Elsevier

Brain Research Bulletin

Volume 71, Issues 1–3, 11 December 2006, Pages 76-82
Brain Research Bulletin

Neural response to the visual familiarity of faces

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.08.003Get rights and content

Abstract

Recognizing personally familiar faces is the result of a spatially distributed process that involves visual perceptual areas and areas that play a role in other cognitive and social functions, such as the anterior paracingulate cortex, the precuneus and the amygdala [M.I. Gobbini, E. Leibenluft, N. Santiago, J.V. Haxby, Social and emotional attachment in the neural representation of faces, Neuroimage 22 (2004) 1628–1635; M.I. Gobbini, J.V. Haxby, Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces, Neuropsychologia, in press; E. Leibenluft, M.I. Gobbini, T. Harrison, J.V. Haxby, Mothers’ neural activation in response to pictures of their, and other, children, Biol. Psychiatry 56 (2004) 225–232]. In order to isolate the role of visual familiarity in face recognition, we used fMRI to measure the response to faces characterized by experimentally induced visual familiarity that carried no biographical information or emotional content. The fMRI results showed a stronger response in the precuneus to the visually familiar faces consistent with studies that implicate this region in the retrieval of information from long-term memory and imagery. Moreover, this finding supports the hypothesis of a key role for the precuneus in the acquisition of familiarity with faces [H. Kosaka, M. Omori, T. Iidaka, T. Murata, T. Shimoyama, T. Okada, N. Sadato, Y. Yonekura, Y. Wada, Neural substrates participating in acquisition of facial familiarity: an fMRI study, Neuroimage 20 (2003) 1734–1742]. By contrast, the visually familiar faces evoked a weaker response in the fusiform gyrus, which may reflect the development of a sparser encoding or a reduced attentional load when processing stimuli that are familiar. The visually familiar faces also evoked a weaker response in the amygdala, supporting the proposed role of this structure in mediating the guarded attitude when meeting someone new.

Introduction

In two previous fMRI studies on familiar face recognition [12], [23] we have suggested that recognizing someone we know is the result of a spatially distributed process that involves not only visual cortical areas but also areas that are involved in other social and cognitive functions such as the theory of mind areas (anterior paracingulate and posterior superior temporal sulcus) [10], [11], the precuneus, the amygdala and the insula. We have hypothesized that different areas encode different types of information that support the successful recognition of a familiar individual [13]. While the precuneus and anterior temporal cortex [14], [24], [26], [32] might be involved in retrieval of information from long-term memory, the theory of mind areas might encode information about personality traits and mental states of a familiar individual [12], [23]. The emotional response that we experience when seeing a familiar face plays an important role in successful recognition of that individual [1], [12], [23] and is reflected by changes in activity in the amygdala and the insula.

In our functional model on face perception [17], we grouped face responsive regions in two systems: the core system that includes areas involved with the visual analysis of a face and the extended system that includes areas that are involved in the extraction of other non-visual information.

We were interested in isolating the effect of simple visual familiarity in face recognition. With this purpose, neural activity was recorded with fMRI while participants viewed faces that were visually familiar, due to experimentally induced learning, with no associated semantic information.

Based on our previous findings, we hypothesized that visual familiarity would modulate activity in the “core system”, namely the fusiform gyrus. We also predicted modulation of activity in parts of the extended system, such as in the precuneus, an area involved with the retrieval of episodic memories [3] and in the amygdala, a structure involved in detecting and processing unexpected or unfamiliar events with potential biological importance [5], [7], [12], [22], [23], [31].

We did not expect any modulation of activity in areas involved with the retrieval of personal traits such as the anterior paracingulate cortex [12], [23], [25] or in areas associated with retrieval of biographical information such as the anterior temporal regions [14], [24], [26], [30].

Section snippets

Subjects

Seven healthy right-handed volunteers with no history of neurological or psychiatric disease (four males and three females) participated in the experiment (mean age 30.7, range: 26–34). Subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All participants gave written informed consent.

Stimuli

Stimuli were faces and nonsense pictures (Fig. 1). Nonsense pictures were phase-scrambled images of the faces used in the study and matched the faces in terms of spatial frequencies and luminance.

Faces of 81

Behavioral results

Behavioral data from day 1 of the experiment did not show any difference in reaction time for the learned faces and the highly repeated novel faces (respectively, 457.5 ms, S.D. 195.4 versus 454.7 ms S.D. 189.7, p = 0.8).

The behavioral data from the training session on day 2 of the experiment demonstrated an improvement in performance through a faster reaction time (RT at the beginning of the training session = 1438.0 ms, S.D. 205.3 versus RT at the end of the training session = 285.4 ms, S.D. 60.5, p < 

Discussion

Facial appearance is only one aspect of how we recognize known individuals. In two previous studies, we have demonstrated that recognizing familiar faces activates a distributed network of areas that involves not only the visual perceptual areas but also areas engaged in emotional response, such as the amygdala and the insula, and areas that are involved with social behavior and “theory of mind” [12], [13], [23]. According to our model of face perception [17], recognizing a face is the result

Acknowledgement

We would like to thank Alex Martin for helpful discussion at the inception of this project. We also would like to thank Carlo Cipolli for helpful comments.

References (36)

  • K. Nakamura et al.

    Neural substrates for recognition of familiar voices: a PET study

    Neuropsychologia

    (2001)
  • C.E. Schwartz et al.

    Differential amygdalar response to novel versus newly familiar neutral faces: a functional MRI probe developed for studying inhibited temperament

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2003)
  • C.L. Wiggs et al.

    Properties and mechanisms of perceptual priming

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (1998)
  • A. Bartels et al.

    The neural basis of romantic love

    Neuroreport

    (2000)
  • V. Bruce et al.

    Understanding face recognition

    Br. J. Psychol.

    (1986)
  • S.M. Courtney et al.

    Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory

    Nature

    (1997)
  • M. Davis et al.

    The amygdala: vigilance and emotion

    Mol. Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • R. Desimone

    Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.

    (1996)
  • Cited by (144)

    • The influence of item familiarization on neural discriminability during associative memory encoding and retrieval

      2021, Brain and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Prior work has linked both parietal and frontal cortices to processing of familiarity. For example, the precuneus has been shown to be responsive to stimulus reactivity (Brodt et al., 2016; Engelmann et al., 2012) and exhibits increased activity when processing familiar information (Dorfel, Werner, Schaefer, von Kummer, & Karl, 2009; Lundstrom, Ingvar, & Petersson, 2005; Yonelinas et al., 2005), including face familiarity (Cloutier, Kelley, & Heatherton, 2011; Gobbini & Haxby, 2006; Lee, Leung, Lee, Raine, & Chan, 2013; Maddock, Garrett, & Buonocore, 2001). With respect to reactivation of previously studied information, experience-related changes within parietal cortex have been detected in as short as one hour (Brodt et al., 2018) and shown to occur after a single stimulus presentation (Brodt et al., 2016).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text