Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 49, Issue 2, 15 January 2010, Pages 1717-1727
NeuroImage

Tease or threat? Judging social interactions from bodily expressions

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.065Get rights and content

Abstract

We casually observe many interactions that do not really concern us. Yet sometimes we need to be able to rapidly appraise whether an interaction between two people represents a real threat for one of them rather than an innocent tease. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we investigated whether small differences in the body language of two interacting people are picked up by the brain even if observers are performing an unrelated task. Fourteen participants were scanned while watching 3-s movies (192 trials and 96 scrambles) showing a male person either threatening or teasing a female one. In one task condition, observers categorized the interaction as threatening or teasing, and in the other, they monitored randomly appearing dots and categorized the color. Our results clearly show that right amygdala responds more to threatening than to teasing situations irrespective of the observers' task. When observers' attention is not explicitly directed to the situation, this heightened amygdala activation goes together with increased activity in body sensitive regions in fusiform gyrus, extrastriate body area—human motion complex and superior temporal sulcus and is associated with a better behavioral performance of the participants during threatening situations. In addition, regions involved in action observation (inferior frontal gyrus, temporoparietal junction, and inferior parietal lobe) and preparation (premotor, putamen) show increased activation for threat videos. Also regions involved in processing moral violations (temporoparietal junction, hypothalamus) reacted selectively to the threatening interactions. Taken together, our results show which brain regions react selectively to witnessing a threatening interaction even if the situation is not attended because the observers perform an unrelated task.

Section snippets

Participants

Fourteen healthy volunteers (five males; 23.6 ± 5.1 years; all right-handed) participated in this experiment after providing written informed consent. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The study was performed in accordance to the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the local medical ethical committee.

Materials

Fourteen students (seven males) of the University of Tilburg were filmed while they were engaged into a social interaction which always involved one male and one

Behavioral results

Threatening situations were well recognized as shown in the average recognition rates during the emotion naming task ( mean = 83.9%, SD = 11.15) and participants also performed well in the color naming task (mean =  90.8%, SD = 8.32) (see Fig. 2). A repeated-measures ANOVA showed that there was an emotion effect (F(1,10) = 5.455, p = .042, η p2 = .353) as well as a task effect (F(1,10) = 13.875, p = .004, η p2 = .581) indicating that recognition rates were highest in the color naming condition and for threatening

Discussion

Our goal was to investigate the brain regions associated with witnessing an interaction between two people in which one person threatens the other and to assess whether explicitly paying attention to the situation makes a significant difference. Our major results are that right AMG is active in the attended as well as unattended threat condition while the body processing regions FG, EBA–hMT+/V5, and STS only for unattended threat. In contrast, left IFG responds specifically to threatening

Conclusion

We showed that right AMG is involved in witnessing threatening situations the observer is not part of, also when not actually paying attention to the situation. This AMG activation during the color naming task co-occurred with activations in body processing regions FG, EBA–hMT+/V5, and STS.

Regions involved in action perception (IFG and TPJ) responded more when the interaction was threatening and when attention was directed explicitly to it. Also left IPL showed a heightened response to

Acknowledgments

This project was funded in part by EU project COBOL (FDP6-NEST-043403).

We thank the reviewers for their useful comments.

References (48)

  • HoshiE. et al.

    Functional specialization in dorsal and ventral premotor areas

    Prog. Brain Res.

    (2004)
  • IacoboniM. et al.

    Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • KoberH. et al.

    Functional grouping and cortical–subcortical interactions in emotion: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies

    NeuroImage

    (2008)
  • LeDouxJ.E.

    Brain mechanisms of emotion and emotional learning

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (1992)
  • LuL.H. et al.

    Category-specific naming deficits for objects and actions: semantic attribute and grammatical role hypotheses

    Neuropsychologia

    (2002)
  • NomuraM. et al.

    Frontal lobe networks for effective processing of ambiguously expressed emotions in humans

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2003)
  • SaxeR. et al.

    People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • SimmonsA. et al.

    Affective ambiguity for a group recruits ventromedial prefrontal cortex

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • VuilleumierP. et al.

    Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study

    Neuron

    (2001)
  • WilliamsM.A. et al.

    Differential amygdala responses to happy and fearful facial expressions depend on selective attention

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • WinstonJ.S. et al.

    Common and distinct neural responses during direct and incidental processing of multiple facial emotions

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • AdolphsR.

    Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour

    Nat. Rev., Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • BarbasH. et al.

    Serial pathways from primate prefrontal cortex to autonomic areas may influence emotional expression

    BMC Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • BeauchampM.S. et al.

    FMRI responses to video and point-light displays of moving humans and manipulable objects

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • Cited by (94)

    • Participation of visual association areas in social processing emerges when rTPJ is inhibited

      2022, eNeurologicalSci
      Citation Excerpt :

      At neural level, an explanation for visual association activation might be related with an enhancing attention mechanism according to the emotional salience of the socio – emotional stimuli. In the one hand, it has been shown repeatedly that occipital areas like those that we found as significant, has been related with emotional information processing [19,25,26,43,44] and more specifically with negative valence information [42] likewise when people deceive others during a money distribution [52]. On the other hand, same areas have been shown as active when participants listening to socially salient information as angry related stimuli compared with neutral sounds [39] or when avatars engage in social interaction with participants compared when avatar doesn't [41], or when processing highly socially provocative aggression [22].

    • Sex differences in neural mechanisms of social and non-social threat monitoring

      2021, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Sex differences in functional brain activation to social threat have also been found in regions associated with aspects of threat-related processing (McClure et al., 2004; Shirao et al., 2005; Veroude et al., 2014). These regions include frontotemporal cortical areas such as the precentral gyrus, which is involved in social threat processing (Anderson et al., 2013; Kret et al., 2011; Sinke et al., 2010), the inferior frontal gyrus which is involved in self-focused threat processing, and the medial temporal gyrus which is involved in reward learning (Beckes et al., 2013). Subcortical regions are also implicated in social threat processing.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text