Disconnection of basolateral amygdala and insular cortex disrupts conditioned approach in Pavlovian lever autoshaping

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2017.11.010Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Contralateral disconnection of BLA-IC reduced food-cup responding in goal-trackers.

  • Contralateral disconnection of BLA-IC reduced lever responding in sign-trackers.

  • Ipsilateral disconnection of BLA-IC has no effect on preferred responding in either group.

Abstract

Previously established individual differences in appetitive approach and devaluation sensitivity observed in goal- and sign-trackers may be attributed to differences in the acquisition, modification, or use of associative information in basolateral amygdala (BLA) pathways. Here, we sought to determine the extent to which communication of associative information between BLA and anterior portions of insular cortex (IC) supports ongoing Pavlovian conditioned approach behaviors in sign- and goal-tracking rats, in the absence of manipulations to outcome value. We hypothesized that the BLA mediates goal-, but not sign- tracking approach through interactions with the IC, a brain region involved in supporting flexible behavior. We first trained rats in Pavlovian lever autoshaping to determine their sign- or goal-tracking tendency. During alternating test sessions, we gave unilateral intracranial injections of vehicle or a cocktail of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) receptor agonists, baclofen and muscimol, unilaterally into the BLA and contralaterally or ipsilaterally into the IC prior to reinforced lever autoshaping sessions. Consistent with our hypothesis we found that contralateral inactivation of BLA and IC increased the latency to approach the food cup and decreased the number of food cup contacts in goal-trackers. While contralateral inactivation of BLA and IC did not affect the total number of lever contacts in sign-trackers, this manipulation increased the latency to approach the lever. Ipsilateral inactivation of BLA and IC did not impact approach behaviors in Pavlovian lever autoshaping. These findings, contrary to our hypothesis, suggest that communication between BLA and IC maintains a representation of initially learned appetitive associations that commonly support the initiation of Pavlovian conditioned approach behavior regardless of whether it is directed at the cue or the location of reward delivery.

Introduction

During Pavlovian lever autoshaping, sign-tracking rats preferentially approach and contact the lever, while goal-tracking rats preferentially approach and contact the food cup (Boakes, 1977, Flagel et al., 2007, Hearst and Jenkins, 1974). Two recent studies have provided evidence that goal-trackers rely on representations of the current value of the outcome to promote flexible behavior, whereas sign-trackers inflexibly respond based on initially learned appetitive associations (Morrison et al., 2015, Nasser et al., 2015). Individual differences in behavioral flexibility of sign- and goal-trackers may be rooted in the recruitment of dissociable basolateral amygdala (BLA) pathways known to mediate behavior that relies on stimulus-response versus stimulus-outcome associations. The BLA has reciprocal interactions with more specialized areas including insular cortex (IC) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (Aggleton et al., 1980, Krettek and Price, 1977, McDonald, 1991, McDonald, 1998, Miranda and McGaugh, 2004, Morecraft et al., 1992, Parkes and Balleine, 2013, Shinonaga et al., 1994, Sripanidkulchai et al., 1984). The IC and OFC are two neighboring regions which are critical for representing gustatory associations and the current motivational value of the outcome that is necessary for flexible, stimulus-outcome driven learning and goal-directed action (Baxter et al., 2000, Fiuzat et al., 2017, Grossman et al., 2008, Johnson et al., 2009, Miranda and McGaugh, 2004, Nasser and McNally, 2013, Ostlund and Balleine, 2007, Parkes and Balleine, 2013, Pickens et al., 2003, Piette et al., 2012, Rudebeck et al., 2013, Rudebeck and Murray, 2008, Schoenbaum et al., 1998, Schoenbaum et al., 1999, Schoenbaum et al., 2003, Stalnaker et al., 2007, Zeeb and Winstanley, 2013). Here, we sought to determine the extent to which BLA-IC communication supports ongoing Pavlovian approach behaviors in sign- and goal-tracking rats, in the absence of changes to outcome value.

Amygdala lesion and inactivation studies examining the neurobiological underpinnings of incentive learning processes (for review see: Wassum & Izquierdo, 2015) provide insights into candidate brain circuits that may mediate such individual differences in flexible behavior. We hypothesize that sign- and goal-tracking differences, particularly with relevance for behavioral flexibility, may be rooted in the recruitment of different BLA pathways known to mediate behavior that relies on stimulus-response associations (Hatfield et al., 1996, Setlow, Gallagher et al., 2002, Setlow, Holland et al., 2002) versus stimulus-outcome associations (Hatfield et al., 1996, Johnson et al., 2009, Lichtenberg et al., 2017, Parkinson et al., 2000, Pickens et al., 2003, Schoenbaum et al., 1999, Schoenbaum et al., 2003, Stalnaker et al., 2007). Higher order associative processes including Pavlovian outcome devaluation and second-order conditioning commonly depend on an intact BLA during acquisition of appetitive associative learning. Outcome devaluation studies examining the involvement of BLA in the formation of stimulus-outcome associations show that BLA is not critical for initially acquiring conditioned responding to reinforced cues, but instead for maintaining or adjusting the acquired cue value to support new learning when outcome value changes (Hatfield et al., 1996, Parkinson et al., 2000, Pickens et al., 2003, Johnson et al., 2009). Inactivation, lesion and recording studies demonstrate that BLA encodes and IC/OFC retrieves the current incentive value of the outcome to promote appropriate goal-directed and flexible behaviors (Parkes and Balleine, 2013, Pickens et al., 2003, Rudebeck et al., 2013, Schoenbaum et al., 2003). In Pavlovian outcome devaluation an acquired appetitive cue-outcome association is modified by degrading outcome value, which is then used to flexibly reduce conditioned responding to the previously appetitive cue. The established differences in devaluation sensitivity previously observed in goal- and sign-trackers may be attributed to differences in the acquisition, modification or use of associative information in BLA pathways (Morrison et al., 2015, Nasser et al., 2015). To begin addressing the neurobiological mechanisms mediating tracking-related differences in incentive learning we aimed to determine the extent to which communication of associative information between BLA and IC drives conditioned approach in sign- and goal-tracking rats. We predicted that only in goal-trackers would BLA-IC disconnection disrupt the representation of the initially appetitive association that supports ongoing food-cup approach. Importantly, our speculation on associative representations is based indirectly on previous studies in which behavioral and neurobiological manipulations are made in contexts where S-O and S-R associations are directly probed.

To this end, second-order conditioning studies examining the role of BLA in the formation of stimulus-response associations demonstrate that the BLA is necessary for the initial acquisition of the incentive value of the conditioned stimulus (Hatfield et al., 1996, Setlow, Gallagher et al., 2002). Further, BLA interactions with nucleus accumbens are necessary for using that acquired motivational information to support conditioning to novel cues (Setlow, Holland et al., 2002). Recent work evaluating the role of BLA in supporting conditioned responding during Pavlovian lever autoshaping (Chang et al., 2012a, Chang et al., 2012b), the procedure used to identify sign- and goal- tracking rats (Meyer et al., 2012), demonstrate that the BLA is also necessary for invigorating lever-directed conditioned responding based on previously acquired appetitive associations. Together, studies employing various Pavlovian conditioning procedures demonstrate that BLA is commonly and critically engaged early in learning to drive incentive learning processes. Via its interactions with downstream targets, BLA maintains both stimulus-outcome and stimulus-response associations needed for driving flexibility after manipulations to outcome value and for invigorating conditioned responding, respectively. The present study aims to determine the role for BLA communication with IC for mediating individual differences in appetitive approach that may underlie tracking-related individual differences in higher order processes (Nasser et al., 2015).

Here we test our hypothesis that BLA mediates approach in goal-trackers through interactions with insular cortex, a brain region involved in supporting flexible behavior driven by either stimulus-outcome or action-outcome associations (Ostlund and Balleine, 2007, Parkes and Balleine, 2013, Pickens et al., 2003, Saddoris et al., 2005, Schoenbaum et al., 2003). We use an anatomical asymmetrical disconnection procedure in which we reversibly inactivate the BLA in one hemisphere and IC in the contralateral hemisphere using gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) receptor agonists GABA-A + GABA-B receptor agonists (muscimol + baclofen) during Pavlovian lever autoshaping after sign- and goal-tracking behaviors have been established. Because of the overwhelmingly unilateral projections between BLA and IC (Krettek and Price, 1977, Parkes and Balleine, 2013, Sripanidkulchai et al., 1984) contralateral, but not ipsilateral, reversible inactivation of BLA and IC is anticipated to substantially disrupt communication between these two interconnected structures. We include the ipsilateral inactivation groups to verify that effects of BLA and IC inactivation on approach behaviors are due to disrupted communication between these two interconnected structures, and not simply due to unilateral inactivation of these to two brain areas independent of information communicated within the pathways. Notably, the anterior portion of insular cortex we target is often damaged by OFC lesions or recorded from in OFC studies examining associative encoding in rats (Chang, 2014, Gallagher et al., 1999, Ostlund and Balleine, 2007, Pickens et al., 2003, Saddoris et al., 2005, Schoenbaum et al., 2003, Stalnaker et al., 2007).

Section snippets

Subjects and apparatus

Male Long-Evans rats (Charles River Laboratories, Wilmington, MA; 250–260 g at time of arrival, total n = 112 were singly housed and maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights off at 6:00 PM). All rats had ad libitum access to water and standard laboratory chow before being individually housed before lever autoshaping and surgical procedures. Prior to conditioning and immediately prior to surgical procedures, rats had ad libitum access to Purina rat chow and water. We weighed rats daily and

Histology

Fig. 1B shows unilateral (right or left counterbalanced) intracranial cannula placements in IC and BLA for contralateral and ipsilateral groups; circles and triangles denote placements for goal-trackers and sign-trackers, respectively. The final group numbers based on BLA and IC placements and day four PCA scores were, contralateral sign-trackers (n = 14), contralateral goal-trackers (n = 11), ipsilateral sign-trackers (n = 10) and ipsilateral goal-trackers (n = 6).

Pavlovian lever autoshaping conditioning

Prior to surgery, we screened

Discussion

We examined the effect of reversibly disrupting communication between the BLA and IC on appetitive approach behaviors in a Pavlovian lever autoshaping task. The primary prediction of our hypothesis was that contralateral disconnection of BLA and IC would specifically disrupt appetitive approach behavior in goal-tracking, but not sign-tracking rats. Consistent with our hypothesis we found that BLA-IC disconnection increased the latency to approach the food cup and decreased the number of food

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a McKnight Memory and Cognitive Disorders Award, a NARSAD Young Investigator Grant #24950, NIDA grant R01DA043533 and the Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology at the University of Maryland, School of Medicine. The authors declare that they do not have any conflicts of interest (financial or otherwise) related to the data presented in this manuscript.

References (83)

  • Q.J. Huys et al.

    The role of learning-related dopamine signals in addiction vulnerability

    Progress in Brain Research

    (2014)
  • D.N. Kearns et al.

    Recovery of Pavlovian sign-tracking (autoshaping) following the discontinuation of inter-trial interval food in rats

    Behavioural Processes

    (2007)
  • A.J. McDonald

    Organization of amygdaloid projections to the prefrontal cortex and associated striatum in the rat

    Neuroscience

    (1991)
  • A.J. McDonald

    Cortical pathways to the mammalian amygdala

    Progress in Neurobiology

    (1998)
  • S.E. Morrison et al.

    Different time courses for learning-related changes in amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex

    Neuron

    (2011)
  • N.H. Naqvi et al.

    The hidden island of addiction: the insula

    Trends in Neurosciences

    (2009)
  • P.H. Rudebeck et al.

    Effects of amygdala lesions on reward-value coding in orbital and medial prefrontal cortex

    Neuron

    (2013)
  • M.P. Saddoris et al.

    Rapid associative encoding in basolateral amygdala depends on connections with orbitofrontal cortex

    Neuron

    (2005)
  • C.L. Samuelsen et al.

    Effects of cue-triggered expectation on cortical processing of taste

    Neuron

    (2012)
  • G. Schoenbaum et al.

    Encoding predicted outcome and acquired value in orbitofrontal cortex during cue sampling depends upon input from basolateral amygdala

    Neuron

    (2003)
  • S.A. Sculfort et al.

    Dopamine antagonism does not impair learning of Pavlovian conditioned approach to manipulable or non-manipulable cues but biases responding towards goal tracking

    Behavioural Brain Research

    (2016)
  • Y. Shinonaga et al.

    Topographic organization of collateral projections from the basolateral amygdaloid nucleus to both the prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens in the rat

    Neuroscience

    (1994)
  • T.A. Stalnaker et al.

    Basolateral amygdala lesions abolish orbitofrontal-dependent reversal impairments

    Neuron

    (2007)
  • A. Tomie

    Locating reward cue at response manipulandum (CAM) induces symptoms of drug abuse

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (1996)
  • A. Tomie et al.

    Behavioral characteristics and neurobiological substrates shared by Pavlovian sign-tracking and drug abuse

    Brain Research Reviews

    (2008)
  • C. Torres et al.

    Dorsomedial striatum lesions affect adjustment to reward uncertainty, but not to reward devaluation or omission

    Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • K.M. Wassum et al.

    The basolateral amygdala in reward learning and addiction

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2015)
  • N.M. White et al.

    Learning not to respond: Role of the hippocampus in withholding responses during omission training

    Behavioural Brain Research

    (2017)
  • A.M. Ahrens et al.

    Neural activity in the ventral pallidum encodes variation in the incentive value of a reward cue

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • M.G. Baxter et al.

    Control of response selection by reinforcer value requires interaction of amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • J.S. Beckmann et al.

    Isolating the incentive salience of reward-associated stimuli: Value, choice, and persistence

    Learning and Memory

    (2015)
  • G.B. Bissonette et al.

    Neurophysiology of reward-guided behavior: Correlates related to predictions, value, motivation, errors, attention, and action

    Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences

    (2016)
  • R. Boakes

    Operant pavlovian interactions

    (1977)
  • S.E. Chang et al.

    An omission procedure reorganizes the microstructure of sign-tracking while preserving incentive salience

    Learning and Memory

    (2016)
  • S.E. Chang et al.

    Chemogenetic manipulation of ventral pallidal neurons impairs acquisition of sign-tracking in rats

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (2015)
  • J.J. Clark et al.

    Dopamine encoding of Pavlovian incentive stimuli diminishes with extended training

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2013)
  • A.G. DiFeliceantonio et al.

    Dorsolateral neostriatum contribution to incentive salience: Opioid or dopamine stimulation makes one reward cue more motivationally attractive than another

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • G. Ferreira et al.

    Basolateral amygdala glutamatergic activation enhances taste aversion through NMDA receptor activation in the insular cortex

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • C.J. Fitzpatrick et al.

    Lesions of the ventral hippocampus attenuate the acquisition but not expression of sign-tracking behavior in rats

    Hippocampus

    (2016)
  • C.J. Fitzpatrick et al.

    Subanesthetic ketamine decreases the incentive-motivational value of reward-related cues

    Journal of Psychopharmacology

    (2017)
  • E.C. Fiuzat et al.

    The role of orbitofrontal-amygdala interactions in updating action-outcome valuations in macaques

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2017)
  • Cited by (15)

    • Medial orbitofrontal neurotrophin systems integrate hippocampal input into outcome-specific value representations

      2022, Cell Reports
      Citation Excerpt :

      Another consideration in our projection-specific inactivation experiments is that vHC projections to the MO can collateralize to the insular cortex (IC) (Verwer et al., 1997). We think that these collaterals likely did not grossly affect outcomes here, because the IC is associated with Pavlovian (and not instrumental) conditioning (Kusumoto-Yoshida et al., 2015; Nasser et al., 2018; Parkes et al., 2016), including CTA (Ferreira et al., 2002; Gutiérrez et al., 1999), such that inactivation disrupts avoidance of a LiCl-paired food. By contrast, none of our manipulations disrupted CTA, and we did not detect fluorescence in the IC in projection-specific inactivation experiments, suggesting that IC collaterals were sparse and did not grossly affect our experiments (Figure S6).

    • Insula lesions reduce stimulus-driven control of behavior during odor-guided decision-making and autoshaping

      2022, Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Other studies have also demonstrated that anterior insula is involved in devaluation (Balleine and Dickinson, 2000; Parkes et al., 2015; Pelloux et al., 2013) and taste aversion (Cubero et al., 1999; Ferreira et al., 2005; Kayyal et al., 2019; Miranda and McGaugh, 2004). Recent work from the Calu lab has also found that inactivating the pathway between basolateral amygdala and anterior insula reduced food cup contacts and increased the latency to approach the food cup in rats that had previously been identified as goal-trackers (Nasser et al., 2018). However, when comparing our results to this body of work it is important to consider that the nomenclature surrounding insula’s subregions can vary from study to study, and oftentimes can cross over into other categorizations of insula’s anatomy and functions.

    • Functional connectivity of anterior retrosplenial cortex in object recognition memory

      2021, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
      Citation Excerpt :

      Together these results suggest that, during the first stages of memory consolidation, object information is not processed by the ACC, but that it may be recruited by other structures during other memory phases like retrieval. Usually, disconnections studies show that unilateral inactivation or lesions of two brain areas in both hemispheres affects memory, while ipsilateral treatments do not impair memory (Hernandez et al., 2017; Holland, 2007; Keefer & Petrovich, 2020; Nasser, Lafferty, Lesser, Bacharach, & Calu, 2018; Warburton, Baird, Morgan, Muir, & Aggleton, 2001). However, some functional connectivity studies observed impaired memory when transiently inactivating both contralaterally and ipsilaterally (Baker, Rao, Rivera, Garcia, & Mizumori, 2019; Gilmartin, Kwapis, & Helmstetter, 2012; Mathis et al., 2017; Scott et al., 2020).

    • Functional coupling of the orbitofrontal cortex and the basolateral amygdala mediates the association between spontaneous reappraisal and emotional response

      2021, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      The BLA is involved in fear learning, by integrating information from the sensory cortex, thalamus and dACC in people with anxiety (Hakamata et al., 2020). The insula could receive signals of discomfort from the body, and then integrate the signals and send the discomfort information to the amygdala (Nasser et al., 2017; Uematsu et al., 2015). Ferreira et al. (2005) found that the connection between the BLA and insula is important during the formation of disgust feelings.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text