Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 22, Issue 2, June 2004, Pages 688-697
NeuroImage

Where arousal meets attention: a simultaneous fMRI and EEG recording study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.048Get rights and content

Abstract

In this fMRI study, we looked for the regions supporting interaction between cortical arousal and attention during three conditions: detection, observation, and rest. Arousal measurements were obtained from the EEG low-frequency (LF) power (5–9.5 Hz) recorded continuously together with fMRI. Whatever the condition, arousal was positively correlated with the fMRI signal of the right dorsal-lateral prefrontal and superior parietal cortices, closely overlapping regions involved in the maintenance of attention. Although the inferior temporal areas also presented a correlation with arousal during detection, path analysis suggests that this influence may be indirect, through the top-down influence of the previously mentioned network. However, those visual-processing areas could account for the correlation between arousal and performances. Lastly, the medial frontal cortex, frontal opercula, and thalamus were inversely correlated with arousal but only during detection and observation so that they could account for the control of arousal.

Introduction

Vigilance or sustained attention is the ability to maintain a performance over a period of time. It is classically explored using tasks that demand detection of changes occurring at random intervals (Mackworth, 1957). Vigilance is an important function in everyday life, as its deficit is said to account for accidents (Haraldsson et al., 1990) and is observed in disorders, for example, schizophrenia (Green, 1998), hyperactivity, epilepsy, or closed head injury (Parasuraman et al., 1998). Fluctuations of performances in vigilance tasks are, at least partially, caused by interaction between attention and arousal (Parasuraman et al., 1998). But little is known about the actual relationship between them, and this is what this study attempts to investigate: does arousal modulate the whole cortex? Or does it only affect specific regions? And if so, which component of the attentional process is affected? Indeed, attention involves two orienting systems that permit to direct it and various sites where it influences information processing. The sites where attention biases information processing are those areas devoted to the processing of the type of information attended to, for example, the inferotemporal areas in the case of visual stimuli. Attention can be oriented by two systems: the top-down and the bottom-up networks (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). The first is the endogenous orienting system that permits to focus voluntarily on specific events. It is a controlled process that relies on a network involving the dorsal-lateral prefrontal cortex [namely, the superior frontal sulcus for spatial items and inferior frontal sulcus for objects (Pardo et al., 1991)] and the parietal cortex (the intraparietal sulcus), subsequently called the top-down network. The second is the exogenous orienting system which permits incoming information to catch attention especially when it is biologically salient, for example, a novel event. It is an automatic process that relies on the inferior frontal gyrus and the temporal-parietal junction principally on the right side (Downar et al., 2000). This network will be subsequently called the bottom-up network. The question is if arousal affects only specific areas, where does it meet attention: on its sites, in the bottom-up or in the top-down network?

To localize these orienting networks, they were dissociated during a rare target detection task: subjects were presented with a succession of visual stimuli including frequent distractors (

) and never-repeated oddballs; half of which were letters and the other half were pictures. The seven participants were presented series of intermingled pictures and letters randomly distributed among frequent distractors. They were requested to detect, in turn, either only letters or only pictures (detection blocks) or to passively look at the series of stimuli (observation blocks). Between the detection and observation blocks, the subjects could relax with their eyes closed (resting blocks). Orienting to exogenous events was supposed to be activated by rare stimuli whatever their type is, that is, letter or picture, and no matter which task the subjects had to perform: press a button to signal the detection of a rare stimulus or ignore it, in the detection blocks or passively look at it in the observation blocks. Accordingly, the regions commonly activated by these six conditions (two types of stimuli × three tasks) or, in other words, their conjunction (Price and Friston, 1997) ought to correspond to the bottom-up network. The paradigm was planned in such a way that the predicted event-related and block-related BOLD responses were roughly decorrelated Chawla et al., 1999, Donaldson et al., 2001, Otten et al., 2002, Visscher et al., 2003. This permitted to identify periods of endogenous maintenance of attention without oddball presentation. Consequently, the conjunction of letter detection and picture detection relative to the observation blocks should activate the top-down network.

To address the issue of arousal measurements, previous works relied either on EEG Danos et al., 2001, Lindgren et al., 1999, self-rating, or pharmacological manipulation Coull et al., 1997, Portas et al., 1998. Experiments in which EEG was used as a means to measure cortical arousal relied on the inverse correlation of arousal with the power of low EEG frequencies during awakening. Although some studies focused on the alpha band as the marker of arousal (Danos et al., 2001) (8 to 12 Hz), the 5- to 9.5-Hz band seems to be the most sensitive: it increases with the first sleep stage (Niedermeyer, 1999a), it increases after sleep deprivation (Aeschbach et al., 2001), it is proportional to subjects' self-rating of arousal or sleepiness Lafrance and Dumont, 2000, Strijkstra et al., 2003, and it is correlated with poorer performances Daniel, 1967, Makeig and Jung, 1996 and especially with their degradation as a function of time, that is, the vigilance decrement Horvath et al., 1976, Makeig and Inlow, 1993. EEG can provide an evaluation of arousal with an accuracy of 1 s, which is about the time scale of fMRI sensitivity. In previous studies, arousal was defined as a constant value over the scanning time defining it as a global or average level. This concept, further referred to as “arousal level,” is generally manipulated by means of sleep deprivation or drugs. However, there are indications that faster fluctuations may be even more relevant Doran et al., 2001, Makeig et al., 2000. These fluctuations are spontaneous and probably related to dynamic interaction between different components of the arousal system. Indeed, self-rating of the arousal level does not correlate as well with the mean low-frequency (LF) power, as it does with the standard deviation (SD) (i.e., amplitude of power fluctuations) (Oken et al., 1995). In other words, a low arousal level is better described as larger arousal fluctuations than as an average decrease in arousal Daniel, 1967, Doran et al., 2001, Makeig et al., 2000. Combining EEG with fMRI permits to correlate short-term changes in cortical arousal with BOLD signal variations.

Besides these endogenous fluctuations, arousal systems, such as the noradrenergic locus coeruleus and the cholinergic nucleus of the basal forebrain, also present a transient increase in activity following the presentation of a rare stimulus Aston-Jones et al., 1999, Rajkowski et al., 1994. Such exogenous stimulation of arousal goes together with a decrease in EEG low-frequency power, that is, the “event-related desynchronization” (Pfurtscheller and Aranibar, 1977), whose amplitude depends on the physical features and novelty of the stimulus (Aston-Jones et al., 1999). One might speculate that endogenous arousal and arousal ensuing exogenous stimulation modulate different regions (Sturm and Willmes, 2001), for example, depending on the activity of these regions. Accordingly, exogenous and endogenous arousal must be dissociated: the EEG low-frequency power corresponding to the event-related desynchronization was analyzed separately from the parts corresponding to the endogenous fluctuations of arousal during the blocks.

Section snippets

Participants

Seven right-handed subjects (three females, ranging in age from 22 to 28 years) with no prior history of neurological injury gave written informed consent before participating in this study approved by the local ethical committee of Alsace. The subjects were nonsmokers and were free of medication. To achieve a homogenous arousal level for all the subjects, each fMRI sittings started at the same time (14 h 30 min), and the participants were not allowed to smoke or drink stimulants (tea, coffee)

Behavioral data

During the detection blocks, the seven subjects accurately detected 94.4% ± 7% of the targets, while the response button was pressed erroneously in 2.1% ± 0.8% of the cases. Therefore, subjects had a good sensitivity (d′: 3.8 ± 0.4) with a bias towards conservative decision criteria (β: 2.8 ± 4.2).

Electrophysiological data

An important point is that the response time (mean = 536 ± 57 ms) was correlated with the power of the 5- to 9.5-Hz frequency band: the higher the power of low frequencies 1 s before stimulus

Discussion

Distinguishing attention maintenance from event processing is a usual issue in studies on sustained attention (Pardo et al., 1991). Most recent studies have attempted to achieve such discrimination, one of which by using warned reaction time tasks, where a variable interval of a few seconds separated the warning cue from the go signal, corresponding to the maintenance of attention (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). In our study, the block-event design permitted to extrapolate those results to

Conclusion

In this study on young healthy subjects, arousal appeared to constantly affect the top-down network without direct effect on the bottom-up network or input processing areas. The impact of arousal was effective, regardless of the origin (endogenous or exogenous) of its fluctuations and whatever the ongoing task. The effect of arousal on the reaction time is probably indirect through top-down influence on the inferior temporal areas. Conversely, the medial frontal regions, frontal opercula, and

Acknowledgements

We are thankful to Pr. Jean-Marie Danion and Pr. Daniel Grucker for reading the manuscript and their fruitful criticism. We would also like to thank Mrs. Corinne Marrer for her hardworking contribution in the acquisition of fMRI data and Mrs. Monique Noël for skillful electrode placement. We are grateful to Pr. Guy Foucher and Ms Nathalie Heider for correcting our manuscript. This work was supported by the UMR 7004 ULP/CNRS-Strasbourg.

References (57)

  • G Pfurtscheller et al.

    Event-related cortical desynchronization detected by power measurements of scalp EEG

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1977)
  • C.J Price et al.

    Cognitive conjunction: a new approach to brain activation experiments

    NeuroImage

    (1997)
  • J Rajkowski et al.

    Locus caeruleus activity in monkey: phasic and tonic changes are associated with altered vigilance

    Brain Res. Bull.

    (1994)
  • A.A Stevens et al.

    Event-related fMRI of auditory and visual oddball tasks

    Magn. Reson. Imaging

    (2000)
  • A.M Strijkstra et al.

    Subjective sleepiness correlates negatively with global alpha (8–12 Hz) and positively with central frontal theta (4–8 Hz) frequencies in the human resting awake electroencephalogram

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2003)
  • W Sturm et al.

    On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness

    NeuroImage

    (2001)
  • K.M Visscher et al.

    Mixed blocked/event-related designs separate transient and sustained activity in fMRI

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • G Aston-Jones et al.

    Attention

  • K.A Bollen

    Structural Equations with Latent Variables

    (1989)
  • D Chawla et al.

    The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • C.R Clark et al.

    Role of monoamine pathways in attention and effort: effects of clonidine and methylphenidate in normal adult humans

    Psychopharmacology (Berl.)

    (1986)
  • C.R Clark et al.

    Role of monoamine pathways in the control of attention: effects of droperidol and methylphenidate in normal adult humans

    Psychopharmacology (Berl.)

    (1986)
  • M Corbetta et al.

    Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain

    Nat. Rev., Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • M Corbetta et al.

    Attentional modulation of neural processing of shape, color, and velocity in humans

    Science

    (1990)
  • J.T Coull et al.

    The neural correlates of the noradrenergic modulation of human attention, arousal and learning

    Eur. J. Neurosci.

    (1997)
  • R.S Daniel

    Alpha and theta EEG in vigilance

    Percept. Mot. Skills

    (1967)
  • P Danos et al.

    EEG alpha rhythm and glucose metabolic rate in the thalamus in schizophrenia

    Neuropsychobiology

    (2001)
  • K.D Davis et al.

    Human anterior cingulate cortex neurons modulated by attention-demanding tasks

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2000)
  • Cited by (93)

    • EEG coherence during subjectively-rated psychological state variations

      2020, International Journal of Psychophysiology
    • Awakening after a sleeping pill: Restoring functional brain networks after severe brain injury

      2020, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      Neurophysiological activity is well-known to change in different behavioral conditions. For instance, increasing arousal is associated with clear decreases in delta, theta, and (lower) alpha activity, which might explain the reduction seen in these frequency bands in our patient (Foucher, Otzenberger, & Gounot, 2004; Niedermeyer & Lopes da Silva, 1999). Experiments in which EEG's were used as means to measure cortical arousal previously even relied on the inverse correlation of arousal with the power of low EEG frequencies during awakening (Danos, Guich, Abel, & Buchsbaum, 2001; Foucher et al., 2004).

    • MinR 10/20 system: Quantitative and reproducible cranial landmark setting method for MRI based on minimum initial reference points

      2016, Journal of Neuroscience Methods
      Citation Excerpt :

      Finally, 329 landmarks on the scalp are available using the 10/5 landmark system (Oostenveld and Praamstra, 2001; Jurcak et al., 2007). These systems have been not only the de-facto standard methods for procedures such as placing electrodes for EEG (Babiloni et al., 2003), locating EEG dipoles on a patient's MRI (Pascau et al., 2000), optode placement for fNIRS (Homae et al., 2010; Watanabe et al., 2013) and TMS (transcranial magnetic stimulation) positioning (Herwig et al., 2003; Beam et al., 2009), but have also been applied to describe the relationship between scalp and cortical anatomies (Blume et al., 1974; Homan et al., 1987; Lagerlund et al., 1993; Okamoto et al., 2004), tomographic measurements including simultaneous EEG–fMRI studies (Foucher et al., 2004; Mulert et al., 2004) and fNIRS–fMRI studies (Schroeter et al., 2006; Sasai et al., 2012). Moreover, the international 10/20 system provides intermediate cranial landmarks for the probabilistic spatial registration methods that use a reference-MRI database instead of a subject's own MRI (Tsuzuki and Dan, 2014).

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Part of these results was presented at the “fMRI experiment V” (London, 10–11th March 2003) and “HBM 2003” (New York, 18–22nd June 2003).

    View full text