Skip to main content
Log in

A novel IV cocaine self-administration procedure in rats: differential effects of dopamine, serotonin, and GABA drug pre-treatments on cocaine consumption and maximal price paid

  • Original Investigation
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Rationale

Behavior occurring during cocaine self-administration can be classified as either consummatory or appetitive. These two concepts are usually addressed independently using separate reinforcement schedules. For example, appetitive behavior can be assessed with a progressive ratio schedule, whereas consummatory behavior is typically measured using a fixed ratio schedule.

Objectives

Depending on the schedule used, it is often difficult to determine whether a particular drug pretreatment is affecting self-administration through an effect on appetitive responding, consummatory responding, or perhaps both. In the present study, we tested the effect of pretreating rats with four different drugs on appetitive and consummatory behaviors.

Materials and methods

We recently developed a technique that provides an independent assessment of both behavioral concepts within the same experimental session. In this threshold procedure, rats are offered a descending series of 11 unit doses (422–1.3 μg/injection) during consecutive timed intervals under a fixed-ratio schedule. Consummatory behavior can be analyzed by assessing intake at high unit doses; an estimate of appetitive responding can be determined from responding occurring at the threshold dose. Applying behavioral economics to these data provides dependent measures of consumption when minimally constrained by price and the maximal price paid (P max) for cocaine.

Results

Haloperidol increased cocaine consumption when minimally constrained by price but decreased P max. In contrast, d-amphetamine increased P max. Fluoxetine decreased P max and consumption when minimally constrained by price. Baclofen selectively decreased P max.

Conclusions

These data suggest that drug pretreatments can alter consummatory and appetitive behavior differently because each concept involves distinct neural mechanisms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ahmed SH, Koob GF (1999) Long-lasting increase in the set point for cocaine self-administration after escalation in rats. Psychopharmacol Berl 146:303–312

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Balthazart J, Absil P, Gerard M, Appeltants D, Ball GF (1998) Appetitive and consummatory male sexual behavior in Japanese quail are differentially regulated by subregions of the preoptic medial nucleus. J Neurosci 18:6512–6527

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett AC, Miller JR, Dohrmann JM, Caine SB (2004) Effects of dopamine indirect agonists and selective D1-like and D2-like agonists and antagonists on cocaine self-administration and food maintained responding in rats. Neuropharmacology 47(Suppl 1):256–273

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel WK, DeGrandpre RJ, Higgins ST, Hughes JR (1990) Behavioral economics of drug self-administration. I. Functional equivalence of response requirement and drug dose. Life Sci 47:1501–1510

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Borgland SL, Chang SJ, Bowers MS, Thompson JL, Vittoz N, Floresco SB, Chou J, Chen BT, Bonci A (2009) Orexin A/hypocretin-1 selectively promotes motivation for positive reinforcers. J Neurosci 29:11215–11225

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brebner K, Phelan R, Roberts DC (2000) Effect of baclofen on cocaine self-administration in rats reinforced under fixed-ratio 1 and progressive-ratio schedules. Psychopharmacol Berl 148:314–321

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell UC, Lac ST, Carroll ME (1999) Effects of baclofen on maintenance and reinstatement of intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacol Berl 143:209–214

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Caprioli D, Paolone G, Celentano M, Testa A, Nencini P, Badiani A (2007) Environmental modulation of cocaine self-administration in the rat. Psychopharmacol Berl 192:397–406

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll ME, Lac ST, Asencio M, Kragh R (1990a) Fluoxetine reduces intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 35:237–244

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll ME, Lac ST, Asencio M, Kragh R (1990b) Intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats is reduced by dietary l-tryptophan. Psychopharmacology 100:293–300

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen C, Silberberg A, Hursh S, Huntsberry M, Riley A (2008) Essential value of cocaine and food in rats: tests of the exponential model of demand. Psychopharmacology 198:221–229

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cosgrove KP, Carroll ME (2002) Effects of bremazocine on self-administration of smoked cocaine base and orally delivered ethanol, phencyclidine, saccharin, and food in rhesus monkeys: a behavioral economic analysis. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 301:993–1002

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cowen MS, Krstew E, Lawrence AJ (2007) Assessing appetitive and consummatory phases of ethanol self-administration in C57BL/6J mice under operant conditions: regulation by mGlu5 receptor antagonism. Psychopharmacol Berl 190:21–29

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Craig W (1918) Appetites and aversions as constituents of instincts. Biol Bull 34:91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Czachowski CL, Samson HH (1999) Breakpoint determination and ethanol self-administration using an across-session progressive ratio procedure in the rat. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 23:1580–1586

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • De Wit H, Wise RA (1977) Blockade of cocaine reinforcement in rats with the dopamine receptor blocker pimozide, but not with the noradrenergic blockers phentolamine or phenoxybenzamine. Can J Psychol 31:195–203

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Depoortere RY, Li DH, Lane JD, Emmett-Oglesby MW (1993) Parameters of self-administration of cocaine in rats under a progressive-ratio schedule. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 45:539–548

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Espana RA, Oleson EB, Locke JL, Brookshire BR, Roberts DC, Jones SR (2010) The hypocretin–orexin system regulates cocaine self-administration via actions on the mesolimbic dopamine system. Eur J Neurosci 31:336–348

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foltin RW (2001) Effects of amphetamine, dexfenfluramine, diazepam, and other pharmacological and dietary manipulations on food “seeking” and “taking” behavior in non-human primates. Psychopharmacol Berl 158:28–38

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Foltin RW (2005) Effects of dietary and pharmacological manipulations on appetitive and consummatory aspects of feeding in non-human primates. Appetite 45:110–120

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gan JO, Walton ME, Phillips PE (2010) Dissociable cost and benefit encoding of future rewards by mesolimbic dopamine. Nat Neurosci 13:25–27

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Godsil B, Tinsley M, Fanselow M (2003) Why do theories of behavior need motivational constructs? In: Weiner I (ed) Handbook of psychology, 4th edn. Wiley, New Jersey, pp 33–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald MK, Hursh SR (2006) Behavioral economic analysis of opioid consumption in heroin-dependent individuals: effects of unit price and pre-session drug supply. Drug Alcohol Depend 85:35–48

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hursh SR (1991) Behavioral economics of drug self-administration and drug abuse policy. J Exp Anal Behav 56:377–393

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hursh SR, Silberberg A (2008) Economic demand and essential value. Psychol Rev 115:186–198

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hursh SR, Winger G (1995) Normalized demand for drugs and other reinforcers. J Exp Anal Behav 64:373–384

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kosten TA, Miserendino MJ, Haile CN, DeCaprio JL, Jatlow PI, Nestler EJ (1997) Acquisition and maintenance of intravenous cocaine self-administration in Lewis and Fischer inbred rat strains. Brain Res 778:418–429

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Läck CM, Oleson EB, Roberts DC (2006) Constant subcutaneous amphetamine decreases the motivation to self-administer cocaine, Program No. 591.3 Neuroscience Meeting Planner. Society for Neuroscience, Atlanta, GA (Online)

  • Loh EA, Roberts DC (1990) Break-points on a progressive ratio schedule reinforced by intravenous cocaine increase following depletion of forebrain serotonin. Psychopharmacol Berl 101:262–266

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch WJ, Carroll ME (2001) Regulation of drug intake. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 9:131–143

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Martelle JL, Czoty PW, Nader MA (2008) Effect of time-out duration on the reinforcing strength of cocaine assessed under a progressive-ratio schedule in rhesus monkeys. Behav Pharmacol 19:743–746

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor A, Lacosta S, Roberts DC (1993) l-Tryptophan decreases the breaking point under a progressive ratio schedule of intravenous cocaine reinforcement in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 44:651–655

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nader MA, Hedeker D, Woolverton WL (1993) Behavioral economics and drug choice: effects of unit price on cocaine self-administration by monkeys. Drug Alcohol Depend 33:193–199

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nicola SM, Deadwyler SA (2000) Firing rate of nucleus accumbens neurons is dopamine-dependent and reflects the timing of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats on a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement. J Neurosci 20:5526–5537

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Oleson EB, Roberts DC (2009) Behavioral economic assessment of price and cocaine consumption following self-administration histories that produce escalation of either final ratios or intake. Neuropsychopharmacology 34:796–804

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oleson EB, Talluri S, Childers SR, Smith JE, Roberts DC, Bonin K, Budygin EA (2008) Dopamine uptake changes associated with cocaine self-administration. Neuropsychopharmacology 34:1174–1184

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orio L, Edwards S, George O, Parsons LH, Koob GF (2009) A role for the endocannabinoid system in the increased motivation for cocaine in extended-access conditions. J Neurosci 29:4846–4857

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paredes R, Agmo A (1989) Stereospecific actions of baclofen on sociosexual behavior, locomotor activity and motor execution. Psychopharmacol Berl 97:358–364

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pettit HO, Justice JB Jr (1989) Dopamine in the nucleus accumbens during cocaine self-administration as studied by in vivo microdialysis. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 34:899–904

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pickens R, Thompson T (1968) Cocaine-reinforced behavior in rats: effects of reinforcement magnitude and fixed-ratio size. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 161:122–129

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson NR, Roberts DC (1991) Fluoxetine pretreatment reduces breaking points on a progressive ratio schedule reinforced by intravenous cocaine self-administration in the rat. Life Sci 49:833–840

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson NR, Smith AM, Roberts DC (1994) A single injection of either flupenthixol decanoate or haloperidol decanoate produces long-term changes in cocaine self-administration in rats. Drug Alcohol Depend 36:23–25

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts DC, Corcoran M, Fibiger H (1977) On the role of ascending catecholaminergic systems in intravenous self-administration of cocaine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 6:615–620

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts DC, Loh EA, Vickers G (1989) Self-administration of cocaine on a progressive ratio schedule in rats: dose–response relationship and effect of haloperidol pretreatment. Psychopharmacol Berl 97:535–538

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts DC, Loh E, Baker G, Vickers G (1994) Lesions of central serotonin systems affect responding on a progressive ratio schedule reinforced either by intravenous cocaine or by food. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 49:177–182

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts DC, Andrews MM, Vickers GJ (1996) Baclofen attenuates the reinforcing effects of cocaine in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 15:417–423

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sharpe AL, Samson HH (2001) Effect of naloxone on appetitive and consummatory phases of ethanol self-administration. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 25:1006–1011

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shoaib M, Swanner LS, Beyer CE, Goldberg SR, Schindler CW (1998) The GABAB agonist baclofen modifies cocaine self-administration in rats. Behav Pharmacol 9:195–206

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith RJ, See RE, Aston-Jones G (2009) Orexin/hypocretin signaling at the orexin 1 receptor regulates cue-elicited cocaine-seeking. Eur J Neurosci 30:493–503

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsibulsky VL, Norman AB (1999) Satiety threshold: a quantitative model of maintained cocaine self-administration. Brain Res 839:85–93

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wade-Galuska T, Winger G, Woods JH (2007) A behavioral economic analysis of cocaine and remifentanil self-administration in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacol Berl 194:563–572

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson MC, Hitomi M, Schuster CR (1971) Psychomotor stimulant self administration as a function of dosage per injection in the rhesus monkey. Psychopharmacologia 22:271–281

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wise RA, Newton P, Leeb K, Burnette B, Pocock D, Justice JB Jr (1995) Fluctuations in nucleus accumbens dopamine concentration during intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacol Berl 120:10–20

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Woolverton WL, English JA (1997) Further analysis of choice between cocaine to food using the unit price model of behavioral economics. Drug Alcohol Depend 49:71–78

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yokel R, Wise R (1975) Increased lever pressing for amphetamine after pimozide in rats: implications for a dopamine theory of reward. Science 187:547–549

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zittel-Lazarini A, Cador M, Ahmed SH (2007) A critical transition in cocaine self-administration: behavioral and neurobiological implications. Psychopharmacol Berl 192:337–346

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Funding for this study was provided by grants R01DA14030 (DCSR) and F31DA024525 (EBO). We would like to thank Leanne N. Thomas for technical assistance and Benjamin Zimmer for helpful comments in the preparation of this manuscript. We would especially like to thank Dr. Steve Hursh for providing spreadsheets and mathematical equations that aided in the mathematical behavioral economic analyses.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erik B. Oleson.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Figure 1

Demand curves are shown for individual rats treated with vehicle (right, open circles) and during baseline responding (left, filled circles). P max values were calculated graphically (gP max) as described in the methods and using an equation (cP max). To assess P max mathematically, each animal’s price intake data were curve-fit using the equation: \( { \log }(Q) = { \log }\left( {{Q_0}} \right) + k \times \left( {{e^{{ - \alpha \times {Q_0} \times C}}} - 1} \right) \). Mathematically calculated P max (cP max) was determined to be the unit price at which the first derivative point slope of the function = −1. Q 0, which represents initial consumption at minimal price, and α, which represents the acceleration of the function in response to changes in price, were manipulated to achieve best fit (R 2). Consumption at minimal price was also calculated as an average of cocaine intake across bins 2–4 of the threshold procedure. The value k, which represents the range of consumption in logarithmic units, was set to 2 for all animals. It should be noted that the dependent measures of the maximal price paid (gP max and cP max) are highly correlated, as are the dependent measures of consumption at minimal price (Cons and Q 0; see ESM Figs. S5 and 6 for correlations; see Hursh and Silberberg 2008 for a complete description of this mathematical approach) (DOC 177 kb)

Supplementary Figure 2

Demand curves are shown for individual rats treated with vehicle (right, open circles) and during baseline responding (left, filled circles). P max values were calculated graphically (gP max) as described in “Materials and methods” and using an equation (cP max). To assess P max mathematically, each animal’s price intake data were curve-fit using the equation: \( { \log }(Q) = { \log }\left( {{Q_0}} \right) + k \times \left( {{e^{{ - \alpha \times {Q_0} \times C}}} - 1} \right) \). Mathematically calculated P max (cP max) was determined to be the unit price at which the first derivative point slope of the function = −1. Q0, which represents initial consumption at minimal price, and α, which represents the acceleration of the function in response to changes in price, were manipulated to achieve best fit (R 2). Consumption at minimal price was also calculated as an average of cocaine intake across bins 2–4 of the threshold procedure. The value k, which represents the range of consumption in logarithmic units, was set to 2 for all animals. It should be noted that the dependent measures of the maximal price paid (gP max and cP max) are highly correlated, as are the dependent measures of consumption at minimal price (Cons and Q 0; see ESm Figs. S5 and 6 for correlations; see Hursh and Silberberg 2008 for a complete description of this mathematical approach) (DOC 177 kb)

Supplementary Figure 3

Demand curves are shown for individual rats treated with vehicle (right, open circles) and during baseline responding (left, filled circles). P max values were calculated graphically (gP max) as described in “Materials and methods” and using an equation (cP max). To assess P max mathematically, each animal’s price intake data were curve-fit using the equation: \( { \log }(Q) = { \log }\left( {{Q_0}} \right) + k \times \left( {{e^{{ - \alpha \times {Q_0} \times C}}} - 1} \right) \). Mathematically calculated P max (cP max) was determined to be the unit price at which the first derivative point slope of the function = −1. Q 0, which represents initial consumption at minimal price, and α, which represents the acceleration of the function in response to changes in price, were manipulated to achieve best fit (R 2). Consumption at minimal price was also calculated as an average of cocaine intake across bins 2–4 of the threshold procedure. The value k, which represents the range of consumption in logarithmic units, was set to 2 for all animals. It should be noted that the dependent measures of the maximal price paid (gP max and cP max) are highly correlated, as are the dependent measures of consumption at minimal price (Cons and Q 0; see ESM Figs. S5 and 6 for correlations; see Hursh and Silberberg 2008 for a complete description of this mathematical approach) (DOC 172 kb)

Supplementary Figure 4

Demand curves are shown for individual rats treated with vehicle (right, open circles) and during baseline responding (left, filled circles). P max values were calculated graphically (gP max) as described in the methods and using an equation (cP max). To assess P max mathematically, each animal’s price intake data were curve-fit using the equation: \( { \log }(Q) = { \log }\left( {{Q_0}} \right) + k \times \left( {{e^{{ - \alpha \times {Q_0} \times C}}} - 1} \right) \). Mathematically calculated P max (cP max) was determined to be the unit price at which the first derivative point slope of the function = −1. Q 0, which represents initial consumption at minimal price, and α, which represents the acceleration of the function in response to changes in price, were manipulated to achieve best fit (R 2). Consumption at minimal price was also calculated as an average of cocaine intake across bins 2–4 of the threshold procedure. The value k, which represents the range of consumption in logarithmic units, was set to 2 for all animals. It should be noted that the dependent measures of the maximal price paid (gP max and cP max) are highly correlated, as are the dependent measures of consumption at minimal price (Cons and Q 0; see ESM Figs. S5 and 6 for correlations; see Hursh and Silberberg 2008 for a complete description of this mathematical approach) (DOC 134 kb)

Supplementary Figure 5

Measures of consummatory behavior are not related to measures of appetitive behavior (TIFF 8222 kb)

Supplementary Figure 6

The dependent measure of consummatory behavior determined by averaging intake across bins 2-4 (Cons) correlates highly with a mathematically derived value of consumption at minimal price (Q0) (TIFF 8222 kb)

Supplementary Figure 7

Graphically determined Pmax (gPmax) correlates highly with mathematically calculated Pmax (cPmax) (TIFF 8222 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Oleson, E.B., Richardson, J.M. & Roberts, D.C.S. A novel IV cocaine self-administration procedure in rats: differential effects of dopamine, serotonin, and GABA drug pre-treatments on cocaine consumption and maximal price paid. Psychopharmacology 214, 567–577 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-2058-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-2058-6

Keywords

Navigation