Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning

Abstract

When planning target-directed reaching movements, human subjects combine visual and proprioceptive feedback to form two estimates of the arm's position: one to plan the reach direction, and another to convert that direction into a motor command. These position estimates are based on the same sensory signals but rely on different combinations of visual and proprioceptive input, suggesting that the brain weights sensory inputs differently depending on the computation being performed. Here we show that the relative weighting of vision and proprioception depends both on the sensory modality of the target and on the information content of the visual feedback, and that these factors affect the two stages of planning independently. The observed diversity of weightings demonstrates the flexibility of sensory integration and suggests a unifying principle by which the brain chooses sensory inputs so as to minimize errors arising from the transformation of sensory signals between coordinate frames.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Two types of reaching errors induced by shifted visual feedback.
Figure 2: Predicted changes in Experiments 1 and 2.
Figure 3: Data collection and task parameters.
Figure 4: Sample data, Experiment 1.
Figure 5: Group data, Experiment 1.
Figure 6: Model fits, Experiment 1.
Figure 7: Sample and group data, Experiment 2.
Figure 8: Model fits, Experiment 2.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Welch, R.B., Widawski, M.H., Harrington, J. & Warren, D.H. Examination of the relationship between visual capture and prism adaptation. Percept. Psychophys. 25, 126–132 (1979).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Ghahramani, Z. Computation and Psychophysics of Sensorimotor Integration. Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, (1995).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Jacobs, R.A. Optimal integration of texture and motion cues to depth. Vision Res. 39, 3621–3629 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. van Beers, R.J., Sittig, A.C. & van der Gon, J.J.D. Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: an experimentally supported model. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 1355–1364 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Ernst, M.O. & Banks, M.S. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415, 429–433 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Sober, S.J. & Sabes, P.N. Multisensory integration during motor planning. J. Neurosci. 23, 6982–6992 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Soechting, J.F. & Flanders, M. Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in 3-dimensional space. J. Neurophysiol. 62, 582–594 (1989).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. McIntyre, J., Stratta, F., Droulez, J. & Lacquaniti, F. Analysis of pointing errors reveals properties of data representations and coordinate transformations within the central nervous system. Neural Comput. 12, 2823–2855 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Flash, T. & Hogan, N. The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model. J. Neurosci. 5, 1688–1703 (1985).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Atkeson, C.G. & Hollerbach, J.M. Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements. J. Neurosci. 5, 2318–2330 (1985).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Uno, Y., Kawato, M. & Suzuki, R. Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement: minimum torque-change model. Biol. Cybern. 61, 89–101 (1989).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M.F., Cooper, S.E. & Ghez, C. Accuracy of planar reaching movements. 2. Systematic extent errors resulting from inertial anisotropy. Exp. Brain Res. 99, 112–130 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Kalil, R.E. & Freedman, S.J. Intermanual transfer of compensation for displaced vision. Percept. Mot. Skills 22, 123–126 (1966).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Wallace, B. & Redding, G.M. Additivity in prism adaptation as manifested in intermanual and interocular transfer. Percept. Psychophys. 25, 133–136 (1979).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. DiZio, P. & Lackner, J.R. Motor adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 1787–1792 (1995).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Sainburg, R.L. & Wang, J. Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information. Exp. Brain Res. 145, 437–447 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Taub, E. & Goldberg, L.A. Prism adaptation: control of intermanual transfer by distribution of practice. Science 180, 755–757 (1973).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Cohen, M.M. Visual feedback, distribution of practice, and intermanual transfer of prism aftereffects. Percept. Mot. Skills 37, 599–609 (1973).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Choe, C.S. & Welch, R.B. Variables affecting intermanual transfer and decay of prism adaptation. J. Exp. Psychol. 102, 1076–1084 (1974).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Kitazawa, S., Kimura, T. & Uka, T. Prism adaptation of reaching movements: specificity for the velocity of reaching. J. Neurosci. 17, 1481–1492 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Warren, D.H. & Schmitt, T.L. On plasticity of visual-proprioceptive bias effects. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 4, 302–310 (1978).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Welch, R.B. & Warren, D.H. Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. Psychol. Bull. 88, 638–667 (1980).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Andersen, R.A., Snyder, L.H., Bradley, D.C. & Xing, J. Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 303–330 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Wise, S.P., Boussaoud, D., Johnson, P.B. & Caminiti, R. Premotor and parietal cortex: corticocortical connectivity and combinatorial computations. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 25–42 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Graziano, M.S.A. Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96, 10418–10421 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Graziano, M.S.A., Cooke, D.F. & Taylor, C.S.R. Coding the location of the arm by sight. Science 290, 1782–1786 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Ghilardi, M.F., Gordon, J. & Ghez, C. Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas 73, 2535–2539 (1995).

  28. Rossetti, Y., Desmurget, M. & Prablanc, C. Vectorial coding of movement: vision, proprioception, or both? J. Neurophysiol. 74, 457–463 (1995).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Goodbody, S.J. & Wolpert, D.M. The effect of visuomotor displacements on arm movement paths. Exp. Brain Res. 127, 213–223 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Sainburg, R.L., Lateiner, J.E., Latash, M.L. & Bagesteiro, L.B. Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 401–415 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Jordan, M. Computational aspects of motor control and motor learning. in Handbook of Perception and Action: Motor Skills (eds. Heuer, H. & Keele, S.) (Academic, New York, 1996).

    Google Scholar 

  32. Good, P. Permutation Tests. Springer Series in Statistics 2nd edn. (Springer-Verlag, New York, 2000).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  33. Efron, B. & Tibshirani, R.J. An Introduction to the Bootstrap (Chapman and Hall, Boca Raton, Florida, 1993).

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank M. Kurgansky and L. Osborne for helpful comments on the manuscript and H. Gutierrez for technical assistance. This research was supported by a McKnight Scholar Award, the National Eye Institute (R01 EY15679-01A2), and the Howard Hughes Medical Institute Biomedical Research Support Program grant #5300246 to the UCSF School of Medicine. S.J.S. was supported by a National Science Foundation Fellowship.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Philip N Sabes.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sober, S., Sabes, P. Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning. Nat Neurosci 8, 490–497 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1427

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1427

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing