Elsevier

Brain, Behavior, and Immunity

Volume 37, March 2014, Pages 134-141
Brain, Behavior, and Immunity

Short exposure to a diet rich in both fat and sugar or sugar alone impairs place, but not object recognition memory in rats

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2013.11.016Get rights and content

Highlight

  • In rats fed high fat/sugar diets, rapid deficits in hippocampal-dependent memory were associated with increased cytokines and oxidative stress markers.

Abstract

High energy diets have been shown to impair cognition however, the rapidity of these effects, and the dietary component/s responsible are currently unclear. We conducted two experiments in rats to examine the effects of short-term exposure to a diet rich in sugar and fat or rich in sugar on object (perirhinal-dependent) and place (hippocampal-dependent) recognition memory, and the role of inflammatory mediators in these responses. In Experiment 1, rats fed a cafeteria style diet containing chow supplemented with lard, cakes, biscuits, and a 10% sucrose solution performed worse on the place, but not the object recognition task, than chow fed control rats when tested after 5, 11, and 20 days. In Experiment 2, rats fed the cafeteria style diet either with or without sucrose and rats fed chow supplemented with sucrose also performed worse on the place, but not the object recognition task when tested after 5, 11, and 20 days. Rats fed the cafeteria diets consumed five times more energy than control rats and exhibited increased plasma leptin, insulin and triglyceride concentrations; these were not affected in the sucrose only rats. Rats exposed to sucrose exhibited both increased hippocampal inflammation (TNF-α and IL-1β mRNA) and oxidative stress, as indicated by an upregulation of NRF1 mRNA compared to control rats. In contrast, these markers were not significantly elevated in rats that received the cafeteria diet without added sucrose. Hippocampal BDNF and neuritin mRNA were similar across all groups. These results show that relatively short exposures to diets rich in both fat and sugar or rich in sugar, impair hippocampal-dependent place recognition memory prior to the emergence of weight differences, and suggest a role for oxidative stress and neuroinflammation in this impairment.

Introduction

Many people in developed countries eat a diet that is rich in saturated fat and refined sugar (Kearney, 2010). Excessive intake of this diet leads to increased body weight, progression into obesity, and the development of metabolic and cardiovascular disorders (Bruce-Keller et al., 2009, Haslam and James, 2005). Epidemiological data has also associated the consumption of high energy diets with an increased risk of age related deficits and neurological diseases including dementia and Alzheimer’s disease (Eskelinen et al., 2008, Grant et al., 2002, Solfrizzi et al., 2010). Studies with rodents have confirmed that the long-term intake of such a diet produces cognitive deficits, especially in spatial tasks that require the hippocampus (Heyward et al., 2012, McNay et al., 2010, Ross et al., 2009). Some of these studies have also identified changes in the hippocampus that may mediate the link between obesity and cognitive deficits. Such changes include oxidative stress, neuroinflammation, as well as decreased levels of neurotrophins (Molteni et al., 2004, Pistell et al., 2010).

Moreover there is very recent evidence that cognitive deficits can not only be produced by long-term excessive intake of a high energy diet but also by relative short exposures to such a diet. Holloway et al. (2011) reported that healthy adults who ate a high fat diet for 5 days performed worse on tasks measuring attention and speed of retrieval than they had prior to the diet. Kanoski and Davidson (2010) also found that rats exposed to a high fat and glucose diet performed worse on a spatial task than chow fed control rats after only 3 days whereas 30 days exposure was necessary for non-spatial memory impairments.

The present experiments constituted a further study of the effects of relatively short-term exposure to a high energy diet on cognition in rats. We set out to confirm and extend previous reports that the consumption of diets high in both fat and sugar is accompanied by impaired memory retention. The modern diet is replete with a diverse range of foods that are rich in fat and sugar and for this reason in the first experiment we chose a cafeteria style diet (cakes, biscuits, lard) supplemented with a 10% sucrose solution. To compare non-spatial and spatial memory we employed the object and place recognition tasks, which are respectively dependent on the perirhinal cortex and the hippocampus (Aggleton and Brown, 2005). We assessed performance on both tasks from 5 to 21 days diet exposure to examine changes in the magnitude of specific deficits. There is some evidence that the hippocampus is particularly sensitive to changes in dietary intake and impairments have been reported on hippocampal-dependent tasks prior to substantial weight gain (Kanoski and Davidson, 2010, Murray et al., 2009). Here we specifically aimed to test short, rather than the more commonly studied long term dietary interventions (3–12 months).

To assess the relative contributions of fat and sugar to the behavioral effects observed, our second experiment included two additional diet groups, a regular diet supplemented with 10% sucrose and the cafeteria style diet without sucrose. At the end of the study we assessed plasma leptin, insulin and triglycerides as well as hippocampal expression of inflammatory markers; tumor necrosis factor (TNF-α) and interleukin 1 (IL-1β), oxidative stress markers; nuclear respiratory factor 1 (NRF1), and sirtuin 3 (SIRT3), as well as brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and neuritin. These markers were selected as CNS inflammation has been reported after as little as 1–3 days on a high energy diet (Hansen et al., 1998, Thaler et al., 2012) and obesity itself is characterized by a state of low grade, chronic inflammation (Bastard et al., 2006). In addition, oxidative stress is believed to be one of the first events following the consumption of a high energy diet and it may be responsible for the decreased levels of BDNF reported in long term diet studies (Molteni et al., 2004). NRF1 is involved in the activation of antioxidant response element-dependent genes (Ohtsuji et al., 2008) while SIRT3 has roles in metabolism, oxidative stress and cell survival (Weir et al., 2013). BDNF has been implicated in a number of processes postulated to underlie hippocampal-dependent learning and memory including long term potentiation, neurogenesis and synaptic plasticity (Stranahan et al., 2008, Yamada et al., 2002).

Section snippets

Subjects

Male Sprague–Dawley rats (Experiment 1: 363–467 g; Experiment 2: 302–366 g; Animal Resource Centre, Perth, WA, Australia) were housed four per polypropylene cage (47 cm × 29 cm × 15 cm) in a temperature controlled (18–20 °C) colony room on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 07:00 h). Rats were acclimatized for 1 week, during which they were maintained ad libitum on standard rat chow (Gordon’s Premium Rat and Mouse Breeder diet, NSW, Australia) and tap water. Prior to diet commencement rats were weight

Effect of diet on body weight and energy intake

A one-way ANOVA of baseline body weight revealed that there was no significant difference between diet conditions (F < 1). All rats gained weight across the diet period (F(5,60) = 243.09, p < .05), and the CAF S+ rats gained weight more rapidly than the RD rats (F(5,60) = 34, p < .05). The main effect of diet was not significant (F(1,12) = 3.07, p = .105). A post hoc analysis revealed that the CAF S+ rats weighed significantly more than control rats after 16 and 21 days on the high energy diet (p < .05). Total

Discussion

Our results confirm and extend previous studies by showing that rats maintained on high energy diets display impaired memory retention. First, the present study showed comparable deficits in the rats place recognition memory following exposure to a cafeteria diet supplemented with 10% sucrose (in two independent cohorts), a regular diet supplemented with 10% sucrose and a cafeteria diet without sucrose. Second, these deficits emerged prior to any differences in body weight. Critically, this

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Fred Westbrook for his invaluable advice and assistance with designing and implementing the behavioral tasks. This work was supported by project grant funding from the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia to Morris and Westbrook.

References (39)

  • R. Molteni et al.

    Exercise reverses the harmful effects of consumption of a high-fat diet on synaptic and behavioral plasticity associated to the action of brain-derived neurotrophic factor

    Neuroscience

    (2004)
  • M. Ohtsuji et al.

    Nrf1 and Nrf2 play distinct roles in activation of antioxidant response element-dependent genes

    J. Biol. Chem.

    (2008)
  • P.J. Pistell et al.

    Cognitive impairment following high fat diet consumption is associated with brain inflammation

    J. Neuroimmunol.

    (2010)
  • A.P. Ross et al.

    A high fructose diet impairs spatial memory in male rats

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (2009)
  • V. Solfrizzi et al.

    Dietary fatty acids in dementia and predementia syndromes: epidemiological evidence and possible underlying mechanisms

    Ageing Res. Rev.

    (2010)
  • A. Wu et al.

    Dietary curcumin counteracts the outcome of traumatic brain injury on oxidative stress, synaptic plasticity, and cognition

    Exp. Neurol.

    (2006)
  • K. Yamada et al.

    Role for brain-derived neurotrophic factor in learning and memory

    Life Sci.

    (2002)
  • J.P. Aggleton et al.

    Contrasting hippocampal and perirhinal cortex function using immediate early gene imaging

    Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B

    (2005)
  • C.L. Andersen et al.

    Normalization of real-time quantitative reverse transcription-PCR data: a model-based variance estimation approach to identify genes suited for normalization, applied to bladder and colon cancer data sets

    Cancer Res.

    (2004)
  • Cited by (182)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text