Skip to main content
Log in

Selective Inhibition in Children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Off and On Stimulant Medication

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Selective inhibition requires discrimination between auditory signals and is assessed using a modification of the stop-signal task. Selective inhibition was assessed in a group of 59 clinic-referred, DSM-IV-diagnosed children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and compared to that of a community sample of 59 children. Methylphenidate (MPH) effects on selective inhibition were assessed in a subset of the ADHD sample that participated in an acute, randomized, placebo-controlled, crossover trial with 3 fixed doses of MPH. Children with ADHD performed more poorly than controls on the majority of selective stop-signal task parameters: they exhibited more anticipatory (invalid) responses, with less accurate and more variable responses on the response execution task, as well as a slower selective inhibition process. MPH improved speed of both inhibition and response execution processes; it also reduced variability of response execution and decreased nonselective inhibition. On the one hand, findings are consistent with purported inhibition deficit in ADHD, but on the other hand, suggest that neither the impairment itself, nor MPH effects, were restricted to inhibition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994), Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65-94.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bedard, A.-C., Nichols, S., Barbosa, J. A., Schachar, R., Logan, G. D., & Tannock, R. (2002). Development of selective inhibitory control across the life-span. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 93-111.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chhabildas, N., Pennington, B. F., & Willcutt, E. G. (2001). A comparison of the neuropsychological profiles of the DSM-IV subtypes of ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 529-540.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners' Rating Scales—Revised: Technical Manual. New York: Multi-Health Systems, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, R. (1991). Partial information or facilitation? Different interpretations of results from speed-accuracy decomposition. Perception and Psychophysics, 50, 333-350.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, R., Coles, M. G., & Logan, G. D. (1995). Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 498-511.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, V. I. (1999). Cognitive control processes in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In H. C. Quay & A. E. Hogan (Eds.), Handbook of disruptive behavior disorders (pp. 105-138). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., & Lyon, G. R. (1998). Intelligent testing and the discrepancy model for children with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 13, 186-203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenhill, L. L., Pliszka, S., Dulcan, M. K., Bernet, W., Arnold, V., Beitchman, J. et al. (2002). Practice parameter for the use of stimulant medications in the treatment of children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 26S-49S.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ickowicz, A., Schachar, R., Sugarman, R., Chen, S., Millete, C., Cook, L., et al. (2002, October). Reliability and validity of the Parent Interview for Child Symptoms (PICS). Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. San Francisco, CA.

  • Kuntsi, J., Oosterlaan, J., & Stevenson, J. (2001). Psychological mechanisms in hyperactivity: I. Response inhibition deficit, working memory impairment, delay aversion, or something else? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 199-210.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1985). On the ability to inhibit simple thoughts and actions: 2. Stop-signal studies of repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 675-691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users' guide to the stop-signal paradigm. In D. D. Carr & T. H. Carr (Eds.). Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189-239). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Burkell, J. (1986). Dependence and independence of responding to double stimulation: A comparison of stop, change, and dual-task paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 549-563.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of act and control. Psychological Review, 91, 295-327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibition. Psychological Science, 8, 60-64.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Institutes of Health Consensus Development Conference Statement: Diagnosis and Treatment of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). (2000). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 182-193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (1999). The ADHD response-inhibition deficit as measured by the stop-signal task: Replication with DSM-IV combined type, extension, and qualification. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 393-402.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T., Blaskey, L. G., Huang-Pollock, C. L., & Rappley, M. D. (2002). Neuropsychological executive functions and DSM-IV ADHD subtypes. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 59-66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oosterlaan, J., Logan, G. D., & Sergeant, J. A. (1998). Response inhibition in AD/HD, CD, comorbid AD/HD + CD, anxious, and control children: A meta-analysis of studies with the stop-signal task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 411-425.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (1998). Response inhibition and response re-engagement in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, disruptive, anxious and normal children. Behavioral Brain Research, 94, 33-43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osman, A., Kornblum, S., & Meyer, D. E. (1990). Does response programming necessitate response execution? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 183-198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overtoom, C. C. E., Kenemans, J. L., Verbaten, M. N., Kemner, C., van der Molen, M. W., van Engeland, H., et al. (2002). Inhibition in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A psychophysiological study of the stop task. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 668-676.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Purvis, K. L., & Tannock, R. (2000). Phonological processing, not inhibition, differentiates ADHD and reading disability. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 485-494.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quay, H. C. (1997). Inhibition and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 7-13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, M. D., & Denney, C. (1997). Titrating methylphenidate in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Is body mass predictive of clinical response? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 523-530.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schachar, R., & Logan, G. D. (1990). Impulsivity and inhibition in normal development and childhood psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 26, 710-720.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachar, R., Mota, V. L., Logan, G. D., Tannock, R., & Klim, P. (2000). Confirmation of an inhibition deficit in attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 227-235.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schachar, R., Tannock, R., & Cunningham, C. (1996). Treatment of hyperactive disorders. In S. Sandberg (Ed.), Hyperactive disorders (pp. 433-477). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Cambridge Monographs in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachar, R., Tannock, R., & Logan, G. D. (1993). Inhibition, impulsiveness, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 721-739.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachar, R., Tannock, R., Marriott, M., & Logan, G. (1995). Deficient inhibition in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 411-437.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2001). Response execution and inhibition in children with AD/HD and other disruptive disorders: The role of behavioral activation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 347-357.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., Holahan, J. M., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1992). Discrepancy compared to low achievement definitions of reading disability: results from the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 639-648.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Advances in cognitive, neurobiological, and genetic research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 65-99.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., Hum, M., Masellis, M., Humphries, T., & Schachar, R. (2002). Teacher Telephone Interview for children's academic performance, attention, behavior, and learning: DSM-IV Version (TTI-IV). Toronto, Canada: The Hospital for Sick Children. Unpublished document.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., Schachar, R. J., Carr, R. P., Chajczyk, D., & Logan, G. D. (1989). Effects of methylphenidate on inhibition in hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 473-491.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., Schachar, R., & Logan, G. D. (1995). Methylphenidate and cognitive flexibility: Dissociated dose effects on behavior and cognition in hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 235-266.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children—Third Edition. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). The Wide Range Achievement Test—Third Edition (WRAT3). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Boada, R., Ogline, J. S, Tunick, R. A., Chhabildas, N. A., et al. (2001). A comparison of the cognitive deficits in reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 157-172.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, B. R., Ponesse, J. S., Schachar, R. J., Logan, G. D., & Tannock, R. (1999). Development of inhibitory control across the lifespan. Developmental Psychology, 35, 205-213.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Test–Revised (WRMT-R). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rosemary Tannock.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bedard, AC., Ickowicz, A., Logan, G.D. et al. Selective Inhibition in Children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Off and On Stimulant Medication. J Abnorm Child Psychol 31, 315–327 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023285614844

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023285614844

Navigation