Elsevier

Cortex

Volume 49, Issue 3, March 2013, Pages 827-836
Cortex

Research report
Stimulus- and state-dependence of systematic bias in spatial attention: Additive effects of stimulus-size and time-on-task

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.12.007Get rights and content

Abstract

Systematic biases in spatial attention are a common finding. In the general population, a systematic leftward bias is typically observed (pseudoneglect), possibly as a consequence of right hemisphere dominance for visuospatial attention. However, this leftward bias can cross-over to a systematic rightward bias with changes in stimulus and state factors (such as line length and arousal). The processes governing these changes are still unknown. Here we tested models of spatial attention as to their ability to account for these effects. To this end, we experimentally manipulated both stimulus and state factors, while healthy participants performed a computerized version of a landmark task. State was manipulated by time-on-task (>1 h) leading to increased fatigue and a reliable left- to rightward shift in spatial bias. Stimulus was manipulated by presenting either long or short lines which was associated with a shift of subjective midpoint from a reliable leftward bias for long to a more rightward bias for short lines. Importantly, we found time-on-task and line length effects to be additive suggesting a common denominator for line bisection across all conditions, which is in disagreement with models that assume that bisection decisions in long and short lines are governed by distinct processes (Magnitude estimation vs Global/local distinction). Our findings emphasize the dynamic rather than static nature of spatial biases in midline judgement. They are best captured by theories of spatial attention positing that spatial bias is flexibly modulated, and subject to inter-hemispheric balance which can change over time or conditions to accommodate task demands or reflect fatigue.

Introduction

‘Pseudoneglect’ represents the tendency for neurologically normal participants to misbisect horizontal lines when asked to judge their midpoint during line bisection tasks (Bowers and Heilman, 1980). The direction of bias has usually been found to be to the left of veridical centre and of a much smaller magnitude than the rightward bias typically exhibited by patients with visuospatial neglect after right hemisphere (RH) stroke (Schenkenberg et al., 1980, Harvey et al., 1995, Vallar, 1998). However, the direction and magnitude of bias varies systematically within participants as a function of a number of stimulus and context factors (Jewell and McCourt, 2000, McCourt, 2001, Failla et al., 2003, Pérez et al., 2009, Heber et al., 2010, Schmitz and Peigneux, 2011).

The magnitude of the leftward bias in line bisection tends to decrease as a function of decreasing line length and has been reported to ‘cross-over’ to become a bias in the opposite, rightward direction in very short lines (<2 cm) (McCourt and Jewell, 1999, Rueckert et al., 2002). Previous studies investigating the robustness of this line length effect in normal participants display mixed findings, though differing bisection tasks and experimental designs have been employed (Manning et al., 1990, Luh, 1995, Laeng et al., 1996, McCourt and Jewell, 1999, Jewell and McCourt, 2000, Mennemeier et al., 2001, Mennemeier et al., 2002, Rueckert et al., 2002, Varnava et al., 2002, Heber et al., 2010). The most commonly employed tasks include the landmark task, a perceptual line bisection judgement task designed to dissociate the contribution of perceptual and motor factors (Milner et al., 1992, Harvey et al., 2000, Olk and Harvey, 2002), and manual line bisection, of which the former shows more reliable cross-over with short lines (Rueckert et al., 2002) suggesting perceptual bisection tasks to be optimal for detection of the line length effect in healthy participants.

Besides stimulus factors, arousal level also seems to influence spatial bias, with leftward bias associated with states of relatively high alertness and rightward bias associated with states of low alertness or fatigue [Bellgrove et al., 2004, Manly et al., 2005, Fimm et al., 2006, Matthias et al., 2009; though see Schmitz et al. (2011) for conflicting results with the landmark task]. In addition, left- to rightward shifts in line bisection judgement have been observed over the course of prolonged performance of the landmark task. This has been labelled the ‘Time-on-task’ effect (Manly et al., 2005, Dufour et al., 2007). These intra-individual variations indicate that visuospatial bias is a dynamic phenomenon fluctuating over time and depending on context. Accounting for fluctuations in attentional asymmetry may represent an important step in fully understanding how the visuospatial attention system is organized. The aim of the present study was to probe models of spatial attention as to their ability to explain the cross-over effect using time-on-task as an experimental manipulation, detailed below.

All influential models of spatial attention assume contribution of both the RH and left hemisphere (LH) to orienting towards the contralateral visual fields, although to different extent (e.g., Kinsbourne, 1970, Heilman and Van Den Abell, 1980, Mesulam, 1981). In line with these models, predominant RH-activation during line bisection tasks (Fink et al., 2000, Foxe et al., 2003, Waberski et al., 2008) may induce an attentional bias towards the left side of the line, thereby increasing its perceived length relative to the right side and shifting the perceived midpoint to the left of veridical centre (Bultitude and Aimola-Davies, 2006). How can the left- to right cross-over from long to short lines then be explained? One model suggests that cross-over results from the leftward attentional asymmetry coupled with a previously reported, general tendency to underestimate the absolute length of long lines and to overestimate the absolute length of short lines (Werth and Poppel, 1988, Tegner and Levander, 1991). This orientation/estimation hypothesis (Mennemeier et al., 2005) posits that once attention is preferentially oriented to one end of a line (typically the left), underestimating the absolute length of long lines leads to the bisection mark being placed short of veridical centre (i.e., to the left), whereas overestimating the absolute length of short lines leads to the bisection mark being placed beyond veridical centre (i.e., to the right). An alternative model suggests that task specific hemispheric dominance for line bisection may switch from the RH in long lines to the LH in short lines. A potential mechanism for this may be RH-dominance for lower spatial frequencies and/or global perception, in contrast to LH-dominance for higher spatial frequencies and/or local perception (Sergent, 1982, Ivry and Robertson, 1998, Monaghan and Shillcock, 2004, Flevaris et al., 2011). Behavioural dissociations have been found in bisection tasks when participants were directed to focus on either the local or global elements of the stimulus respectively, and these differences have been suggested as a possible explanation for the line length effect (Hughes et al., 2005, Gallace et al., 2008). We call this the “Local/Global” hemispheric specialization hypothesis. Importantly, these models lead to different predictions as to the outcome of time-on-task on bisection judgement performance in long versus short lines.

The time-on-task modulation of attentional bias by arousal level has been interpreted to represent an interaction between orienting and arousal networks in the RH, which biases attention towards the left visual field in states of high alertness but results in a reduction or even reversal of this bias as RH-activation decreases with reduced alertness/increasing fatigue (Corbetta et al., 2005, Manly et al., 2005, Fimm et al., 2006, Dufour et al., 2007). To date, the time-on-task effect in the landmark task has only been investigated using relatively long lines (Manly et al., 2005, Dufour et al., 2007). We investigated for the first time the influence of time-on-task on midpoint judgements in short (1 cm) as well as long lines (29 cm). The models above would predict the following outcomes. Under both the orientation/estimation hypothesis and the “local/global” hemispheric specialization hypothesis, long and short lines should differ in directional bisection errors, with more leftward bias in long than short lines (line length effect) at the beginning of the experiment (high alertness). Under the orientation/estimation hypothesis, a rightward shift in spatial bias over the course of the experimental session should lead to a reversal of the direction of cross-over. Once attention is shifted rightwards (time-on-task effect), underestimating the length of long lines should lead to the bisection mark being placed short (i.e., to the right) of veridical centre. In contrast, overestimation of short lines should lead to the bisection mark being placed beyond (i.e., to the left of) veridical centre. Under the “local/global” hemispheric specialization hypothesis, one would expect decreasing alertness and thus RH-depletion to primarily affect the bisection of long but not short lines (due to RH-dominance for long line but LH-dominance for short line processing), therefore leading to a rightward shift primarily in long but not short lines. We tested these predictions by assessing line bisection biases for short and long lines in the landmark task at the beginning and end of the experimental session, before and after an extended practice period (about 1 h time-on-task) in which participants performed the task either exclusively on long lines or on short lines (group design).

Section snippets

Participants

Thirty-two right-handed participants (18 male, 14 female, mean age = 22.32 years, standard deviation (SD) = 2.6) took part in the experiment. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant. All participants were volunteers naive to the experimental hypothesis being tested. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and reported no history of neurological disorder. The experiment was carried out within the School of Psychology at the University of Glasgow and was

Subjective alertness

Stanford Sleepiness Scale ratings confirmed a reduction in subjective alertness over the course of the experimental session, with the overall mean score on the Stanford Sleepiness Scale increasing from 2.5 to 4.2. A 2 (pre vs post experiment) × 2 (group) factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) confirmed a main effect of time-on-task on sleepiness rating [F(1, 30) = 32.869, p = .000] independently of group [no interaction between group and time-on-task: F(1, 30) = .619, p = .438].

Psychometric functions and subjective midpoint

Fig. 3A presents

Discussion

The results of the current study highlight the dynamic nature of visuospatial attentional bias in healthy young participants. The direction and magnitude of bias displayed on a landmark task can be modulated by both line length and time-on-task within participants, suggesting that systematic attentional bias does not represent a static trait within individuals, but fluctuates over time and depends on context and stimulus characteristics. Our results replicate those of previous studies that have

Conclusion

The current findings emphasize the dynamic nature of subjective midpoint judgement in healthy participants suggesting that both line length and time-on-task can influence performance in the landmark task. The results add further weight to an interpretation of the time-on-task effect in pseudoneglect as representing an endogenous shift in attentional bias, rather than learning. Our results best fit a model which takes into account the dynamic nature of spatial attention and emphasizes a common

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank those who participated in the study. This work was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council [grant number ES/I02395X/1].

References (71)

  • J.J. Foxe et al.

    Right hemisphere control of visuospatial attention: Line-bisection judgments evaluated with high-density electrical mapping and source analysis

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • M. Harvey et al.

    Differential effects of line length on bisection judgements and hemispatial neglect

    Cortex

    (1995)
  • M. Harvey et al.

    Effects of visible and invisible cueing procedures on perceptual judgments in young and elderly subjects

    Neuropsychologia

    (2000)
  • I. Heber et al.

    Horizontal and vertical pseudoneglect in peri and extrapersonal space

    Brain and Cognition

    (2010)
  • L.E. Hughes et al.

    The effects of local and global processing demands on perception and action

    Brain and Cognition

    (2005)
  • G. Jewell et al.

    Pseudoneglect: A review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks

    Neuropsychologia

    (2000)
  • M. Kinsbourne

    The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention

    Acta Psychologica

    (1970)
  • B. Laeng et al.

    Tactile rod bisection: Hemispheric activation and sex differences

    Neuropsychologia

    (1996)
  • T. Manly et al.

    Rightward shift in spatial awareness with declining alertness

    Neuropsychologia

    (2005)
  • L. Manning et al.

    Individual variation in line bisection: A study of normal subjects with application to the interpretation of visual neglect

    Neuropsychologia

    (1990)
  • E. Matthias et al.

    Attentional and sensory effects of lowered levels of intrinsic alertness

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • M.E. McCourt et al.

    Visuospatial attention in line bisection: Stimulus modulation of pseudoneglect

    Neuropsychologia

    (1999)
  • M.E. McCourt

    Performance consistency of normal observers in forced-choice tachistoscopic visual line bisection

    Neuropsychologia

    (2001)
  • M. Mennemeier et al.

    Crossover by line length and spatial location

    Brain and Cognition

    (2001)
  • A.D. Milner et al.

    To halve and to halve not: An analysis of line bisection judgements in normal subjects

    Neuropsychologia

    (1992)
  • A.D. Milner et al.

    Line bisection errors in visual neglect: Misguided action or size distortion?

    Neuropsychologia

    (1993)
  • A.D. Milner et al.

    Distortion of size perception in visuospatial neglect

    Current Biology

    (1995)
  • B. Olk et al.

    Effects of visible and invisible cueing on line bisection and landmark performance in hemispatial neglect

    Neuropsychologia

    (2002)
  • A. Pérez et al.

    Hemispheric modulations of alpha-band power reflect the rightward shift in attention induced by enhanced attentional load

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • P.A. Reuter-Lorenz et al.

    Hemispheric control of spatial attention

    Brain and Cognition

    (1990)
  • L. Rueckert et al.

    Pseudoneglect and the cross-over effect

    Neuropsychologia

    (2002)
  • R. Schmitz et al.

    Selective modulations of attentional asymmetries after sleep deprivation

    Neuropsychologia

    (2011)
  • R. Schmitz et al.

    Age-related changes in visual pseudoneglect

    Brain and Cognition

    (2011)
  • W. Sturm et al.

    Functional reorganisation in patients with right hemisphere stroke after training of alertness: A longitudinal PET and fMRI study in eight cases

    Neuropsychologia

    (2004)
  • M. Urbanski et al.

    Line bisection in left neglect: The importance of starting right

    Cortex

    (2008)
  • Cited by (48)

    • Interactions between spatial attention and alertness in healthy adults: A meta-analysis

      2019, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      The observed heterogeneity in the current review suggests indeed that this might be the case. Such possible factors are, for example, line length (Benwell, Harvey, Gardner, & Thut, 2013), and initial bias (Benwell, Thut, Learmonth, & Harvey, 2013). In the study by Benwell, Harvey, et al. (2013) Benwell, Thut, et al. (2013) the length of the Landmark task stimulus influenced whether the spatial bias following a decrease in alertness was statistically significant, as only long (not short) stimuli showed the rightward shift with lowered alertness.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text