Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Brief Communication
  • Published:

A psychophysical test of the vibration theory of olfaction

Abstract

At present, no satisfactory theory exists to explain how a given molecule results in the perception of a particular smell. One theory is that olfactory sensory neurons detect intramolecular vibrations of the odorous molecule. We used psychophysical methods in humans to test this vibration theory of olfaction and found no evidence to support it.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Additive synthesis and homologous series.
Figure 2: Isotope substitution.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Burr, C. The Emperor of Scent (Random House, New York, 2002).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Gilbert, A.N. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 335 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Maslin, J. The New York Times February 6 (2003), p. E8.

  4. Givhan, R. The Washington Post February 16 (2003), p. X–T8.

  5. Dyson, G.M. Chem. Ind. 647–651 (1938).

  6. Wright, R.H. J. Theor. Biol. 64, 473–502 (1977).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Turin, L. Chem. Senses 21, 773–791 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Laska, M. & Teubner, P. Chem. Senses 24, 263–270 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Turin, L. & Yoshii, F. in Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation (ed. Doty, R.L.) 275–294 (Marcel Dekker, New York, 2003).

    Google Scholar 

  10. Laska, M. & Teubner, P. Chem. Senses 24, 161–170 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Livermore, A. & Hummel, T. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 75, 305–313 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Haffenden, L.J.W., Yaylayan, V.A. & Fortin, J. Food Chem. 73, 67–72 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Watson, W.L., Laing, D.G., Hutchinson, I. & Jinks, A.L. Dev. Psychobiol. 39, 137–145 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Kobal, G. & Hummel, C. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 71, 241–250 (1988).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank A. Gilbert for expert advice, members of the Vosshall laboratory for comments on the manuscript, and E. Gotschlich, B. Coller and the staff of the Rockefeller University Hospital. A.K. is an M.S. Stoffel Fellow in Mind, Brain and Behavior.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andreas Keller.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Fig. 1.

Additive synthesis. Turin suggests that the smell of a mixture of guaiacol and benzaldehyde has a vanilla character not found in its components because the combined molecular vibrations of benzaldehyde and guaiacol roughly approximate the vibrations of vanillin. (a) To test this prediction, subjects rated the vanilla character of benzaldehyde, guaiacol and a 1:1 mixture of both. This is the same experiment as in Fig. 1b but done at lower stimulus concentration (10-4). The benzaldehyde/guaiacol mixture had a vanilla character no stronger than either of its components (N=24, 12 females, 12 males, middle line: median, box boundaries and whiskers: 25%, 75% and 10% and 90% quantiles, respectively; P > 0.05; Student-Newman-Keuls test for multiple comparisons after Friedman's test). (b) In another version of this experiment, odor similarity was rated on a scale from 0 (smells the same) to 10 (smells very different). Subjects did not rate the 1:1 mixture of undiluted benzaldehyde and guaiacol more similar to undiluted vanillin than each individual component of the mixture. The vanillin vs. components and the mixture vs. components values include the comparison between vanillin or the mixture and both benzaldehyde and guaiacol (N=36, 22 females, 14 males) (P > 0.05; Student-Newman-Keuls test for multiple comparisons after Friedman's test). (JPG 37 kb)

Supplementary Fig. 2.

Odor similarity rating of aldehydes. Vibration theory, as formulated by Turin, suggests that aldehydes with an even number of carbon atoms smell different from those with an odd number. We had subjects rate pairs of undiluted aldehydes with eight to eleven carbon atoms for perceived smell dissimilarity. The results show that within-pair smell dissimilarity increased along with the difference in carbon atom number. Odor dissimilarity was rated on a scale from 0 (smells the same) to 10 (smells very different). The three carbon atoms difference includes the pair octanal/undecanal, the two carbon difference includes octanal/decanal and nonanal/undecanal, the one carbon difference octanal/nonanal, nonanal/decanal, and decanal/undecanal (N=36, 22 females, 14 males, middle line: median, box boundaries and whiskers: 25%, 75% and 10% and 90% quantiles, respectively). (JPG 31 kb)

Supplementary Methods (PDF 21 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Keller, A., Vosshall, L. A psychophysical test of the vibration theory of olfaction. Nat Neurosci 7, 337–338 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1215

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1215

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing