Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 112, 15 May 2015, Pages 327-340
NeuroImage

Parametric variation of gamma frequency and power with luminance contrast: A comparative study of human MEG and monkey LFP and spike responses

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.062Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Parametric effects of contrast on gamma oscillation frequency in human MEG

  • Comparable effects of contrast on gamma oscillation frequency in human MEG, monkey LFP and single unit spike trains

  • Comparable effects of contrast on a novel metric of gamma oscillations; spectral asymmetry, in the three data sources

  • Different effects of contrast on gamma power in MEG data compared to LFP and single unit spike trains

Abstract

Gamma oscillations contribute significantly to the manner in which neural activity is bound into functional assemblies. The mechanisms that underlie the human gamma response, however, are poorly understood. Previous computational models of gamma rely heavily on the results of invasive recordings in animals, and it is difficult to assess whether these models hold in humans. Computational models of gamma predict specific changes in gamma spectral response with increased excitatory drive. Hence, differences and commonalities between spikes, LFPs and MEG in the spectral responses to changes in excitatory drive can lead to a refinement of existing gamma models. We compared gamma spectral responses to varying contrasts in a monkey dataset acquired previously (Roberts et al., 2013) with spectral responses to similar contrast variations in a new human MEG dataset. We found parametric frequency shifts with increasing contrast in human MEG at the single-subject and the single-trial level, analogous to those observed in the monkey. Additionally, we observed parametric modulations of spectral asymmetry, consistent across spikes, LFP and MEG. However, while gamma power scaled linearly with contrast in MEG, it saturated at high contrasts in both the LFP and spiking data. Thus, while gamma frequency changes to varying contrasts were comparable across spikes, LFP and MEG, gamma power changes were not. This indicates that gamma frequency may be a more stable parameter across scales of measurements and species than gamma power. The comparative approach undertaken here represents a fruitful path towards a better understanding of gamma oscillations in the human brain.

Introduction

In monkey visual cortex, gamma oscillations have been studied for many years, and are thought to mediate a number of computational functions (Buzsáki and Wang, 2012, Engel et al., 1999, Fries, 2009, Fries et al., 2007, Tiesinga and Sejnowski, 2010). Gamma oscillations are thought to be related to local inhibition. Because of the inhibition, excitatory neurons show low excitability in general (Buzsáki, 2006). However, the inhibition is thought to vary over time leading to a rhythmic modulation of neuronal excitability, thereby defining windows of opportunity for neurons to fire (Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004, Csicsvari et al., 1999, Lakatos et al., 2005). The length of the information–integration phases, the spacing between them, and the duration of the firing phases, is governed by the frequency of the oscillation. Hence, communication among different neural populations is enabled by an appropriate and consistent relationship in time among these windows of opportunity (Fries, 2005), which requires a sufficient match in the oscillatory frequencies between communicating populations (Roberts et al., 2013). More generally, the theory of weakly coupled oscillators predicts that similar frequencies are a prerequisite for synchronization of nearby neuronal populations (Lowet et al., 2015). Thus, oscillation frequency is a network parameter of utmost functional relevance and theoretical interest (Barardi et al., 2014, Cohen, 2014, Colgin et al., 2009). This is supported by correlations between gamma peak frequency and relevant parameters including perceptual performance (Edden et al., 2009), age (Gaetz et al., 2012), and cortical structure (Schwarzkopf et al., 2012). Power is the other highly relevant feature of spectral responses. Studies in monkeys (Jia et al., 2013, Ray and Maunsell, 2010, Roberts et al., 2013) have shown that power tends to be maximized around a mid-level of contrast or excitatory drive, and a reduction of power at much higher or lower levels of drive is observed suggesting an optimization of resonance at those contrasts. At the local network level, such phenomena require a specific architecture of the network, and hence the study of power modulations as a function of excitatory drive provides another important probe into the underlying gamma generating mechanisms. Further, in spatially-aggregate signals power provides an indirect measure of the strength of the underlying synchronization (Hadjipapas et al., 2009, Musall et al., 2014).

Advances in MEG beamformer source reconstruction have enabled the study of gamma oscillations localized to human visual cortex (Adjamian et al., 2004, Hall et al., 2005, Hoogenboom et al., 2006). Using these methods, several MEG studies reported human gamma oscillations to exhibit marked stimulus specificity (Adjamian et al., 2004, Duncan et al., 2010, Hadjipapas et al., 2007, Hall et al., 2005, Koelewijn et al., 2010, Muthukumaraswamy and Singh, 2008, Perry et al., 2013, Swettenham et al., 2009), paralleling neurophysiological findings in the macaque (Berens et al., 2008, Friedman-Hill et al., 2000, Frien et al., 2000, Gieselmann and Thiele, 2008, Gray and Singer, 1989, Henrie and Shapley, 2005). Although these stimulus-dependent effects in monkeys and humans were recorded in experiments that are difficult to compare, they suggest that gamma may be generated by neural mechanisms, which if not in common, are at least partially shared across the two species and across recording modalities. Yet, it is still not clear whether characteristics of gamma-band activity observed in invasive macaque recordings can be linked in a straightforward manner to characteristics observed from human non-invasive recordings. More specifically, can observed modulations in gamma frequency and power in a human MEG experiment be thought of as equivalent to frequency and power modulations in monkey electrophysiological recordings? This is a non-trivial question of crucial importance for non-invasive EEG/MEG studies on human brain functions that base their hypothesis on prior monkey electrophysiology studies.

Here we investigated whether the functional behavior of gamma oscillation frequency and power in the human is consistent with the behavior observed in the macaque and further whether it is a function of the scales of measurement (spikes, LFP, MEG). To this end, it was necessary to identify a stimulus parameter with robust and well-understood effects on gamma frequency and manipulate this parameter in both human and non-human primates. In computational studies, gamma is often modeled in terms of an interaction between excitation (E) and inhibition (I). In Pyramidal Interneuron Gamma (PING) networks (Tiesinga and Sejnowski, 2009), spikes from E-neurons quickly lead to spiking in I-neurons, which in turn briefly shut down the network. The recurrence of this process leads to oscillations, which are in the gamma range due to the time constants of the neurons involved (Börgers and Kopell, 2005). Increasing E-drive leads to a faster recovery from inhibition, resulting in a higher oscillation frequency (Brunel and Wang, 2003, Buia and Tiesinga, 2006, Tiesinga and Sejnowski, 2009, Traub et al., 1996). The gamma frequency increase predicted to follow from increased E-drive has been confirmed in neurophysiological studies in the macaque, in which gamma oscillation frequency as measured in the LFP recorded from early visual cortex was shown to increase strongly with stimulus contrast (Jia et al., 2013, Ray and Maunsell, 2010, Roberts et al., 2013). Recently contrast-induced frequency shifts have also been shown in human MEG (Perry et al., 2014). Moreover, PING models typically yield an optimal frequency at which gamma power is maximized, with lesser power for frequencies below or above the optimal frequency (Lowet et al., 2015, Roberts et al., 2013). Stimulus contrast can be thought of as an experimentally accessible proxy for E-drive to primary visual cortex (Contreras and Palmer, 2003, Sclar et al., 1990). Hence, parametric variation of contrast is a paradigm that is theoretically well understood such that it may permit the identification of biophysical mechanisms.

We applied this rationale to a human MEG experiment, which was designed for comparison with a previous study in macaque V1 conducted in our group (Roberts et al., 2013). Designing the MEG experiment for comparative purposes was not trivial: on the one hand, one needs to match the monkey and human experiments as closely as possible; at the same time due to the low gamma SNR in the MEG for a number of experimental conditions, some parameters in the MEG experiment need to be adapted such that sufficient SNR could be obtained. Despite the resulting limitations in achieving a perfect comparison, the study benefited from the use of a visual parameter in both studies of which the effects on gamma are well-understood, and from identical spectral analyses of human and macaque data. In addition, in the macaque data we also analyzed the gamma response in single-unit spike trains as a function of visual contrast. We found that increases in visual contrast led to similar shifts in gamma frequency in monkey single unit spiking, in monkey LFP, and in human MEG. Further, we found spectral asymmetries, which were consistently modulated by contrast in all recording modalities (single unit spikes, LFP, MEG). Alongside the similarities between MEG, LFP and spikes, we also observed a major difference: whereas there was a linear increase in the gamma oscillation power with stimulus contrast in the MEG data consistent with previous observations (Hall et al., 2005, Perry et al., 2014), in the LFP data, saturation and even reduction of power were observed at high contrasts.

Overall, the pronounced similarity of these findings across measurement scales and across human and macaque suggests major similarities in the generative model of gamma-band activity in macaque monkeys and humans. However, the pronounced difference in contrast-dependent power modulation indicates that MEG-measured gamma power cannot be easily equated to LFP gamma power. These results as well as theoretical considerations indicate that oscillation frequency might be a more robust parameter for comparing findings across species and spatial scales of measurement. At the same time it will be necessary to explore whether power differences are related to differences in gamma generating mechanisms between macaque monkeys and humans, or to potential differences in signals picked up during invasive neurophysiological recordings and MEG measurements.

Section snippets

Participants

All 9 participants provided informed consent according to the guidelines of the local ethics committee (Commissie Mensgebonden Onderzoek/Committee on Research Involving Human Participants, Region Arnhem—Nijmegen, The Netherlands). The sample consisted of 4 male and 5 female participants. The age range was from 25 to 41 years and all were healthy and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

Task

Participants were positioned comfortably in the MEG scanner, and instructed to sit still while looking at

Comparison of stimuli and experimental designs

In a previous study from our group, Roberts et al. (2013) reported a ~ 25 Hz increase in LFP frequency in V1 and V2 of 2 macaque monkeys as luminance contrast was increased in square-wave gratings from 2% to 72% (grating diameter 3–5°, eccentricity 4–6°, spatial frequency 2 cycles/degree., lower visual field quadrant, 1300 ms analysis time window). To test the generality of the relationship between grating contrast and gamma frequency, we compared this monkey data with MEG data collected from 9

Discussion

In this paper, we present human MEG and monkey electrophysiological (LFP and spiking) recordings obtained in V1, where gamma oscillations were studied under similar stimulation parameters. MEG beamformer source reconstructions are well-suited to make this comparison due to their capacity to localize sources to visual cortex and to improve signal SNR (Brookes et al., 2009, Duncan et al., 2010, Hadjipapas et al., 2007). We used a paradigm where luminance contrast is varied parametrically. Because

Conclusions

Our results in primary visual cortex show that contrast-induced modulations in gamma frequency and gamma spectral asymmetry in human MEG matched the results from macaque microelectrode recordings, including results from well-isolated single unit recordings. However, there were discrepancies in gamma power. The latter is more complex to understand not least because gamma power in the MEG depends on longer-range spatial synchrony. Thus, while oscillation frequency was found to be more stable

Acknowledgments

Research was supported by NWO VICI grant to P.D.W. (#453-04-002), NWO VENI grant to M.R. (#451-09-025), European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under grant agreement no. 604102 (Human Brain Project) to P.D.W. and A.H., Maastricht University Graduate funding in cooperation with the Donders Institute for Brain Behavior and Cognition to P.D.W. All data collection and initial analysis was conducted at the Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour.

Conflict of interest

References (87)

  • S.D. Muthukumaraswamy et al.

    Visual gamma oscillations and evoked responses: variability, repeatability and structural MRI correlates

    NeuroImage

    (2010)
  • G. Perry et al.

    The properties of induced gamma oscillations in human visual cortex show individual variability in their dependence on stimulus size

    NeuroImage

    (2013)
  • G. Perry et al.

    Evidence for increased visual gamma responses in photosensitive epilepsy

    Epilepsy Res.

    (2014)
  • S. Ray et al.

    Differences in gamma frequencies across visual cortex restrict their possible use in computation

    Neuron

    (2010)
  • M.J. Roberts et al.

    Robust gamma coherence between macaque V1 and V2 by dynamic frequency matching

    Neuron

    (2013)
  • G. Sclar et al.

    Coding of image contrast in central visual pathways of the macaque monkey

    Vis. Res.

    (1990)
  • E. Spaak et al.

    Layer-specific entrainment of gamma-band neural activity by the alpha rhythm in monkey visual cortex

    Curr. Biol.

    (2012)
  • P. Tiesinga et al.

    Cortical enlightenment: are attentional gamma oscillations driven by ING or PING?

    Neuron

    (2009)
  • S. Van Pelt et al.

    Visual stimulus eccentricity affects human gamma peak frequency

    NeuroImage

    (2013)
  • M. Vinck et al.

    Attentional modulation of cell-class-specific gamma-band synchronization in awake monkey area V4

    Neuron

    (2013)
  • J. Vrba et al.

    Signal processing in magnetoencephalography

    Methods

    (2001)
  • P. Adjamian et al.

    Induced visual illusions and gamma oscillations in human primary visual cortex

    Eur. J. Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • D.G. Albrecht et al.

    Striate cortex of monkey and cat: contrast response function

    J Neurophysiol

    (1982)
  • A. Barardi et al.

    Phase-coherence transitions and communication in the gamma range between delay-coupled neuronal populations

    PLoS Comput. Biol.

    (2014)
  • P. Berens et al.

    Feature selectivity of the gamma-band of the local field potential in primate primary visual cortex

    Neuroscience

    (2008)
  • C. Börgers et al.

    Effects of noisy drive on rhythms in networks of excitatory and inhibitory neurons

    Neural Comput.

    (2005)
  • C.A. Bosman et al.

    A microsaccadic rhythm modulates gamma-band synchronization and behavior

    J. Neurosci.

    (2009)
  • C.A. Bosman et al.

    Attentional stimulus selection through selective synchronization between monkey visual areas

    Neuron

    (2012)
  • D.H. Brainard

    The Psychophysics Toolbox

    Spat. Vis.

    (1997)
  • N. Brunel et al.

    What determines the frequency of fast network oscillations with irregular neural discharges? I. Synaptic dynamics and excitation–inhibition balance

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2003)
  • N. Brunet et al.

    Visual cortical gamma-band activity during free viewing of natural images

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2013)
  • N.M. Brunet et al.

    Stimulus repetition modulates gamma-band synchronization in primate visual cortex

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2014)
  • E.A. Buffalo et al.

    Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2011)
  • C. Buia et al.

    Attentional modulation of firing rate and synchrony in a model cortical network

    J. Comput. Neurosci.

    (2006)
  • S.P. Burns et al.

    Searching for autocoherence in the cortical network with a time–frequency analysis of the local field potential

    J. Neurosci.

    (2010)
  • S.P. Burns et al.

    Is gamma-band activity in the local field potential of V1 cortex a “clock” or filtered noise?

    J. Neurosci.

    (2011)
  • G. Buzsáki

    Rhythms of the Brain

    (2006)
  • G. Buzsáki et al.

    Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks

    Science

    (2004)
  • G. Buzsáki et al.

    Mechanisms of gamma oscillations

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • G. Buzsáki et al.

    The origin of extracellular fields and currents—EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • J.A. Cardin et al.

    Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses

    Nature

    (2009)
  • M.X. Cohen

    Fluctuations in oscillation frequency control spike timing and coordinate neural networks

    J. Neurosci.

    (2014)
  • L.L. Colgin et al.

    Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus

    Nature

    (2009)
  • Cited by (49)

    • Predictive coding of natural images by V1 firing rates and rhythmic synchronization

      2022, Neuron
      Citation Excerpt :

      In addition to predictability, natural images also show substantial variability in luminance contrast (Figure S4A). Luminance contrast influences the contextual modulation of sensory responses (Sceniak et al., 1999; Kapadia et al., 1999; Cavanaugh et al., 2002) and correlates positively with γ-synchronization for artificial grating stimuli (Henrie and Shapley, 2005; Roberts et al., 2013; Hadjipapas et al., 2015). We therefore wondered how the influence of predictability on neural activity depended on luminance contrast.

    • Empirically constrained network models for contrast-dependent modulation of gamma rhythm in V1

      2021, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, contrast affects crucial network interactions in PING models. However, we found gamma power in LFP to saturate or even decrease at high contrast (Hadjipapas et al., 2015; Roberts et al., 2013). This finding has not been widely studied and is poorly understood.

    • Spatial suppression in visual motion perception is driven by inhibition: Evidence from MEG gamma oscillations

      2020, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      The combination of regular orientation, high contrast, large size, and slow motion velocity of the grating facilitated high amplitude gamma oscillations. Indeed, each of these visual features enhances GRs in the primary visual cortex in humans and animals (Gieselmann and Thiele, 2008; Hadjipapas et al., 2015; Hermes et al., 2015; Jia et al., 2011, 2013; Lima et al., 2010; Orekhova et al., 2015, 2018; Perry, 2015; Perry et al., 2013, 2015; Ray and Maunsell, 2015; van Pelt and Fries, 2013). The crucial factor affecting visual gamma oscillations seems to be the size of the visual stimulus --- gamma is generated only when the stimulus is large enough to engage both the center and the inhibitory surround of the neurons’ receptive fields (Gieselmann and Thiele, 2008; Shushruth et al., 2012).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Former address: Radboud University Nijmegen, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

    2

    Shared first authorship.

    View full text