Elsevier

Cortex

Volume 71, October 2015, Pages 134-147
Cortex

Research report
Online feedback enhances early consolidation of motor sequence learning and reverses recall deficit from transcranial stimulation of motor cortex

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.012Get rights and content

Abstract

Feedback and monetary reward can enhance motor skill learning, suggesting reward system involvement. Continuous theta burst (cTBS) transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of the primary motor area (M1) disrupts processing, reduces excitability and impairs motor learning. To see whether feedback and reward can overcome the learning impairment associated with M1 cTBS, we delivered real or sham stimulation to two groups of participants before they performed a motor sequence learning task with and without feedback. Participants were trained on two intermixed sequences, one occurring 85% of the time (the “probable” sequence) and the other 15% of the time (the “improbable” sequence). We measured sequence learning as the difference in reaction time (RT) and error rate between probable and improbable trials (RT and error difference scores). Participants were also tested for sequence recall with the same indices of learning 60 min after cTBS. Real stimulation impaired initial sequence learning and sequence knowledge recall as measured by error difference scores and impaired sequence knowledge recall as measured by RT difference score. Relative to non-feedback learning, the introduction of feedback during sequence learning improved subsequent sequence knowledge recall indexed by RT difference score, in both real and sham stimulation groups and feedback reversed the RT difference score based sequence knowledge recall impairment from real cTBS that we observed in the non-feedback learning condition. Only the real cTBS group in the non-feedback condition showed no evidence of explicit sequence knowledge when tested at the end of the study. Feedback improves recall of implicit and explicit motor sequence knowledge and can protect sequence knowledge against the effect of M1 inhibition. Adding feedback and monetary reward/punishment to motor skill learning may help overcome retention impairments or accelerate training in clinical and other settings.

Introduction

Procedural learning, the process by which skills are acquired by practice, is a fundamental and critical function of the brain. It is a key component of higher skills, such as math, where the rules can be understood explicitly, but facility comes only with repeated problem solving (Fayol & Thevenot, 2012). The benefit of repeated practice is evident in rehabilitation strategies for brain damaged patients where outcome is improved by extensive practice of specific movements (Nadeau, 2002). It also allows acquisition of intuitive skills that help humans and animals function in uncertain environments.

One popular procedural learning paradigm is the serial reaction time task (SRTT, Nissen & Bullemer, 1987), where participants respond rapidly to a stream of cues by pressing buttons with the fingers of one hand. If a long, repeating sequence is embedded in the stream, speed improves before the participant is aware that a sequence is present. That is, the knowledge that a particular cue is likely to follow another is acquired as an unconscious motor skill through practice. This predictive knowledge, built from associations between events, could be acquired through reinforcement learning and promoted by the dopamine (DA) reward system (Schultz, 2002). An implication of this theory is that boosting the activity of the reward system could make procedural learning more efficient and aid skill acquisition. Acquisition of motor sequence learning on the SRTT is not always exclusively an implicit process, and depending on the learning environment, implicit and explicit/conscious sequence learning can occur in parallel (Willingham and Goedert-Eschmann, 1999, Willingham et al., 2002). While this fact makes the task unsuitable for studying implicit knowledge in isolation, it is consistent with much learning in the real world.

The reward system is considered one of the networks involved in procedural motor sequence learning. For example, learning on the SRTT is impaired in patients with Parkinson (PD) and Huntington (HD) diseases (Doyon et al., 1997, Jackson et al., 1995, Knopman and Nissen, 1991, Muslimovic et al., 2007, Wilkinson and Jahanshahi, 2007, Wilkinson et al., 2009), focal lesions of the basal ganglia (Obeso et al., 2009), and traumatic brain injury (TBI) (De Beaumont et al., 2012, Mutter et al., 1994, Vakil, 2005) as well as in an animal models of DA depletion (Matsumoto, Hanakawa, Maki, Graybiel, & Kimura, 1999).

There is also evidence from studies in healthy humans that incentive and feedback can improve motor skill learning. For instance, adding monetary reward to a force-tracking task which incorporated a repeating pattern improved retention as demonstrated by offline gains (Abe et al., 2011). Adding similar feedback and incentive to the SRTT improved learning (Wachter, Lungu, Liu, Willingham, & Ashe, 2009). These findings imply that monetary reward might augment rehabilitation after injury or accelerate learning in healthy people.

Human learning deficits can also be produced in the laboratory. When delivered to the primary motor cortex (M1), inhibitory transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), in particular continuous theta burst stimulation (cTBS), reduces local cortical excitability (Huang, Edwards, Rounis, Bhatia, & Rothwell, 2005) and temporarily impairs motor sequence learning (Rosenthal et al., 2009, Wilkinson et al., 2010). The magnitude of these deficits in healthy volunteers is similar to those in patients (De Beaumont et al., 2012, Doyon et al., 1997, Jackson et al., 1995, Knopman and Nissen, 1991, Muslimovic et al., 2007, Mutter et al., 1994, Obeso et al., 2009, Vakil, 2005, Wilkinson and Jahanshahi, 2007, Wilkinson et al., 2009). However, the addition of feedback, including monetary reward, does not improve non-motor procedural learning in PD and HD (Holl et al., 2012, Shohamy et al., 2004, Wilkinson et al., 2008).

Adding feedback and incentive to procedural tasks in clinical and training settings to boost learning has great appeal. Unlike interventional strategies currently under investigation, including the several forms of noninvasive brain stimulation (Reis et al., 2008, Sandrini and Cohen, 2013), there is no need for devices requiring large clinical trials and regulatory approval. There are no ethical problems posed by its use in healthy populations. However, its viability and comparative value depend on the magnitude of its effects and the ability to produce them in impaired or otherwise refractory subjects. Therefore, we decided to investigate whether adding feedback and monetary incentives to the SRTT can overcome the healthy volunteers' temporary impairment produced by inhibitory TMS and whether the size of its statistical effects is of clinical interest. We delivered sham and real cTBS to M1 just before administering a probabilistic version of the SRT, which is less likely than the conventional task to produce explicit sequence knowledge. We hypothesized that feedback would enhance sequence learning and/or recall as well as protect learning and knowledge retention from the effect of cTBS. We also tracked the development of awareness during learning closely to show whether the motor sequence learning was always unconscious or whether the procedural and declarative systems interacted during the task with and without feedback, and after inhibition of M1 via cTBS.

Section snippets

Participants

We recruited 40 right-handed, healthy volunteers, all of whom met safety criteria for TMS (Keel, Smith, & Wassermann, 2001). All were free of neurological and psychiatric illness and none was on continuous medication other than oral contraceptives. We estimated intelligence with the National Adult Reading Test (NART). The study was approved by the Combined Neuroscience Institutional Review Board at the National Institutes of Health. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Results

The sham and real cTBS groups did not differ in age, estimated IQ, years of education, time between sessions, time to complete the pSRTT (30 min per group/condition), or sex. Mean winnings across learning blocks did not differ between groups: sham, M = $19.15, SD = $9.70, real, M = $22.00, SD = $8.90.

Discussion

We set out to show that feedback could boost procedural learning and restore learning after disruptive TMS delivered to M1. We saw significant effects in the predicted directions for both feedback and real cTBS on RT-based sequence knowledge 30 min after the end of training and 1 h after cTBS. This time course for the effect of feedback, with the greatest effect on delayed sequence recall, is consistent with other observations on feedback and procedural learning. Abe et al. (2011) showed

Acknowledgments

We thank Mr. Devin Bageac for helping with data collection and entry. We are also extremely grateful to Mr. Phil Koshy for proof-reading and editing this manuscript.

The authors declare no conflict of interest. Funding came from the Clinical Neuroscience Program of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (1ZIANS002977-14) and the Center for Neuroscience and Regenerative Medicine at the Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences, via the Henry Jackson Foundation (

References (65)

  • D. Knopman et al.

    Procedural learning is impaired in Huntingtons-disease – evidence from the serial reaction-time-task

    Neuropsychologia

    (1991)
  • S.E. Nadeau

    A paradigm shift in neurorehabilitation

    Lancet Neurology

    (2002)
  • M.J. Nissen et al.

    Attentional requirements of learning – evidence from performance-measures

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1987)
  • J.A. Obeso et al.

    What can man do without basal ganglia motor output? the effect of combined unilateral subthalamotomy and pallidotomy in a patient with Parkinson's disease

    Experimental Neurology

    (2009)
  • J. Reis et al.

    Consensus: can transcranial direct current stimulation and transcranial magnetic stimulation enhance motor learning and memory formation?

    Brain Stimulation

    (2008)
  • E.M. Robertson

    New Insights in human memory interference and consolidation

    Current Biology

    (2012)
  • E.M. Robertson et al.

    Awareness modifies the skill-learning benefits of sleep

    Current Biology

    (2004)
  • M. Sandrini et al.

    Noninvasive brain stimulation in neurorehabilitation

    Handbook of Clinical Neurology

    (2013)
  • H.E. Schendan et al.

    An fMRI study of the role of the medial temporal lobe in implicit and explicit sequence learning

    Neuron

    (2003)
  • W. Schultz

    Getting formal with dopamine and reward

    Neuron

    (2002)
  • E. Vakil et al.

    Impaired skill learning in patients with severe closed-head injury as demonstrated by the serial reaction time (SRT) task

    Brain and Cognition

    (2002)
  • E.M. Wassermann

    Variation in the response to transcranial magnetic brain stimulation in the general population

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2002)
  • L. Wilkinson et al.

    The striatum and probabilistic implicit sequence learning

    Brain Research

    (2007)
  • L. Wilkinson et al.

    The role of the basal ganglia and its cortical connections in sequence learning: evidence from implicit and explicit sequence learning in Parkinson's disease

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • L. Wilkinson et al.

    The effect of feedback on non-motor probabilistic classification learning in Parkinson's disease

    Neuropsychologia

    (2008)
  • P. Bedard et al.

    Basal ganglia-dependent processes in recalling learned visual-motor adaptations

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2011)
  • R.G. Brown et al.

    Pallidotomy and incidental sequence learning in Parkinson's disease

    NeuroReport

    (2003)
  • R.M. Brown et al.

    Inducing motor skill improvements with a declarative task

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • R.M. Brown et al.

    Off-line processing: reciprocal interactions between declarative and procedural memories

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • L. De Beaumont et al.

    Altered bidirectional plasticity and reduced implicit motor learning in concussed athletes

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2012)
  • A. Destrebecqz et al.

    The neural correlates of implicit and explicit sequence learning: Interacting networks revealed by the process dissociation procedure

    Learning & Memory

    (2005)
  • J. Doyon et al.

    Functional anatomy of visuomotor skill learning in human subjects examined with positron emission tomography

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (1996)
  • Cited by (12)

    • Intranasal oxytocin attenuates the effects of monetary feedback on procedural learning

      2022, Psychoneuroendocrinology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Particularly, monetary rewards can improve procedural learning (Wächter et al., 2009) and retention of a motor memory over time (Abe et al., 2011; Galea et al., 2015). Monetary rewards can even help overcome impairments in procedural learning externally induced by inhibitory non-invasive brain stimulation (Wilkinson et al., 2015). Translating these findings to clinical practice, monetary reward (and punishment) enhanced motor adaptation also in stroke patients (Quattrocchi et al., 2017).

    • Embodied negation and levels of concreteness: A TMS study on German and Italian language processing

      2021, Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The peak-to-peak amplitude (mV) of each MEP was normalized by means of a log10 logarithmic transformation (Poole et al., 2018). As a standard procedure (Candidi et al., 2010; Wilkinson et al., 2015), we discarded MEPs followed by incorrect responses to control questions (.29%), below 50 mV (2.51%), and outside the range of +/- 3 SDs from the mean MEPs of each experimental condition (2.72%; for the number of trials analyzed for each experimental condition, see Table S3 available at https://osf.io/5bw7m/). Transformed amplitudes were entered in a repeated-measures ANOVA with polarity (affirmative, negative), concreteness (AA, AC, CC) and phrase (verb, noun, adverb) as within-participants factors.

    • Learning with the wave of the hand: Kinematic and TMS evidence of primary motor cortex role in category-specific encoding of word meaning

      2019, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      To disrupt M1 function, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) which can alter neural processing in the healthy human brain, thereby establishing the causal role of the stimulated region in a given mental task (Jahanshahi and Rothwell, 2000; Klomjai et al., 2015; Pascual-Leone et al., 2000; Song et al., 2011). Previous research has already shown that cTBS decreases cortical excitability, and results in an effect resembling long-term synaptic depression (LTD) (Chen et al., 1997; Huang et al., 2005; Vukovic et al., 2016; Wilkinson et al., 2015). We assessed word learning performance using several online and offline behavioural measures, and an interactive virtual environment (VE) task which allowed both experimental control and approximation of naturalistic verb learning scenarios.

    • Motor cortex inhibition by TMS reduces cognitive non-motor procedural learning when immediate incentives are present

      2017, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      It remains an open question whether the substrata of the effects of inhibitory rTMS on learning and the motor evoked potential (MEP) are the same, or whether the MEP can serve as a biomarker of that effect. In our previous work (Wilkinson et al., 2015), we found no discernible relationship between the degree of MEP amplitude reduction and the negative effect on motor learning overall, despite the presence of a significant positive relationship between MEP reduction and learning in the presence of money reward. In this study, we again included an exploratory analysis of the effect of cTBS on the MEP from TMS.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Present address: Washington University School of Medicine, Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Saint Louis, MO 63110, USA.

    View full text