Elsevier

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

Volume 68, September 2016, Pages 530-536
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

A neural circuit encoding sexual preference in humans

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.025Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Identification of a core neural circuit using meta-analytic remodeling.

  • Sexual preference is encoded by phylogenetically old, subcortical brain structures.

  • Hypothalamus, thalamus, septal area, and para-/hippocampus control sexual preference.

  • Substantia innominata regulates sexual non-preference.

Abstract

Sexual preference determines mate choice for reproduction and hence guarantees conservation of species in mammals. Despite this fundamental role in human behavior, current knowledge on its target-specific neurofunctional substrate is based on lesion studies and therefore limited. We used meta-analytic remodeling of neuroimaging data from 364 human subjects with diverse sexual interests during sexual stimulation to quantify neural regions associated with sexual preference manipulations. We found that sexual preference is encoded by four phylogenetically old, subcortical brain structures. More specifically, sexual preference is controlled by the anterior and preoptic area of the hypothalamus, the anterior and mediodorsal thalamus, the septal area, and the perirhinal parahippocampus including the dentate gyrus. In contrast, sexual non-preference is regulated by the substantia innominata. We anticipate the identification of a core neural circuit for sexual preferences to be a starting point for further sophisticated investigations into the neural principles of sexual behavior and particularly of its aberrations.

Introduction

Sexuality is a central component of human behavior. In both animals and humans, ⿿nature displays an interesting range of sexual behaviors, some of which may offend some of us⿿ (Gross, 2014). These behaviors are significantly affected by individual sexual preferences. In humans, sexual preferences refer to the favored way of reaching orgasm and to corresponding preferred sexual activities or preferred types of sexual partners (Langevin, 1983, Quinsey, 2003). These preferences hence reflect the ultimate choice of a sexual activity/partner over another that one would make (Bailey, 2009). Albeit often conflated, researchers make efforts to distinguish between sexual preference and sexual orientation. While sexual orientation is considered a ⿿stable and enduring internal preference for same- versus opposite-sex sexual interactions and partnering⿿, sexual preference is assumed to represent ⿿the manifestation of behavioral choices rather than a stable internal predisposition⿿ (Bailey and Zuk, 2009). Yet, the nomenclatural shift from sexual preference to sexual orientation has also been challenged. Particularly, it has been noted that it may overemphasize biological determination while neglecting psychological and sociocultural dimensions, but on the other hand also disregard that ⿿choices are seldom if ever entirely volitional⿿ (De Cecco and Parker, 1995). The debate on definitions seems heated at times, and it has been noted that a consistent definition of sexual orientation is still lacking (Moser, 2016). Nobody may disagree that hetero- and homosexuality represent sexual orientations (Moser, 2016). However, the notion of, for instance, asexuality (Bogaert, 2015) and pedophilia (Seto, 2012) as sexual orientations or the consideration of absence of sexual orientation in women (Bailey, 2009) are highly controversial. This mini-review is not limited to same-sex and opposite-sex preferences (i.e., sexual orientation) but includes also sexual preferences relating to other variables such as age (i.e., disorders of sexual preference). Therefore, it employs the term and refers to sexual preference as the preferred way of reaching orgasm concerning particular sexual activities or types of partners (Langevin, 1983, Quinsey, 2003).

Evidence for influence of epi-/genetic mechanisms but also of emotional maternal bond have fueled the controversial debate over the origin of sexual preferences, which has centered on the nature vs. nurture dichotomy (Hamer et al., 1993, Kendrick et al., 1998, Liu et al., 2011, Majerus et al., 1982, Ngun and Vilain, 2014). A quasi intermediate model between the ⿿nature⿿ and ⿿nurture⿿ view on the basis of genetic, hormone, and birth order studies proposes that sexual preference in humans may be stipulated in neural circuitry during early fetal development (Rahman, 2005). It has therefore been advocated for a future focus on elucidating ⿿the fundamental neural architecture underlying the target-specific direction of human sexual orientation⿿ (Rahman, 2005). A postmortem study had reported that the interstitial nuclei of the anterior hypothalamus (group 3; INAH 3) were twice as large in heterosexual men as not only in heterosexual women but also in homosexual men (LeVay, 1991). The inferred thesis of neuroanatomical sexual dimorphism being a biological substrate for sexual preference was further tested using modern neuroimaging techniques. Positron emission tomography (PET) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) showed sex-atypical (i.e., opposite-sex-like) cerebral asymmetry and functional connections (of the amygdala) in homosexual subjects (Savic and Lindström, 2008). The findings of these pioneering experiments seem plausible, given that homosexual subjects⿿ sexual preference is similar to that of their opposite sex. The results however leave open whether these sexually dimorphic structures are significantly involved in the functional brain response to sexual stimuli that is specific to one⿿s sexual preference, i.e., the neural basis underlying its target-specific direction.

Notably, the only MRI study that explicitly aimed at unraveling the functional neural correlates of sexual orientation in humans found stronger neuronal response to preferred as compared to non-preferred visual sexual stimuli in the reward and motor system (including ventral striatum, centromedian thalamus, and ventral premotor cortex) but not in the suggested candidate regions (Ponseti et al., 2006). The data currently available regarding consistently recruited areas mediating sexual preferences hence seems contradictory and the initial question remains unresolved. When however reviewing the rich corpus of studies investigating human brain response to sexual stimuli, we noticed that a considerable proportion implicitly also assessed brain activations related to sexual preference by comparing preferred with relatively non-preferred sexual stimuli, which were used as control stimuli, in various subject groups. In other words, we became aware of the existence of an extensive data pool that seemed ideally suited for identifying neural regions associated with sexual preference manipulations when recapitulated and re-analyzed in this context.

This meta-analytic mini-review reassesses if sexual preference-related brain activity is actually found in regions where previous neuroanatomical studies located sexual preference-related changes in brain anatomy. The overlap and link between brain structure and function might serve as evidence that the candidate regions are indeed functionally involved in encoding sexual preference and related neuroanatomical variations not merely epiphenomena. Such spatial localization could provide further hints at the temporal manifestation of sexual preferences and confine the spectrum of neurotransmitters and receptors that regulate this aspect of personality (LeVay, 1991).

Section snippets

Data selection

We applied a similar search and selection strategy as a previous meta-analysis of neural sexual stimulus processing in men (Poeppl et al., 2014). A stepwise procedure to identify the relevant experimental studies was used. First, we selected studies through a standard search in the PubMed (http://www.pubmed.gov) and ISI Web of Science (http://apps.isiknowledge.com) databases using the terms ⿿sexual⿿ or ⿿erotic⿿ in combination with ⿿fMRI⿿, ⿿functional MRI⿿, ⿿functional magnetic resonance⿿,

Results

We systematically collected the appropriate studies. We then used activation likelihood estimation (ALE) (Fox et al., 2014) to meta-analytically remodel available neuroimaging data from 364 subjects including healthy hetero- and homosexual individuals as well as individuals with gender identity and sexual preference disorders (i.e., transsexualism and pedophilia). Our analyses identified a robust neural pattern of activity, idiosyncratic for encoding sexual preference, that converged in four

Discussion

It is remarkable that the identified neural circuit encoding sexual preference exclusively encompasses phylogenetically old, subcortical brain structures, but no regions of the neocortex. In the light of Haeckel⿿s notion that ontogenesis is a brief and rapid recapitulation of phylogenesis (Haeckel, 1899), this distinctive neuroanatomical feature supports the view of an in utero determination of sexual preference during early fetal development (Bao and Swaab, 2011). The lack in involvement of

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Sylvia Dorner-Mitschke and Ines Poeppl for assistance in data keying. This study was in part supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG; BZ 2/2-1, BZ 2/3-1, EI 816/4-1, EI 816/6-1, LA 3071/3-1), the National Institute of Mental Health (R01-MH074457), the Helmholtz Portfolio Theme ⿿Supercomputing and Modeling for the Human Brain⿿ and the European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under grant agreement no. 604102 (Human Brain Project).

All authors

References (55)

  • A.R. Laird et al.

    Comparison of the disparity between Talairach and MNI coordinates in functional neuroimaging data: validation of the Lancaster transform

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • Y. Liu et al.

    Molecular regulation of sexual preference revealed by genetic studies of 5-HT in the brains of male mice

    Nature

    (2011)
  • T.C. Ngun et al.

    The biological basis of human sexual orientation: is there a role for epigenetics?

    Adv. Genet.

    (2014)
  • L.D. Nickerson et al.

    A tool for comparison of PET and fMRI methods: calculation of the uncertainty in the location of an activation site in a PET image

    Neuroimage

    (2001)
  • J.G. Pfaus

    Neurobiology of sexual behavior

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (1999)
  • J. Ponseti et al.

    A functional endophenotype for sexual orientation in humans

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • Q. Rahman

    The neurodevelopment of human sexual orientation

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2005)
  • R.L. Roof

    The dentate gyrus is sexually dimorphic in prepubescent rats: testosterone plays a significant role

    Brain Res.

    (1993)
  • J. Spape et al.

    Gender-specific genital and subjective sexual arousal to prepotent sexual features in heterosexual women and men

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2014)
  • S. Stoléru et al.

    Functional neuroimaging studies of sexual arousal and orgasm in healthy men and women: a review and meta-analysis

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2012)
  • M. Walter et al.

    Distinguishing specific sexual and general emotional effects in fMRI⿿subcortical and cortical arousal during erotic picture viewing

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • L. Zaborszky et al.

    Stereotaxic probabilistic maps of the magnocellular cell groups in human basal forebrain

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • K. Amunts et al.

    Cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human amygdala, hippocampal region and entorhinal cortex: intersubject variability and probability maps

    Anat. Embryol. (Berl.)

    (2005)
  • V.V. Amunts

    Structural asymmetry of the basal nucleus of Meynert in men and women

    Neurosci. Behav. Physiol.

    (2007)
  • J.M. Bailey

    What is sexual orientation and do women have one?

    Nebr. Symp. Motiv.

    (2009)
  • A.D. Baird et al.

    Neurological control of human sexual behaviour: insights from lesion studies

    J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry

    (2007)
  • T.E. Behrens et al.

    Non-invasive mapping of connections between human thalamus and cortex using diffusion imaging

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • Cited by (26)

    • Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation as a Potential Tool to Reduce Sexual Arousal: A Proof of Concept Study

      2020, Journal of Sexual Medicine
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although the side effects of deep brain stimulation in general might be lesser than systemic side effects of antiandrogenic drug therapies, it is undeniable that side effects of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques might even be comparatively negligible. Considering not only the involvement in sexual arousal but also its crucial role for encoding sexual preference,33 neurostimulation of the hypothalamus might lead to unwanted side effects regarding sexual orientation, for example altered sexual partner preference as observed in male ferrets with lesions of the preoptic area/anterior hypothalamus.34 In contrast, our non-invasive approach of cortical stimulation may provide a more specific, better tolerated, and safer method that is easy to apply.

    • The biological basis of sexual orientation: How hormonal, genetic, and environmental factors influence to whom we are sexually attracted

      2019, Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Similar to the vertebrates mentioned above, steroid receptors are expressed in the human brain; thus, sex steroids control the differentiation of gender-typical external and internal genital structures during embryonic life (Ball et al., 2014). The regional size and cell density in several brain structures have been shown to differ between men and women and between homosexual and heterosexual individuals (Poeppl et al., 2016). A summary of the differences in brain structure and function along with structural connection and functional responses related to sexual orientation is shown in Fig. 1.

    • No Sex Difference Found: Cues of Sexual Stimuli Activate the Reward System in both Sexes

      2019, Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      The higher activations in the ventral thalamus and hypothalamus in women in comparison to men were unexpected. Most previous studies, which investigated sex differences of neural responses towards SEM, found similar activity patterns in any brain region (Poeppl et al., 2016a; Mitricheva et al., 2019). A possible explanation why previous studies failed to register this sex difference might be a methodological one.

    • The neural basis of sex differences in sexual behavior: A quantitative meta-analysis

      2016, Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology
      Citation Excerpt :

      This may account for the absence of any significant cortical role in rodent sexual arousal in either sex. In contrast, sexual preference is exclusively controlled by subcortical regions in both rodents and humans (Balthazart, 2016; Poeppl et al., 2016; Sakuma, 2008). The brain regions that consistently respond to sexual stimuli in men and women according to our meta-analyses might be considered as potential targets for manipulations in an effort to modulate sexual arousal.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text