Elsevier

Cortex

Volume 94, September 2017, Pages 131-141
Cortex

Research report
No evidential value in samples of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) studies of cognition and working memory in healthy populations

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.06.021Get rights and content

Abstract

A substantial number of studies have been published over the last decade, claiming that transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) can influence performance on cognitive tasks. However, there is some skepticism regarding the efficacy of tDCS, and evidence from meta-analyses are mixed. One major weakness of these meta-analyses is that they only examine outcomes in published studies. Given biases towards publishing positive results in the scientific literature, there may be a substantial “file-drawer” of unpublished negative results in the tDCS literature. Furthermore, multiple researcher degrees of freedom can also inflate published p-values. Recently, Simonsohn, Nelson and Simmons (2014) created a novel meta-analytic tool that examines the distribution of significant p-values in a literature, and compares it to expected distributions with different effect sizes. Using this tool, one can assess whether the selected studies have evidential value. Therefore, we examined a random selection of studies that used tDCS to alter performance on cognitive tasks, and tDCS studies on working memory in a recently published meta-analysis (Mancuso et al., 2016). Using a p-curve analysis, we found no evidence that the tDCS studies had evidential value (33% power or greater), with the estimate of statistical power of these studies being approximately 14% for the cognitive studies, and 5% (what would be expected from randomly generated data) for the working memory studies. It is likely that previous tDCS studies are substantially underpowered, and we provide suggestions for future research to increase the evidential value of future tDCS studies.

Introduction

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is an affordable, non-invasive technique used to electrically stimulate the brain. Typically, two electrode pads (a positively charged anode and a negatively charged cathode) are placed on the participant. A relatively weak current (typically 1–2 mA) then runs from the cathode to the anode. This current is thought to change the resting membrane potential of neurons, resulting in hyperpolarization (less activity) under the cathode, and hypopolarization (more activity) under the anode (Bindman et al., 1962, Nitsche and Paulus, 2000), along with long-term potentiation/depression-like plasticity after (respectively) anodal/cathodal stimulation (Stagg & Nitsche, 2011). This technique has been used to study changes in motor cortex excitability and motor learning (Nitsche et al., 2003). In addition to research on motor processes, tDCS has more recently been applied to a number of other domains, including altering cognitive (Sparing, Dafotakis, Meister, Thirugnanasambandam, & Fink, 2008) and emotional (Boggio, Zaghi, & Fregni, 2009) function in typical populations, as a therapy for stroke sufferers (Fregni, Boggio, Mansur, et al., 2005) and individuals with mental illness (Boggio et al., 2008), and to augment athletic training in high-level athletes (Banissy & Muggleton, 2013). In addition, it is inexpensive (with commercially available devices costing less than $200) and safe (Bikson et al., 2016). If such a simple device can be used to improve performance in all of these domains, its application could revolutionize brain science and rehabilitation. Therefore, it is of critical importance that claims regarding its effectiveness be examined and scrutinized.

There has been a substantial increase in the number of published tDCS studies, including studies of tDCS and cognitive processes, over the last five years.1 Intuitively, the sheer number of manuscripts claiming an effect of tDCS on cognitive processes would suggest that this method clearly modulates behavior. However, there are multiple reasons why the number of published papers in a field is not always indicative of evidential value.2 First, null results are typically not submitted for publication (the “file-drawer problem”, Rosenthal, 1979), as editors are more likely to accept positive versus null results (Franco, Malhotra, & Simonovits, 2014). Although there are several studies showing a significant effect of tDCS on cognitive processes, there could be a larger number of unpublished studies that found no effect.

Second, decisions made during data analysis can falsely inflate significance. For example, researchers often have a number of decisions to make when collecting and analyzing data, including deciding on how many participants to test, whether (and how) to remove outliers, data transformations (e.g., using raw vs percentage scores, whether to normalize, etc.), which dependent variables should be reported or analyzed, whether to include covariates, whether to use median splits, type of statistical analysis to use, etc. Although it is best practice to decide on the analysis pipeline before data collection, these decisions can be made during data analysis and lead to potential biases (Gelman and Loken, 2013, Gelman and Loken, 2014, Kunda, 1990). Furthermore, researchers may generate hypotheses after, not before, testing (HARKing: Hypothesizing After the Results are Known – see Kerr, 1998). For example, researchers may initially hypothesize that a specific manipulation influences task performance. Not finding the predicted effect, the researcher can probe the data to examine if dividing the population into subsets (e.g., sex differences, median splits on a different variable) results in a significant effect of the manipulation. HARKing, using multiple analysis pipelines, and other practices (such as adding participants until a significant outcome is reached) all significantly increase the odds of a false positive finding (see Simmons, Nelson, & Simonsohn, 2011).

Third, underpowered studies are at risk of, not only false negatives (Type II errors) but also false positives (Type I errors) and overestimation of true effect sizes (Type M errors, see Gelman & Carlin, 2014). Small, underpowered studies can only detect large effects. If the true effect size is small or non-existent, only studies that overestimate the true effect size via randomness will cross significance thresholds (the “winner's curse”, see Button et al., 2013, Ioannidis, 2008). Therefore, a number of factors can lead to a substantial literature with limited evidential value.

Given these concerns, an important question is how to assess evidential value in the literature. Simonsohn, Nelson, and Simmons (2014b) have developed a method for testing the evidential value of a literature by examining reported p-values. Using this method, one first finds the distribution of significant (p < .05) p-values in a selection of published studies, ignoring any p-values that are not statistically significant. Next, one compares this distribution of p-values from the selected literature to distributions that would be expected given different effect sizes. For example, the distribution of p-values from a series of studies with no effect is expected to be flat, such that the same number of p-values should be observed between .12 and .13 or .74 and .75. Importantly, this is also true for significant p-values. If there is no true effect (d = 0), then there should be the same number of p-values from .01 to .02 as there are from .04 to .05. In the presence of a real effect, this p-value distribution should be right skewed, such that there are more observed p-values between .00 and .01 than between .04 and .05. On the other hand, given certain questionable research practices, researchers may stop collecting data or do exploratory analyses once they have crossed the critical p < .05 boundary. This practice, at times called p-hacking, would result in a distribution of p-values with left skew (more p-values closer to .05 than .00).

Previous papers have examined the effects of tDCS on various aspects of cognition using traditional meta-analysis techniques, with varying results. In the working memory domain, Brunoni and Vanderhasselt (2014) found that tDCS led to improvements in reaction time, but not accuracy. Hill, Fitzgerald, and Hoy (2016) found a small effect of anodal tDCS on offline (but not online) reaction time, while Mancuso, Ilieva, Hamilton, & Farah, 2016 found that left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) tDCS improved performance when paired with training, with no other meta-analyses being significant. Dedoncker, Brunoni, Baeken, and Vanderhasselt (2016) reported that anodal, but not cathodal, DLPFC stimulation altered performance on cognitive tasks, whereas Horvath, Forte and Carter (Horvath, Forte, & Carter, 2015) found no evidence that tDCS influenced performance on cognitive tasks (though see Price & Hamilton, 2015 for discussion). However, Price, McAdams, Grossman, and Hamilton (2015) selected the language studies from Horvath, Forte & Carter, and reported that tDCS can influence performance on language tasks – though this finding has been questioned as well (see Westwood, Olson, Miall, Nappo, & Romani, 2017).

One issue with traditional meta-analytic techniques is that they can only be done on available data. Given publication biases for reporting significant results throughout the scientific literature, meta-analyses that pull from the published literature will be made primarily (or solely) of significant results, and are not able to take into account unpublished null findings. The most commonly used method to correct for publication bias is Trim and Fill (Duval and Tweedie, 2000a, Duval and Tweedie, 2000b), in which one examines a funnel plot for asymmetry, “trims” the studies with the largest effect sizes until symmetry is obtained, and then replaces the trimmed studies maintaining a symmetric funnel plot. However, Trim and Fill has been found to be inadequate at detecting publication bias. Simonsohn, Nelson, and Simmons (2014a) simulated datasets with varying true effect sizes (from d = .0 to .8), and then examined the estimated effect sizes from Trim and Fill compared to the p-curve analysis. They found that Trim and Fill vastly overestimated the effect size, such that Trim and Fill meta-analyses reported an effect size of about .7 from datasets with a true effect size of zero. In contrast, p-curve analyses provided an accurate estimate of the true effect size.

Given its advantages, we used a p-curve analysis to examine the evidential value of tDCS studies of cognition. This method has been used both to demonstrate that certain sets of studies have evidential value (e.g., studies of syntactic priming, see Mahowald, James, Futrell, & Gibson, 2016) and others do not (e.g., studies on “far transfer” in working memory with active controls and “power posing”, see Melby-Lervåg et al., 2016, Simmons and Simonsohn, 2016). Therefore, we conducted two sets of meta-analyses to examine the effect of tDCS on cognitive processes. In our first set, we randomly selected 30 studies that could be included in the p-curve analysis that examined the effect of tDCS on cognition. Then, to account for potential criticisms of the first meta-analysis (study heterogeneity, possibilities of a biased sample), we conducted a second set of meta-analyses which included all studies examining the effects of anodal stimulation on working memory in a recently published meta-analysis (Mancuso et al., 2016). In summary, both meta-analyses did not find evidence that tDCS influences behavior on cognitive and working memory tasks.

Section snippets

Manuscript selection criteria

Our goal for the first meta-analyses was to select empirical articles that examined the effect of tDCS on cognitive processes. First, we did a Pubmed search in 2014 of all articles with either transcranial direct current stimulation or tDCS and the following terms: language, phonological/phonology, orthographic/orthography, syntax, semantic(s), spelling, number(s), space/spatial, body schema, vision/visual, sensation, object recognition, touch, haptic(s), somatosensation/somatosensory,

Methods

The methods were the same as in Experiment 1, with a few key differences regarding study selection. Instead of selecting from a random sample of studies, we chose to examine every experiment that was analyzed in Mancuso et al. (2016). This included 34 experiments from 31 manuscripts. The p-curve disclosure table for the selected studies is in Supplemental Table 4. The Mancuso et al. (2016) meta-analysis focused on simply examining the effects of anodal versus sham tDCS on working memory (i.e.,

General discussion

Our analysis shows that there is minimal to no evidence that tDCS influences cognitive processes more generally, and working memory more specifically, in the current literature. None of our analyses provided evidence that the studies reviewed contained evidential value that tDCS influenced cognition. The estimate of statistical power of these studies ranged from 8 to 16%, meaning that approximately 8–16% of these studies would be predicted to replicate. Given potential concerns regarding study

Acknowledgments

This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. 1632849 and the University of Delaware Research Foundation.

References (89)

  • H.L. Filmer et al.

    Improved multitasking following prefrontal tDCS

    Cortex

    (2013)
  • A. Floel et al.

    Non-invasive brain stimulation improves object-location learning in the elderly

    Neurobiology of Aging

    (2012)
  • J. Gill et al.

    It's the thought that Counts: examining the task-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on executive function

    Brain Stimulation

    (2015)
  • T.E. Gladwin et al.

    Enhancement of selective attention by tDCS: Interaction with interference in a Sternberg task

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2012)
  • A.T. Hill et al.

    Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory: A systematic review and meta-analysis of findings from healthy and neuropsychiatric populations

    Brain Stimulation

    (2016)
  • J.C. Horvath et al.

    Quantitative review finds No evidence of cognitive effects in healthy populations from single-session transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)

    Brain Stimulation

    (2015)
  • K.E. Hoy et al.

    Testing the limits: Investigating the effect of tDCS dose on working memory enhancement in healthy controls

    Neuropsychologia

    (2013)
  • T.-Y. Hsu et al.

    Transcranial direct current stimulation over right posterior parietal cortex changes prestimulus alpha oscillation in visual short-term memory task

    NeuroImage

    (2014)
  • L. Jacobson et al.

    Oppositional transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of parietal substrates of attention during encoding modulates episodic memory

    Brain Research

    (2012)
  • K.T. Jones et al.

    The strategy and motivational influences on the beneficial effect of neurostimulation: A tDCS and fNIRS study

    NeuroImage

    (2015)
  • D. Keeser et al.

    Prefrontal direct current stimulation modulates resting EEG and event-related potentials in healthy subjects: A standardized low resolution tomography (sLORETA) study

    NeuroImage

    (2011)
  • G. Lupyan et al.

    Categorization is modulated by transcranial direct current stimulation over left prefrontal cortex

    Cognition

    (2012)
  • K. Mahowald et al.

    A meta-analysis of syntactic priming in language production

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (2016)
  • O. Meiron et al.

    Unilateral prefrontal direct current stimulation effects are modulated by working memory load and gender

    Brain Stimulation

    (2013)
  • N. Metuki et al.

    Enhancing cognitive control components of insight problems solving by anodal tDCS of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

    Brain Stimulation

    (2012)
  • N. Nozari et al.

    More attention when speaking: Does it help or does it hurt?

    Neuropsychologia

    (2013)
  • C. Plewnia et al.

    Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on executive functions: Influence of COMT Val/Met polymorphism

    Cortex

    (2013)
  • A.R. Price et al.

    A Re-evaluation of the cognitive effects from single-session transcranial direct current stimulation

    Brain Stimulation

    (2015)
  • A.R. Price et al.

    A meta-analysis of transcranial direct current stimulation studies examining the reliability of effects on language measures

    Brain Stimulation

    (2015)
  • R. Rosenthal

    The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1979)
  • C. Rosso et al.

    Connectivity between right inferior frontal gyrus and supplementary motor area predicts after-effects of right frontal cathodal tDCS on picture naming speed

    Brain Stimulation

    (2014)
  • T. Sela et al.

    Prefrontal control during a semantic decision task that involves idiom comprehension: A transcranial direct current stimulation study

    Neuropsychologia

    (2012)
  • R. Sparing et al.

    Enhancing language performance with non-invasive brain stimulation–a transcranial direct current stimulation study in healthy humans

    Neuropsychologia

    (2008)
  • P.E. Turkeltaub et al.

    Left lateralizing transcranial direct current stimulation improves reading efficiency

    Brain Stimulation

    (2012)
  • M.A. Vanderhasselt et al.

    Nosce te ipsum–Socrates revisited? Controlling momentary ruminative self-referent thoughts by neuromodulation of emotional working memory

    Neuropsychologia

    (2013)
  • S.J. Westwood et al.

    Limits to tDCS effects in language: Failures to modulate word production in healthy participants with frontal or temporal tDCS

    Cortex

    (2017)
  • L. Wolkenstein et al.

    Amelioration of cognitive control in depression by transcranial direct current stimulation

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2013)
  • A.J. Woods et al.

    Space, time, and causality in the human brain

    NeuroImage

    (2014)
  • Y.-J. Wu et al.

    Modulating the interference effect on spatial working memory by applying transcranial direct current stimulation over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

    Brain and Cognition

    (2014)
  • M. Asthana et al.

    Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on consolidation of fear memory

    Front Psychiatry

    (2013)
  • M.J. Banissy et al.

    Transcranial direct current stimulation in sports training: Potential approaches

    Front Hum Neurosci

    (2013)
  • L.J. Bindman et al.

    Long-lasting changes in the level of the electrical activity of the cerebral cortex produced by polarizing currents

    Nature

    (1962)
  • P.S. Boggio et al.

    A randomized, double-blind clinical trial on the efficacy of cortical direct current stimulation for the treatment of major depression

    Int J Neuropsychopharmacol

    (2008)
  • S. Bona et al.

    Accuracy and confidence of visual short-term memory do not go hand-in-hand: Behavioral and neural dissociations

    Plos One

    (2014)
  • Cited by (107)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text