Elsevier

Consciousness and Cognition

Volume 27, July 2014, Pages 89-99
Consciousness and Cognition

The use of realistic and mechanical hands in the rubber hand illusion, and the relationship to hemispheric differences

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.04.010Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Similar RHI strength for realistic and for non-biological hand shaped objects.

  • Similar RHI strength, measured by drift and questionnaire, for left and right hands.

  • Stronger relationship between drift and agreement on the questionnaire (left hand).

Abstract

Embodiment, as measured through the rubber-hand illusion (RHI), depends on the similarity between object to be embodied and part of the body it replaces. We compared a fake hand similar to a real hand, and one matched in size but made of wires (mechanical). Left and right versions were tested to investigate whether the effect of appearance was stronger in the left hand. We found that the mechanical hand induced embodiment, though to a reduced degree relative to the realistic hand (N = 120). Left and right versions of the mechanical hand did not differ in strength of the illusion. However, with the left realistic hand there was a stronger relationship between drift (an objective measure of the illusion) and agreement on the questionnaire (subjective experience). With the mechanical hand, objective and subjective measures were unrelated. We discuss the results in relation to factors that influence the RHI and hemispheric differences.

Introduction

The rubber hand illusion (RHI) plays a key role in the study of how the inner representation of one’s body changes over time based on experience. In the standard procedure, one’s hand is hidden, and a fake hand is visible. When both hands are stimulated at the same time, for instance by a paintbrush, the visual experience of seeing the fake hand touched is combined with the corresponding tactual sensation. After less than a minute, most participants experience a sense of ownership of the fake hand, including a sense that the fake hand feels the touch (Botvinick & Cohen, 1998, for a review see Serino & Haggard, 2010). The illusion is strongest following synchronous stroking, weaker with asynchronous stroking, and weaker with only visual exposure to the fake hand and no stimulation (e.g. Longo et al., 2008, Rohde et al., 2011).

The conditions necessary for the illusion have been debated in the literature. Some authors have suggested that the correlation between vision and touch is sufficient for inducing the experience of ownership of objects totally different from a hand, for instance a table (Armel & Ramachandran, 2003). Other authors, however, have concluded that the fake hand has to have a plausible appearance, and be placed in a plausible relationship to the body. We review this literature below, and next we consider reasons to predict a difference in the strength of the illusion when the left hand or the right hand is stimulated (Ocklenburg, Rüther, Peterburs, Pinnow, & Güntürkün, 2011), and how this might relate to the appearance of the fake hand. Our study addresses both the role of appearance, by using two types of hands, and the role of laterality, by testing both left and right hands of each participant.

Section snippets

The appearance of the fake hand

In our study, we revisited the role of the appearance of the hand. In particular, we compared hands that looked like human hands, and hands that had the size and shape of a hand, including the presence of fingers, but were clearly non-biological. There is empirical evidence that the plausibility of the hand determines the strength of the illusion. Tsakiris and Haggard (2005) compared fake hands with wooden sticks, and the latter failed to elicit the illusion. A rectangular wooden block has been

Hemispheric differences and body representation

The right hemisphere has been connected to a stronger awareness of the physical and mental self, with evidence coming from neurological patients and neurophysiological techniques (Feinberg and Keenan, 2005, Karnath and Baier, 2010, Keenan et al., 2001). Recently, differences in representations related to self have been addressed through the RHI. There is evidence that the right hemisphere accepts a fake hand more easily than the left hemisphere. Skin conductance response to the fake hand being

Different measures of the strength of the illusion

Two measures of the illusion were taken. The objective measure required participants to close their eyes, and point toward the middle finger of the illuded hand with the index finger of the non-illuded hand. Proprioceptive drift toward the fake hand, and away from the real hand, measures the degree of change in the representation of the position of the real hand. The subjective measure included three questions selected and adapted from Botvinick and Cohen (1998). Drift and subjective experience

Experiment

As mentioned in the introduction we conducted an experiment that compared two types of hands. One type was realistic and was created from casting a real pair of male hands. The other hand matched the realistic hand in size but was made of metallic wires (see Fig. 1). We manipulated appearance and tested both left and right hands for each participant in a large sample (N = 120) of young adults. A subset of the same data, excluding the mechanical hand and combined with measures of

Cross-manual pointing

The distance between the position of the middle finger of the real hand and the pointed location is plotted in Fig. 2. The larger the error the greater the drift toward the fake hand.

Drift was normally distributed; thus parametric analyses were run. Two repeated factors, Hand (left, right) and Brushing (visual, asynchronous, synchronous), were placed in an ANOVA with Appearance as a between-subject factors. The dependent variable was the proprioceptive error in pointing to the real hand.

Discussion

We explored the role of the appearance of the fake hand on the strength of the RHI, and whether effects of appearance were lateralized. We contrasted hands that shared many of the characteristics of real hands; however, one was mechanical, and therefore less plausible in contrast to a more realistic one. We tested both a left and right version of each hand. We expected that the mechanical hand would lead to a reduced strength in the illusion, and that this reduction would be greater when the

Acknowledgment

We would like to thank Carole Bode for designing and building the fake and mechanical hands used in the study.

References (47)

  • D.M. Lloyd

    Spatial limits on referred touch to an alien limb may reflect boundaries of visuo-tactile peripersonal space surrounding the hand

    Brain and Cognition

    (2007)
  • M.R. Longo et al.

    Visual enhancement of touch and the bodily self

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2008)
  • T. McElroy et al.

    On the other hand am I rational? Hemispheric activation and the framing effect

    Brain and Cognition

    (2004)
  • S. Ocklenburg et al.

    The rubber hand illusion modulates pseudoneglect

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2012)
  • A. Reinersmann et al.

    The rubber hand illusion in complex regional pain syndrome: Preserved ability to integrate a rubber hand indicates intact multisensory integration

    Pain

    (2013)
  • A. Serino et al.

    Touch and the body

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2010)
  • M. Tsakiris et al.

    The role of the right temporoparietal junction in maintaining a coherent sense of one’s body

    Neuropsychologia

    (2008)
  • M. Tsakiris et al.

    Having a body versus moving your body: Neural signatures of agency and body-ownership

    Neuropsychologia

    (2010)
  • L.Q. Uddin

    Brain connectivity and the self: The case of cerebral disconnection

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2011)
  • K.C. Armel et al.

    Projecting sensations to external objects: Evidence from skin conductance response

    Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B: Biological Sciences

    (2003)
  • E.L. Austen et al.

    Mislocalizations of touch to a fake hand

    Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2004)
  • M. Botvinick et al.

    Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see

    Nature

    (1998)
  • L. Cardinali et al.

    Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema

    Current Biology

    (2009)
  • Cited by (20)

    • The rubber hand universe: On the impact of methodological differences in the rubber hand illusion

      2019, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      In addition, bodily self-awareness has been predominantly associated with the right cortical hemisphere (for a recent review see Blanke et al., 2015), and therefore, in right-handers, the non-dominant left hand might be more directly linked to processes affecting body ownership. Some studies did not find substantial differences between hands (Mussap and Salton, 2006; Niebauer et al., 2002; Smit et al., 2017; Zeller and Hullin, 2018) or only side effects such as a higher relationship between proprioceptive drift and subjective ratings for the left compared to the right hand (Bertamini and O’Sullivan, 2014). However, other studies reported higher subjective ratings (Ocklenburg et al., 2011; Reinersmann et al., 2013), larger proprioceptive drifts (Dempsey-Jones and Kritikos, 2019) and increased skin conductance responses after a threat (Ocklenburg et al., 2011) for the left compared to the right hand.

    • The role of visual similarity and memory in body model distortions

      2016, Acta Psychologica
      Citation Excerpt :

      If this is the case, an object with greater visual similarity to a real hand (e.g. a rubber hand) might depict distortions that are closer to the hand than the rake. This idea would be in line with research on embodiment showing that objects can be experienced as part of one's body (i.e. as embodied) when they share important structural and visual information about the body part (Bertamini & O'Sullivan, 2014; Holmes, Snijders, & Spence, 2006; Tsakiris, Carpenter, James, & Fotopoulou, 2010; Tsakiris & Haggard, 2005). Studies on the rubber hand illusion suggest that the degree to which fake body parts (rubber hand and non-biological mechanical hand) can be embodied depends on the similarity between the actual body part and the tested stimulus.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text