Elsevier

Acta Psychologica

Volume 132, Issue 1, September 2009, Pages 31-39
Acta Psychologica

Effects of instantaneous object input and past experience on object-based attention

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.02.004Get rights and content

Abstract

Evidence for object-based attention typically comes from studies using displays with unchanged objects, and no consensus has yet been reached as to whether the object effect would be altered by changing object displays or having seen this change across-trials. We examined this by using modifications of the double-rectangle cuing paradigm of Egly et al. [Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: Evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 161–177], and our results, when the objects remained unchanging, replicated the original object effect. However, no object effect was found when the rectangles disappeared from view in the last (target) frame. This was true regardless of the likelihood of the rectangles disappearing, indicating the importance of instantaneous object inputs for object-based attention. The across-trial experience of seeing a different object (boomerang), however, was found to influence the object effect when the cued rectangles persisted throughout the trial. Unlike previous studies, which emphasize one or the other, we demonstrate clearly that instantaneous object inputs and past experience interact to determine the way attention selects objects.

Section snippets

Experiment 1

The rectangles were presented until the onset of the target for 20% of total trials in Experiment 1A and for 80% of the total trials in Experiment 1B. Otherwise, they remained present throughout the trial. In the rectangle-absent trials, the previously presented rectangles disappeared and left only the target (a square) in the target-present trials, and an empty space in the target-absent (catch) trials. According to the cued object hypothesis, once the object is cued and sufficient time is

Results and discussion

The false alarm rate was 2.6% and the miss rate was .05% in Experiment 1A, and, respectively, they were 3.2% and .2% in Experiment 1B. Correct RTs faster than 150 ms and beyond three standard deviations from the mean RT in each condition were removed, which resulted in 2.0% and 1.7% removal rates in Experiments 1A and 1B, respectively.

Correct RTs (Table 1) in Experiments 1A and 1B were submitted to a three-way ANOVA of object-presence probability (20% or 80%) × object (presence or absence) × target

Experiment 2

The previous experiment showed clearly that the object effect were associated with the presence of the object in the target frame but not with the cue, supporting the dynamic updating hypothesis over the cued object hypothesis. However, the probability of the object’s presence had little effect when object inputs were suddenly truncated. Once they are gone, the object effect are gone as well, regardless of how frequently the objects are seen. Is it possible that “disappearance” is too strong to

Experiment 3

In this experiment, the correspondence between the orientations of the cue and boomerang was designed to restore the object effect when the rectangles were continuously presented until the target frame. That is, the boomerang orientation was such that it would cause the object effect in a direction consistent with the rectangles. If the learned correspondence of cue and boomerang orientation in previous trials was important to determine OBA, the object effect should remain when it was the

General discussion

By adopting the cueing paradigm of Egly et al. (1994) and causing the objects in the last frame to disappear or be replaced with another object, this study yielded three important findings. First, instantaneous object inputs were found to be critical in the occurrence of the object effect, which supports the dynamic updating hypothesis over the cued object hypothesis. In Experiment 1, we obtained the object effect only when the rectangles remained in the target frame, but not when they

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from the National Science Council in Taiwan (NSC94-2811-H-002-002, NSC95-2413-H-002-020, and NSC96-2752-H-002-008-PAE). We thank Zhe Chen, Morris Goldsmith, and one anonymous reviewer for their helpful comments, and Yi-Jia Su for her assistance in conducting part of the experiments.

References (43)

  • K.R. Cave et al.

    From searching for features to searching for threat: Drawing the boundary between preattentive and attentive vision

    Visual Cognition

    (2006)
  • Z. Chen

    Switching attention within and between objects: The role of subjective organization

    Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1998)
  • P.E. Downing

    Interactions between visual working memory and selective attention

    Psychological Science

    (2000)
  • J. Duncan

    Selective attention and the organization of visual information

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (1984)
  • R. Egly et al.

    Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: Evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (1994)
  • C.W. Eriksen et al.

    Allocation of attention in the visual field

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1985)
  • M.J. Farah

    Psychophysical evidence for a shared representational medium for mental images and percepts

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (1985)
  • M.J. Farah

    Mechanisms of imagery-perception interaction

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1989)
  • C.L. Folk et al.

    Can new objects override attentional control settings?

    Perception & Psychophysics

    (1999)
  • C.L. Folk et al.

    Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1992)
  • A. Gellatly et al.

    Accuracy of target detection in new-object and old-object displays

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (2000)
  • Cited by (18)

    • Dissociating location-based and object-based cue validity effects in object-based attention

      2018, Vision Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The difference between the RTs of the ID and IF trials was marginally significant (p = .08). That is, Experiment 1 replicated the typical result patterns in studies using the double-rectangle display (e.g., Avrahami, 1999; Chou & Yeh, 2011, 2012; Ho & Yeh, 2009; Lamy & Tsal, 2000; Lin & Yeh, 2011, 2012; Moore et al., 1998) and showed that both the spatial-cueing effect and same-object advantage were found when both the location-based cue and object-based cue were informative. In Experiment 1, the target was 20% less likely to appear in the IS trials than in the ID trials (10% vs. 30%).

    • Forced to remember: When memory is biased by salient information

      2015, Behavioural Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      This is in contrast with the above mentioned approaches (e.g., the saliency computational model by Itti et al. [26]), which model single object features, such as intensity, colour and orientation, and resembles the classic debate of object-based attention [e.g., 90] vs. feature integration theories [23]. The importance of modelling objects instead of simple features would be supported by several recent studies showing that objects predict eye-fixations better than low-level perceptual features [32,91–93; though see 31]. Yanulevskaya et al. [89] implemented a proto-object-based model for visual attention.

    • A model of proto-object based saliency

      2014, Vision Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Most theories and computational models of attention surmise that it is a feature driven process (Itti, Koch, & Niebur, 1998; Koch & Ullman, 1985; Treisman & Gelade, 1980; Walther et al., 2002). However, there is a growing body of evidence, both psychophysical (Cave & Bichot, 1999; Duncan, 1984; Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994; Einhauser, Spain, & Perona, 2008; He & Nakayama, 1995; Ho & Yeh, 2009; Kimchi, Yeshurun, & Cohen-Savransky, 2007; Matsukura & Vecera, 2006; Scholl, 2001) and neurophysiological (Ito & Gilbert, 1999; Qiu, Sugihara, & von der Heydt, 2007; Roelfsema, Lamme, & Spekreijse, 1998; Wannig, Stanisor, & Roelfsema, 2011), which shows that attention does not only depend on image features but also on the structural organization of the scene into perceptual objects. In the Kimchi, Yeshurun, and Cohen-Savransky (2007) experiment a display of 9 red and green, ‘L’-shaped elements was used to show that objects can automatically attract attention in a stimulus driven fashion.

    • Spontaneous Perspective Taking of an Invisible Person

      2022, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text