PT - JOURNAL ARTICLE AU - Ray Dingledine TI - Why Is It so Hard to Do Good Science? AID - 10.1523/ENEURO.0188-18.2018 DP - 2018 Sep 01 TA - eneuro PG - ENEURO.0188-18.2018 VI - 5 IP - 5 4099 - http://www.eneuro.org/content/5/5/ENEURO.0188-18.2018.short 4100 - http://www.eneuro.org/content/5/5/ENEURO.0188-18.2018.full SO - eNeuro2018 Sep 01; 5 AB - “Good science” means answering important questions convincingly, a challenging endeavor under the best of circumstances. Our inability to replicate many biomedical studies has been the subject of numerous commentaries both in the scientific and lay press. In response, statistics has re-emerged as a necessary tool to improve the objectivity of study conclusions. However, psychological aspects of decision making introduce preconceived preferences into scientific judgment that cannot be eliminated by any statistical method. The psychology of decision making, expounded by Kahneman, Tversky, and Thaler, is well known in the field of economics, but the underlying concepts of cognitive psychology are also relevant to scientific judgments. I repeated experiments carried out on undergraduates by Kahneman and colleagues four to five decades ago, but with scientists, and obtained essentially the same results. The experiments were in the form of written reactions to scenarios, and participants were scientists at all career stages. The findings reinforce the roles that two inherent intuitions play in scientific decision making: our drive to create a coherent narrative from new data regardless of its quality or relevance and our inclination to seek patterns in data whether they exist or not. Moreover, we do not always consider how likely a result is regardless of its p value. Low statistical power and inattention to principles underpinning Bayesian statistics reduce experimental rigor, but mitigating skills can be learned. Overcoming our natural human tendency to make quick decisions and jump to conclusions is a deeper obstacle to doing good science; this too can be learned.