Elsevier

Neurobiology of Aging

Volume 24, Issue 4, July–August 2003, Pages 615-626
Neurobiology of Aging

Enrichment enhances spatial memory and increases synaptophysin levels in aged female mice

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0197-4580(02)00138-0Get rights and content

Abstract

The present study tested whether environmental enrichment can reduce age-related spatial reference memory deficits and alter synaptic protein levels in aged female mice. Female C57BL/6 mice, (4 or 27–28 months), were tested in spatial and cued Morris water maze tasks. Prior to (14 days) and during testing, a subset of aged females was exposed to rodent toys and running wheels for 3 h per day. The remaining aged females were group housed but were not exposed to enriching objects. At the conclusion of testing, levels of the presynaptic protein synaptophysin were measured in hippocampus and frontoparietal cortex. Enrichment improved spatial memory acquisition; relative to young controls, aged enriched females performed similarly, whereas aged control females were impaired. Enrichment also accelerated the development of a spatial bias in spatial probe trials. In contrast, the cued task was not significantly affected by enrichment. Hippocampal and cortical synaptophysin levels were increased in aged enriched females relative to young and aged controls. These data suggest that environmental enrichment can be a potent cognitive enhancer for aged females and suggests a potential neurobiological mechanism of this effect.

Introduction

Memory decline is a common feature of aging in humans [19], [30], [67]. It is thought that this decline is the result of deterioration in brain regions critical for memory, such as the neocortex, hippocampus, and basal forebrain. The discovery that non-human animals, from rodents to primates, experience similar memory and neural dysfunction to that of humans has spurred decades of animal research designed to develop treatments to alleviate age-related memory loss (see [2], [3] for reviews). Most often, these treatments involve administration of drugs that alter the function of various neurotransmitter or hormone systems. In contrast, few investigations have utilized behavioral manipulations to reduce or prevent age-related memory loss. Emerging evidence suggests that one such intervention, environmental enrichment, may be a particularly effective cognitive enhancer. For example, intellectual abilities in humans are maintained longer in people who continue to engage in intellectually stimulating activities throughout their lives [64], [69]. Although animal models afford the ability to understand the neurobiological basis of this benefit, relatively few studies have examined the effects of this treatment in aging animals.

The impact of environmental stimulation on cognitive and neural development has been known for several decades. Seminal work in the 1960s and 1970s revealed that the cerebral cortices of rats raised in enriched environments (group housed, exposed to stimulus objects) were profoundly altered compared to the brains of rats raised in impoverished conditions (singly housed, no stimulation). Alterations in environmentally enriched rats included increased enzyme levels, cortical thickness, dendritic spines and branching, synaptic contacts and transmission, and neuron size (e.g. see [14], [16], [28], [31], [34], [35], [36], [49], [62] for review). Further, enriched rats exhibited enhanced learning and memory abilities relative to impoverished littermates [9], [37], [73], [80].

Subsequent work indicates that this effect is not limited to early development. Environmental enrichment appears to benefit cortical plasticity and learning abilities at any point in the lifespan [32], [62], [71]. For example, enrichment can completely protect against learning and memory deficits in adult mice with a conditional knockout of the NMDAR1 receptor subunit in the hippocampal CA1 region [61]. Moreover, enrichment can enhance the proliferation of new neurons [47] and dendritic spines [61] in the hippocampus of adult mice. Particularly relevant to aging are studies examining enrichment-induced alterations in middle-aged and aged rodents. In rodents examined at middle-age, enrichment initiated during middle-age increases cortical dendritic branching [32], forebrain weight [13], neurotrophin levels [38], [60], and hippocampal neurogenesis [48], as well as enhances learning of a Hebb-Williams maze [13] and the spatial reference memory version of the Morris water maze [23], [48], [60]. In rodents examined at old age, enrichment initiated either at weaning or during old age, results in a variety of alterations in the brain including increased Purkinje cell dendritic branching [33], increased cortical thickness, RNA content, and presynaptic vesicle number [15], [57], [76], reduced aging-induced hippocampal gliosis [71], and increased cortical and hippocampal levels of the presynaptic protein synaptophysin [63]. Furthermore, enrichment in aged male rodents improves several types of learning, reverses short-term memory deficits, increases spontaneous alternation, and increases food-seeking behaviors [71], [75], [76], [79]. However, the benefit to behavior is not universal; enrichment in aged male rodents does not increase reactivity to spatial novelty or general motor activity, nor does it improve performance in the Lashley III maze or a brightness discrimination [75], [76]. Nevertheless, the data in aging rodents suggest that environmental enrichment can profoundly alter neural structure and improve several types of behavior.

An interesting feature of the research to date examining enrichment in aging rodents is that the vast majority of these studies utilized males as their subjects. This discrepancy may be important, as numerous studies have reported sex differences in both the brain and behavior of young rodents in response to environmental enrichment. For example, sex differences in rats have been reported in cortical and hippocampal dendritic branching [42], [43], myelinated axons in corpus callosum [45], and exploratory behavior [41], despite the fact that male and female rats interact with the enriched environment similarly [46]. Spatial memory, a type of memory particularly vulnerable to decline in aged humans (e.g. [19], [64], [67]), is enhanced by enrichment in both young males and females [44], [66], [73]. However, no study has thus far examined whether enrichment can improve spatial memory or alter synaptic plasticity in aged females. Spatial reference memory, as tested in the Morris water maze, is enhanced by enrichment in middle-aged females [23], [48], as is cortical dendritic branching [32] and hippocampal neurogenesis [48], suggesting that the female brain retains plasticity well into aging. Thus, we hypothesized that enrichment in aged females would reduce spatial memory decline, perhaps by augmenting synaptic plasticity.

The present study was designed to test this hypothesis. Aged group-housed female mice were exposed to an enriched environment for 3 h per day both prior to and during testing in the spatial and cued (non-spatial) versions of the Morris water maze. Their performance was compared to that of young and aged controls who were also group-housed, but not exposed to the enriched environment. Aged female mice, like aged male mice and aged rats, are typically impaired in the spatial version of the Morris water maze, but not the cued version ([20], [22], [25], [26], [29], [50], [52], but see [10], [39]). Thus, enrichment was predicted to affect the spatial, but not the cued, water maze task. Synaptic alterations were examined by measuring levels of the protein synaptophysin in the hippocampus and neocortex, brain regions critical for spatial memory. Synaptophysin is a 38-kDa calcium-binding glycoprotein found in the membranes of neurotransmitter-containing presynaptic vesicles [40], [78]. In non-demented humans, synaptophysin decreases with age in the hippocampus and various cortical regions [17], [51], [55]. In patients with Alzheimer’s disease, synaptophysin immunoreactivity is significantly reduced in frontal cortex and hippocampus, and is correlated with impaired cognitive abilities [51], [72], [74], [82]. In rodents, some studies have found similar age-related reductions in cortical or hippocampal synaptophysin [11], [63], whereas other studies report no age-related changes in this protein [10], [59] or reductions restricted to specific hippocampal subregions [70]. Despite these discrepancies, several studies report significant correlations between synaptophysin levels and spatial memory in the water maze, such that more synaptophysin is associated with better spatial memory [10], [11], [70]. In addition, findings in aged male rats indicate that environmental enrichment can significantly increase presynaptic vesicle number in the frontal cortex [57] and synaptophysin expression in the hippocampus and several cortical regions [63]. Thus, modulation of synaptophysin may play a role in enrichment-induced alterations in memory. The present study will be the first to examine this potential relationship. Our findings suggest that environmental enrichment, even if initiated during old age, can significantly accelerate spatial memory acquisition and increase cortical and hippocampal synaptophysin levels.

Section snippets

Subjects

Subjects were 8 young and 14 aged female C57BL/6 mice obtained at the ages of 3 months and 26–27 months from Hilltop Lab Animals, Inc. (Scottdale, PA). Upon arrival at Yale, the mice were quarantined at our medical school animal facility for 3 weeks. They were then moved to an animal facility adjacent to the laboratory in the Department of Psychology, and handled for five days prior to testing to habituate them to being picked up by the experimenter. Thus, at the beginning of behavioral

Subjects

All mice appeared in good health, although alopecia was evident in most mice. One aged control female and two aged enriched females were excluded from the experiment due to difficulty swimming or inability of the tracking system to detect them consistently (due to alopecia). Thus, the final samples sizes for the behavioral analyses were: young control (n=8), aged control (n=5), and aged enriched (n=6). Two of the young controls were perfused for histological analysis of their brains (data not

Discussion

The results of the present study demonstrate for the first time that environmental enrichment, initiated in old age, can significantly improve spatial memory and increase synaptic protein levels in females. Aged females in this study were exposed to only 14 days of environmental enrichment prior to testing in the water maze. The findings suggest that this enrichment improved acquisition of a spatial reference memory task in aged females. This conclusion is supported by the swim time and swim

Conclusions

The present study indicates that short-term environmental enrichment initiated late in life can accelerate spatial memory acquisition and increase synaptic protein levels in aged females. Our behavioral findings are consistent with previous studies in humans that indicate that intellectual stimulation can delay or prevent age-related memory loss [64], [69]. Our synaptophysin data suggest a potential neurobiological mechanism of this improvement. Thus, aged rodents may provide a relevant animal

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Dr. Greg Rose of Memory Pharmaceuticals, Inc. (Montvale, NJ) for the generous donation of the mice in this study, Tovia Martirosian, Anthony Taglienti, Julie Rosenthal, and Sue Vanderhill for assistance with behavioral testing, and Nancy Stearns and Dr. Joanne Berger-Sweeney for the development of the synaptophysin ELISA protocol. Funding was provided by Yale University.

References (82)

  • M. Gallagher et al.

    Spatial learning deficits in old rats: a model for memory decline in the aged

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (1988)
  • R.B. Gibbs et al.

    Estrogen and basal forebrain cholinergic neurons: implications for brain aging and Alzheimer’s disease-related cognitive decline

    Horm. Behav.

    (1998)
  • A.J. Gower et al.

    The aged mouse as a model of cognitive decline with special emphasis on studies in NMRI mice

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (1993)
  • C.L. Grady et al.

    Changes in memory processing with age

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2000)
  • E.J. Green et al.

    Effects of complex or isolated environments on cortical dendrites of middle-aged rats

    Brain Res.

    (1983)
  • W.T. Greenough et al.

    Environmental conditions modulate degeneration and new dendrite growth in cerebellum of senescent rats

    Brain Res.

    (1986)
  • W.T. Greenough et al.

    Pattern of dendritic branching in occipital cortex of rats reared in complex environments

    Exp. Neurol.

    (1973)
  • W.T. Greenough et al.

    Effects of rearing complexity on dendritic branching in frontolateral and temporal cortex of the rat

    Exp. Neurol.

    (1973)
  • W.T. Greenough et al.

    Possible memory storage differences among mice reared in environments varying in complexity

    Behav. Biol.

    (1972)
  • B. Ickes et al.

    Long-term environmental enrichment leads to regional increases in neurotrophin levels in rat brain

    Exp. Neurol.

    (2000)
  • D.K. Ingram et al.

    Developing mouse models of aging: a consideration of strain differences in age-related behavioral and neural parameters

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (1999)
  • J.M. Juraska

    Sex differences in dendritic response to differential experience in the rat visual cortex

    Brain Res.

    (1984)
  • J.M. Juraska et al.

    Sex differences in the dendritic branching of dentate granule cells following differential experience

    Brain Res.

    (1985)
  • J.M. Juraska et al.

    Sex and environmental influences on the size and ultrastructure of the rat corpus callosum

    Brain Res.

    (1988)
  • Y. Lamberty et al.

    Age-related changes in spontaneous behavior and learning in NMRI mice from maturity to middle age

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1990)
  • K.R. Magnusson

    Influence of dietary restriction on ionotropic glutamate receptors during aging in C57BL mice

    Mech. Ageing Dev.

    (1997)
  • J.F. Nelson et al.

    Neuroendocrine involvement in aging: evidence from studies of reproductive aging and caloric restriction

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (1995)
  • T.M. Pham et al.

    Changes in brain nerve growth factor levels and nerve growth factor receptors in rats exposed to environmental enrichment for one year

    Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • M.R. Rosenzweig et al.

    Psychology of plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain and behavior

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (1996)
  • G. Schlaf et al.

    A novel enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay for determination of synaptophysin as compared with other quantification procedures

    J. Neuroimmunol.

    (1996)
  • M. Soffié et al.

    Behavioural and glial changes in old rats following environmental enrichment

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (1999)
  • M. Van Waas et al.

    Differential environmental modulations on locomotor activity, exploration and spatial behaviour in young and old rats

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1996)
  • B. Wiedenmann et al.

    Identification and localization of synaptophysin, an integral membrane glycoprotein of Mr 38,000 characteristic of presynaptic vesicles

    Cell

    (1985)
  • E.A. Woodcock et al.

    Effects of environmental enrichment on rate of contextual processing and discriminative ability in adult rats

    Neurobiol. Learn Mem.

    (2000)
  • C.S. Woolley

    Estrogen-mediated structural and functional synaptic plasticity in the female rat hippocampus

    Horm. Behav.

    (1998)
  • Barnes CA. Memory changes during normal aging: neurobiological correlates. In: Martinez JLJ, Kesner RP, editors....
  • J. Berger-Sweeney et al.

    Effects of carbon dioxide-induced anesthesia on cholinergic parameters in rat brain

    Lab. Anim. Sci.

    (1994)
  • Berger-Sweeney JE, Stearns NA, Pan JY, Frick KM. Effects of environmental enrichment on GAD activity and synaptophysin...
  • B. Berry et al.

    Spatial learning and memory at defined points of the estrous cycle: effects of performance of a hippocampal-dependent task

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (1997)
  • J.E. Black et al.

    Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult rats

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1990)
  • R.T. Brown

    Early experience and problem-solving ability

    J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.

    (1968)
  • Cited by (262)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text