Trends in Neurosciences
Volume 34, Issue 10, October 2011, Pages 526-535
Journal home page for Trends in Neurosciences

Review
Special Issue: Hippocampus and Memory
Updating hippocampal representations: CA2 joins the circuit

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2011.07.007Get rights and content

The hippocampus integrates the encoding, storage and recall of memories, binding the spatio-temporal and sensory information that constitutes experience and keeping episodes in their correct context. The rapid and accurate processing of such daunting volumes of continuously changing data relies on dynamically assigning different aspects of mnemonic processing to specialized, interconnected networks corresponding to the anatomical subfields of dentate gyrus (DG), CA3 and CA1. However, differentially processed information ultimately has to be reintegrated into conjunctive representations, and this is unlikely to be achieved by unidirectional, sequential steps through a DG-CA3–CA1 loop. In this Review, we highlight recently discovered anatomical and physiological features that are likely to necessitate updates to the hippocampal circuit diagram, particularly by incorporating the oft-neglected CA2 region.

Introduction

Adaptations of the hippocampus that are likely to reflect the demands of memory processing are immediately apparent in its gross histology: the dense hippocampal cell layers are precisely arranged in a circuit of subfields encompassing the arrowhead of dentate gyrus (DG) and the curve of CA1–3. No single, homogeneous neural network can process all aspects of episodic memory simultaneously and, indeed, anatomical, neurophysiological and behavioural studies over the past two centuries or more have informed influential models of these subfields as specialized processing modules, each contributing to different facets of hippocampal function.

In piecing together this jigsaw of hippocampal subfields and connections, the collective tendency has been to start with the DG and build around a trisynaptic circuit to CA3 and then CA1 (Figure 1a,b). Most models emphasize sequential steps of information processing in this circuit: layer II principal cells of the entorhinal cortex (EC) project to the granule cells of the DG through the perforant path (PP), the granule cells project to CA3 pyramidal cells through mossy fibers (MF), CA3 pyramidal cells synapse onto CA1 pyramidal cells via the Schaffer collaterals (SC), then CA1 outputs to subiculum, deep-layer EC pyramidal cells and related parahippocampal and frontal neocortical regions. Prominent examples of differential information processing include pattern separation in DG (granule cells are abundant and sparse firing, hence different patterns of EC inputs are highly unlikely to activate identical subsets of granule cells and may be ‘orthoganolized’ at this stage) followed by pattern completion in CA3 (where dense, recurrent, excitatory projections within its own pyramidal cell population endow ‘auto-associative’ properties) (1, 2, 3, 4, 5; see also [6] in this Issue). The neat hippocampal loop has therefore been presumed to allow integration and processing of information provided via association cortex, then subsequent feedback to the cortex via CA1.

However, as the resolution of anatomical knowledge reaches the subcellular level and the nature of hippocampal network activity during a diverse behavioral repertoire of encoding, processing, storage and recall is increasingly well documented, simplifying models inevitably become more complex (Box 1). Here, we review recent discoveries that are likely to necessitate updates to the prevailing hypotheses, with particular emphasis on the potentially unique contributions made by the oft-neglected subfield, CA2.

Section snippets

Coding the spatial context of memories

As in humans, the hippocampi of non-human animals play crucial roles in the memory of where, when and what aspects of events 7, 8, 9, 10 and their relative positions in space and time [11]. The rodent hippocampus in particular has proved a powerful model in which to test numerical and computational aspects of memory using anatomical and functional studies, respectively. Multi-neuron recordings pioneered in behaving rodents have uncovered the nature of information processing in different

Some quirks of entorhinal–hippocampal connectivity

In the superficial MEC, grid cells in layer III differ from those in layer II in that many (approximately 66%) also convey information regarding the direction the animal is heading [i.e. head direction (HD)] [29]. In addition to these conjunctive cells, MEC layer III also contains HD cells similar to those found in thalamic, subicular and retrosplenial regions 30, 31, 32. Finally, both layers II and III contain border cells, which respond to edges of a local environment and have been suggested

CA2 comes in from the cold

Since its definition on the basis of lack of MF input or thorny excrescences [44], CA2 has been quietly ignored for the most part, and has been notably absent from the vast majority of hippocampal circuit diagrams and models. However, building on the small existing literature, recent studies have begun to establish a unique connectivity and physiology consistent with CA2 being far more than a passive transition zone between CA3 and CA1.

The borders of rodent CA2 with enveloping CA3 and CA1 are

Selecting circuits within circuits: who does what, when?

Clues to deciphering CA2 function can be gleaned from interventional studies, some aimed specifically at CA2 and others targeting CA3. Mice lacking the Avpr1b gene, which encodes the vasopressin 1b receptor, which is enriched in, although not restricted to, CA2 pyramidal cells, demonstrate intact spatial learning [65], but impairments in two tasks related to the memory of temporal order [66]. Unfortunately, the physiological impact of the mutation was not determined. Mutant mice lacking the

Concluding remarks

The hippocampus is typically taken as a model of sequential processing in the nervous system, with a chain of specialized subfields each contributing to different aspects of episodic memory function. Although this is broadly consistent with place cell data relating to encoding of spatial information, views of the trisynaptic loop through DG, CA3 and CA1 need updating, particularly by incorporating CA2, to accommodate a wealth of new anatomical, genetic and physiological data. Anatomy dictates

Acknowledgments

MWJ would like to thank the Medical Research Council, Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council and The Wellcome Trust for support. TJM would like to thank the RIKEN Brain Science Institute, RIKEN Rijicho Fund and the Japan Science and Technology Agency Core Research of Evolutional Science & Technology Program for support and the members of his laboratory for comments on the manuscript.

References (135)

  • R.Q. Quiroga

    Sparse but not ‘grandmother-cell’ coding in the medial temporal lobe

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2008)
  • D. Derdikman et al.

    A manifold of spatial maps in the brain

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2010)
  • J.L. Calton et al.

    Where am I and how will I get there from here? A role for posterior parietal cortex in the integration of spatial information and route planning

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (2009)
  • V. Chevaleyre et al.

    Strong CA2 pyramidal neuron synapses define a powerful disynaptic cortico-hippocampal loop

    Neuron

    (2010)
  • P. Beed

    Analysis of excitatory microcircuitry in the medial entorhinal cortex reveals cell-type-specific differences

    Neuron

    (2010)
  • W.S. Young

    The vasopressin 1b receptor is prominent in the hippocampal area CA2 where it is unaffected by restraint stress or adrenalectomy

    Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • T. Ochiishi

    High level of adenosine A1 receptor-like immunoreactivity in the CA2/CA3a region of the adult rat hippocampus

    Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • S.T. Bland

    Expression of fibroblast growth factor-2 and brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA in the medial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus after uncontrollable or controllable stress

    Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • H. Takahashi et al.

    Pathway interactions and synaptic plasticity in the dendritic tuft regions of CA1 pyramidal neurons

    Neuron

    (2009)
  • G. Buzsaki

    Theta oscillations in the hippocampus

    Neuron

    (2002)
  • K. Mizuseki

    Theta oscillations provide temporal windows for local circuit computation in the entorhinal-hippocampal loop

    Neuron

    (2009)
  • V.H. Brun

    Impaired spatial representation in CA1 after lesion of direct input from entorhinal cortex

    Neuron

    (2008)
  • W.X. Pan et al.

    The supramammillary area: its organization, functions and relationship to the hippocampus

    Prog. Neurobiol.

    (2004)
  • J. Csicsvari

    Ensemble patterns of hippocampal CA3-CA1 neurons during sharp wave-associated population events

    Neuron

    (2000)
  • J. O’Neill

    Play it again: reactivation of waking experience and memory

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2010)
  • S. Cheng et al.

    New experiences enhance coordinated neural activity in the hippocampus

    Neuron

    (2008)
  • T. Nakashiba

    Hippocampal CA3 output is crucial for ripple-associated reactivation and consolidation of memory

    Neuron

    (2009)
  • T. Porkka-Heiskanen et al.

    Adenosine, energy metabolism and sleep homeostasis

    Sleep Med Rev

    (2011)
  • E.J. Henriksen

    Spatial representation along the proximodistal axis of CA1

    Neuron

    (2010)
  • M.S. Fanselow et al.

    Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures?

    Neuron

    (2010)
  • A. Jovalekic

    Horizontal biases in rats’ use of three-dimensional space

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2011)
  • A. Adhikari

    Synchronized activity between the ventral hippocampus and the medial prefrontal cortex during anxiety

    Neuron

    (2010)
  • D.J. Willshaw et al.

    An assessment of Marr's theory of the hippocampus as a temporary memory store

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci.

    (1990)
  • J.L. McClelland

    Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory

    Psychol. Rev.

    (1995)
  • T.R. Zentall

    Episodic-like memory in pigeons

    Psychon. Bull. Rev.

    (2001)
  • R.G. Morris

    Episodic-like memory in animals: psychological criteria, neural mechanisms and the value of episodic-like tasks to investigate animal models of neurodegenerative disease

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci.

    (2001)
  • P.A. Lipton et al.

    Complementary roles of hippocampus and medial entorhinal cortex in episodic memory

    Neural. Plas.

    (2008)
  • J. O’Keefe et al.

    The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map

    (1978)
  • R.G. Morris

    Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions

    Nature

    (1982)
  • J. O’Keefe et al.

    Hippocampal place units in the freely moving rat: why they fire where they fire

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1978)
  • M.A. Wilson et al.

    Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space

    Science

    (1993)
  • M.A. Wilson et al.

    Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep

    Science

    (1994)
  • B. Poucet

    Sensory and memory properties of hippocampal place cells

    Rev. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • E. Pastalkova

    Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus

    Science

    (2008)
  • A.D. Ekstrom

    Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation

    Nature

    (2003)
  • C.F. Doeller

    Evidence for grid cells in a human memory network

    Nature

    (2010)
  • T. Hafting

    Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex

    Nature

    (2005)
  • E.I. Moser

    Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2008)
  • F. Sargolini

    Conjunctive representation of position, direction, and velocity in entorhinal cortex

    Science

    (2006)
  • J.S. Taube

    The head direction signal: origins and sensory-motor integration

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text