mRNA expression for heat-shock proteins, that are proposed to regulate dendritic spines, is greater in adult male compared to adult female mice [171]. More studies directly comparing males and females are essential for further understanding of underlying mechanisms of sex differences in hippocampal function. These transcriptome results give us important clues how sex may interact with hippocampal function, and more research is needed to elucidate the molecular mechanisms of sex differences. As stress modulates learning and memory, and plasticity-related gene expression in the hippocampus differently between sexes, researchers should carefully control experimental environment of research subjects. Furthermore, future studies should include detailed information about experimental procedures especially factors that potentially influence behavioral outcomes such as length of handling, housing conditions, and water temperature of water maze.

Sex differences in immediate early gene (IEG) expression after memory retrieval

Cellular activity in response to spatial memory can be examined by quantifying expression of IEG proteins such as zif268, cfos and Arc, which are rapidly induced after learning and regulate learning-related neural plasticity [173, 174]. Although LTP is not required for c-fos induction [175, 176], the IEG zif268 encodes the zinc finger transcription factors zif268/Egr1 (early growth response protein1) which plays a critical role in the maintenance of LTP in the hippocampus and in the consolidation of long-term memory [174, 177, 178]. Only a few studies have examined sex differences in response to spatial and non-spatial learning on IEGs (see Fig. 4). Yagi et al. [147] found that in the CA3 male place learners showed greater cfos expression, while female place learners showed greater zif268 expression. Intriguingly, there were sex by strategy use differences in activation with female place strategy users showing greater zif268 expression but less cfos expression in the CA3 compared to cue strategy users. This same relationship in the CA3 region was seen after pattern separation with greater zif268 expression in females, but more cfos expression in males [109]. These sex differences in the expression of IEGs were only seen in the dorsal hippocampus, which is intriguing as the dorsal area is more tightly linked to spatial reference memory [179]. Furthermore, none of these same sex differences in IEG expression were present in the CA1 or DG. These results suggest that greater number of zif268-expressing CA3 pyramidal neurons in response to memory retrieval in female compared to male rats, may help recruit LTP, as zif268 is strongly associated with LTP. Intriguingly, changes in cfos expression after spatial memory retrieval are seen across the estrous cycle with rats in proestrous having greater cfos expression in the dentate gyrus than rats not in proestrus [147]. While a few studies have examined sex differences in the expression of cfos and zif268, no studies have been conducted using Arc to determine underlying mechanisms of sex differences in spatial learning ability, or Arc expression of newly produced neural dendrites in the dentate gyrus, which is a clear gap in the literature.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Summary for sex and strategy differences in different (zif268, cfos) immediate early gene (IEG) expression in the hippocampus 90 min after memory retrieval in different hippocampus-dependent tasks (data compiled from [109, 147]). Female place strategy users have greater zif268 expression in the CA3 compared to female response strategy users and males, while male place strategy users have greater cfos expression in the CA3 compared to all other groups after a cue competition task [147]

One study has reported sex differences in activation of adult-born cells in the dentate gyrus relating to performance [129]. In that study, zif268 protein expression was examined after memory retrieval in the Morris water maze 20 days after cell birth. Spatially-trained rats showed greater activation than cue-trained rats with no overall sex differences in expression of activated new neurons. However, greater activation of new neurons was strongly associated with better performance in female rats but not in male rats, suggesting the pattern of the associations are different between males and females, although the correlations were not statistically compared in the original paper, in doing so now these correlations are statistically different (z = −2.05, p = 0.04). This sex difference may be due to sex differences in excitability of 20 days old neurons, in the timing of expression of zif268, and/or the maturation rate of new neurons and further research is encouraged in these areas.

Researchers should also be aware that production of these IEG proteins after learning has different time courses dependent on the IEG [180, 181]. For instance, zif268 protein expression reaches peak levels 60 min after learning [180], whereas, albeit to different stimuli (pentylenetetrazol-induced seizures), cfos protein expression reaches its peak at 60–120 min after the seizure [181]. These differences in induction time courses must be carefully considered when comparing the different IEG expression after learning. We suggest that these sex differences in IEG expression seen after learning may be due not only to different IEGs being recruited during different tasks, but also different regions may show different IEG expression responses, and there may be different timing in IEG expression between the sexes that contribute to these differences.

Sex differences in contextual fear conditioning

The hippocampus is also important for contextual fear learning and memory. Women show a greater prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) with more generalization of fear memories compared to men [182, 183]. Furthermore, women show greater pain sensitivity and anxiety within the context of previously experienced painful event [184]. Female rats demonstrated greater generalization of fear related contextual memory and prolonged fear memory compared to male rats [185]. Lynch et al. [186] examined fear memory generalization using two different contexts: one with electric foot shock and the other without foot shock. They examined passive avoidance of fear (foot shock) related context 1d, 3d, 5d, and 7d after conditioning. Females gradually showed increased fear related response in the neutral context while male rats did not show such an increase and even at 7d after conditioning, male rats showed the ability to discriminate between the two contexts. These studies and others [187] demonstrate that females may have more fear generalization than males which may contribute to the findings that women are more susceptible to fear generalization disorders such as PTSD.

Females show less extinction to contextual fear conditioning than males [188, 189], but males show stronger retention and more freezing to the conditioned stimulus especially after pre-exposure [185, 187, 190]. Males are typically found to have stronger or greater long-term retention of contextual fear conditioning [187], which coincided with increased levels of phosphorylated ERK in the ventral hippocampus of males but not females. Using a context pre-exposure paradigm that relies on the integrity of the dentate gyrus, gonadetomized males and intact males were found to have stronger contextual fear conditioning than females [185, 190]. A caveat with respect to these findings is that researchers have traditionally used freezing as an index of fear memory. However, females show more active patterns of fear expression termed ‘darting’ [191], and thus care must be taken in fear conditioning paradigms to assess the appropriate behaviors in males and females.

These sex differences in conditioned fear responses may be related to sex differences in activation with greater neural activation (cFos) in the basal amygdala of females and in the dorsal hippocampus of males [185, 192]. Keiser et al. [185] suggest this may have to do with the competition between the amygdala and hippocampus but it is also possible that different IEGs or different time points may find a different pattern of activation after fear conditioning. Sex differences in neural patterns may be seen even when there are no sex differences in conditioning strength, as work from the Shansky laboratory as noted sex differences in the prefrontal cortex to amygdala circuits, even when there were no overt sex differences in behavior [193]. It is clear that care must be taken when studying sex differences in fear conditioning, including type of conditioning (cue, contextual), behavior measured (freezing, darting), and pre-exposure to the context.

Future directions and clinical implications

Studying sex differences in hippocampal cognition and neural plasticity, while potentially controversial, is important, as it will aid in elucidating the underlying mechanisms of sex differences in prevalence or manifestation of disorders that disrupt hippocampal integrity and result in cognitive disruption. Furthermore, studying sex differences in cognitive outcome may be important in understanding precision medicine. For example, studies indicate that exercise can offset cognitive decline with the type of exercise related to differential efficacy in men versus women [194] and indeed physical activity can increase hippocampal subregions differently in women versus men [195]. It is vital to understand what exercise regimen (or any type of treatment) may be more beneficial to men versus women to combat disease and aging-related cognitive decline [194]. A number of studies demonstrate sex differences in associations between hippocampal adult neurogenesis and cognition. These findings imply that sex-specific approaches for clinical treatment to increase the treatment efficacy for cognitive decline or disruption may be needed. We emphasize the importance of studying sex differences in measures of hippocampal function. We also caution the community as while the use of both sexes has been mandated in NIH-sponsored clinical trials since 1993 (NIH, 1993), the appropriate sample size, and analyses have not always been reported in the literature. Furthermore, this sex-inclusion mandate does not extend to clinical trials that are not sponsored by NIH, and these trials form the majority of clinical trials listed on clinicaltrials.gov (over 85%). Researchers are encouraged to include males and females in their experiments, and to analyze their data stratified by sex adhering to issues on statistical interpretations of an interaction effect [196]. Furthermore, it is equally important to consider age and hormone status in their studies to gain a clearer picture of how sex differences may be contributing to measures of interest. These efforts will provide the academic and clinical communities with invaluable information that we hope will lead to more efficient clinical treatment for both men and women.