Abstract
Primary sensory cortices display functional topography, suggesting that even small cortical volumes may underpin perception of specific stimuli. Traditional loss-of-function approaches have a relatively large radius of effect (>1 mm), and few studies track recovery following loss-of-function perturbations. Consequently, the behavioral necessity of smaller cortical volumes remains unclear. In the mouse primary vibrissal somatosensory cortex (vS1), “barrels” with a radius of ∼150 μm receive input predominantly from a single whisker, partitioning vS1 into a topographic map of well defined columns. Here, we train animals implanted with a cranial window over vS1 to perform single-whisker perceptual tasks. We then use high-power laser exposure centered on the barrel representing the spared whisker to produce lesions with a typical volume of one to two barrels. These columnar-scale lesions impair performance in an object location discrimination task for multiple days without disrupting vibrissal kinematics. Animals with degraded location discrimination performance can immediately perform a whisker touch detection task with high accuracy. Animals trained de novo on both simple and complex whisker touch detection tasks showed no permanent behavioral deficits following columnar-scale lesions. Thus, columnar-scale lesions permanently degrade performance in object location discrimination tasks.
Significance Statement
Sensory cortical inactivation perturbs many behaviors. Whether small patches of sensory cortex are necessary for behavior remains unclear as typical inactivation methods have poor resolution (>1 mm), and few studies allow for behavioral recovery. In the rodent whisker system, individual whiskers map to cortical “barrels” with a radius of ∼150 μm. We produce lesions on the scale of one to two barrels. We find that such lesions permanently degrade performance in tasks that require mice to discriminate between object locations using their whiskers, but not in tasks that require mice to report the presence or absence of an object. Thus, volumes of somatosensory cortex containing around 10,000 neurons act as perceptual bottlenecks in object location discrimination but not object detection behaviors.
Introduction
To understand the neural basis of perception, it is important to identify brain areas that causally contribute to behavior. The behavioral necessity of a brain area is typically assessed using loss-of-function perturbations. Across sensory modalities, such experiments suggest that primary sensory cortices are essential for some tasks but not others (Lashley, 1931a,b; Schneider, 1969; Hutson and Masterton, 1986; Newsome and Paré, 1988; Prusky and Douglas, 2004; Glickfeld et al., 2013; Shih et al., 2013; Poort et al., 2015; Goard et al., 2016; Resulaj et al., 2018; Stüttgen and Schwarz, 2018; Slonina et al., 2022). Typical loss-of-function experiments inactivate spatially extensive tissue volumes and rarely allow for behavioral recovery, complicating interpretation and leaving open the question of whether small cortical volumes act as permanent perceptual bottlenecks.
Traditional loss-of-function perturbations have poor spatial resolution: permanent lesions (Lashley, 1931a; Hutson and Masterton, 1986; Resulaj et al., 2018) typically have a radius in excess of 1 mm, impacting hundreds of thousands of neurons. Transient loss-of-function experiments require careful calibration to measure effect radius. Unfortunately, this is rarely done, and when it is, the radius of effect is found to often be in excess of 1 mm: optogenetic silencing drives inactivation >1 mm away even when the spatial extent of stimulation light is far smaller (Li et al., 2019); pharmacological inactivation requires careful calibration to achieve a radius of effect of ∼1 mm (Krupa et al., 1999); Peltier cooling exerts effects at least 1 mm away (Long and Fee, 2008). In addition, most loss-of-function studies do not allow time for behavioral recovery, leaving open the question of whether other structures could compensate for the experimentally induced loss. Indeed, lesions of brain areas sensitive to transient inactivation reveal that behavioral recovery can occur relatively quickly, and it is often unclear whether sensitivity to transient inactivation is because of method-specific downstream effects (Otchy et al., 2015; Hong et al., 2018; Wolff and Ölveczky, 2018). Thus, more spatially focused loss-of-function approaches combined with long-term monitoring of behavior are essential for identifying the minimal subset of cortical activity needed for specific behaviors.
In the mouse vibrissal system, individual whiskers project to small, defined patches of primary vibrissal somatosensory cortex (vS1) called “barrels” (radius, ∼150 μm; Lefort et al., 2009). Most inactivation studies in vS1 do not explore the behavioral contribution of individual barrels because of poor spatial resolution. For instance, optogenetic inactivation experiments targeting vS1 often directly perturb activity not just in vS1 but also in adjacent structures such as secondary vibrissal somatosensory cortex. Lesions of vS1 are also usually extensive, often impacting adjacent cortical areas along with subcortical structures (Hutson and Masterton, 1986; Hong et al., 2018). Inactivation of vS1 degrades performance on aperture size discrimination (Krupa et al., 2001), object location discrimination (O’Connor et al., 2010; Guo et al., 2014b), gap crossing (Hutson and Masterton, 1986; Shih et al., 2013), texture discrimination (Guic-Robles et al., 1992), and whisker touch detection (Miyashita and Feldman, 2013; Sachidhanandam et al., 2013; Hong et al., 2018) tasks. However, whether vS1 is necessary for these behaviors remains unclear: mice can relearn a touch detection task in a single day following vS1 aspiration, but transient optogenetic inactivation degrades performance (Hong et al., 2018). While vS1 is thus implicated in many tasks, in most cases, it remains unclear whether perceptually relevant activity is mostly confined to the barrels of specific whiskers and whether observed behavioral deficits are permanent.
Here, we precisely lesion small volumes of barrel cortex in awake mice previously implanted with a cranial window using a femtosecond laser. Our approach obviates the need for postoperative recovery and allows for the lesioning of a targeted barrel along with partial removal of adjacent barrels. We perform lesions immediately before behavioral testing, and track performance for several days to allow for behavioral recovery. In mice performing a vibrissal go/no-go object location discrimination task with a single whisker, lesions centered around the barrel representing the spared whisker persistently degraded performance for several days. Lesions did not impact whisking kinematics. Mice trained on a go/no-go object detection task showed a small, transient decline in performance after a columnar-scale lesion in vS1, suggesting that such tasks are not dependent on individual vS1 barrels. Mice trained on a more complex detection task with two response contingencies and a delay period between stimulus presentation and response also showed only a small and transient decline in performance after columnar-scale lesions, suggesting that even this more complex detection task ultimately does not require individual barrels. Thus, we find that columnar-scale vS1 lesions result in permanent performance degradation on vibrissal object location discrimination but not touch detection tasks.
Materials and Methods
Animals
Adult C57BL/6J (stock #000664, The Jackson Laboratory) mice (9 female, 13 male) and Ai162 (stock #031562, The Jackson Laboratory) × Slc17a7-Cre (stock #023527, The Jackson Laboratory; Daigle et al., 2018) mice (4 female, 3 male) were used (Table 1). In cortex, Ai162 × Slc17a7-Cre mice express GCaMP6s in excitatory neurons. To suppress transgene expression during development, Ai162 × Slc17a7-Cre breeders were fed a diet that included doxycycline (625 mg/kg doxycycline; Teklad), so that mice received doxycycline until weaning. All animal procedures were approved by the New York University Animal Welfare Committee.
Surgery
Mice (6–10 weeks old) were anesthetized with isoflurane during cranial window and headbar implantation (3% induction, 1.5% maintenance). A titanium headbar was attached to the skull with cyanoacrylate (Vetbond). A circular craniotomy (diameter, 3.5 mm) was made in the left hemisphere over vS1 (center: 3.5 mm lateral, 1.5 mm posterior from bregma) using a dental drill (FG 1/4 drill bit, Midwest Tradition). Following the craniotomy, a double-layer cranial window (external diameter, 4.5 mm; inner diameter, 3.5 mm; #1.5 coverslip; adhered with Optical Adhesive 61, Norland Products) was placed over the craniotomy. The cranial window and headbar were affixed to the skull with dental acrylic (Orthojet, Lang Dental).
Behavior
Following surgical recovery, mice were water restricted and trimmed to whiskers C1 to C3. Mice were trained on one of the following three tasks. (1) Go/no-go object location discrimination task. A metal pole (diameter, 0.5 mm; Drummund Scientific) moved vertically (i.e., along the dorsal–ventral axis of the animal) into the mouse’s whisking plane in a series of proximal positions or in a series of distal positions. Mice were rewarded with water for licking while the pole was in reach if the pole appeared in one of the proximal positions (in reach time, 2–3 s). Licking when the pole appeared in one of the distal positions led to a timeout period and an aversive sound. (2) Go/no-go object detection task. A metal pole moved vertically into the mouse’s whisking plane in or out of reach of its one spared whisker. Mice were rewarded for licking when the pole was in reach and given a timeout period and an aversive sound for licking when the pole was out of reach. (3) Lick-left/lick-right object detection task. Each trial consisted of three epochs: stimulus epoch (2 s); delay (300 ms); and response (<2 s). During the stimulus epoch, a metal pole moved vertically into the whisking plane of the mouse at an in-reach proximal position or an out-of-reach distal position and then moved outside the whisking plane. During the delay epoch, the lickport moved into reach. During the response epoch, an auditory cue indicated the mouse should respond, and mice were rewarded for licking the right lickport during in reach trials, and the left lickport during out-of-reach trials. Incorrect responses resulted in a timeout period and immediate withdrawal of the lickport.
All behavioral training proceeded in a standard sequence. First, mice were handled and habituated to the behavioral apparatus. After habituation, mice were trained to lick for a water reward and then proceeded directly to behavioral training on the go/no-go tasks. For the two-lickport task, mice also learned the timing of the task and to lick both left and right lickports before training progressed by using blocks where only a single lickport was rewarded. In the location discrimination task, mice started with distal positions out of reach. Distal positions were quickly brought into reach once mice became accustomed to the task structure and whisked vigorously. In detection tasks, the pole was presented in a position that was easily reached but did require active whisking. All animals began training on dedicated training rigs. Once animals learned the task using whiskers C1 to C3 (i.e., attained a d′ > 1.5), animals were trimmed to a single whisker (typically C2, occasionally C3; Table 1). Subsequent trimming occurred every 2–3 d. To collect whisker video during behavior, mice were moved to a rig with whisker videography once they were proficient at the final version of their task. Lesions in these animals were not performed until they exhibited stable performance on the whisker videography rig, as some animals took one to two sessions to adjust to the new apparatus.
The behavioral task was controlled by a BPod State Machine (Sanworks) and custom MATLAB software (MathWorks) running on a behavioral computer (System 76). The auditory response tone was controlled by a low-latency audio board (Bela). Lickport motion was controlled by a set of three motorized actuators (Zaber) and an Arduino. Licks were sensed using a custom electrical detection circuit.
Barrel identification
For the Ai162 × Slc17a7-Cre mice, the locations of barrels in vS1 corresponding to whiskers C1 to C3 were identified by measuring the GCaMP6s ΔF/F at coarse resolution (4×; field of view, 2.2 × 2.2 mm) on a two-photon microscope while the whiskers were individually deflected with the standard stimulus pole. This was done in awake mice not engaged in a task. Barrel locations in the C57BL/6J mice were identified using intrinsic signal imaging while the whiskers were individually deflected. In this case, mice previously implanted with vS1 cranial windows were anesthetized with isoflurane (typically, 1%). Imaging was performed with a 10 bit ACE camera (Basler) mounted on a stereoscope and using custom MATLAB software. Green illumination (catalog #M530L4, Thorlabs) was used to image the vasculature, and red illumination (catalog #M625L4, Thorlabs) was used for functional imaging. Individual whiskers were placed inside a capillary tube and stimulated with a piezo stimulator (catalog #PB4NB2S, Thorlabs). Stimulation consisted of 5 repetitions of 10 deflections, each lasting 50 ms, with a 100 ms interval in between. Deflections typically resulted in an angular displacement of 5–10° at the follicle. For each whisker, the region with the highest change in reflectance (ΔR/R) was mapped with respect to the vasculature, and two to three whiskers were mapped per mouse.
Whisker videography
Whisker video was acquired using custom MATLAB software from a CMOS (complementary metal–oxide–semiconductor) camera (model Ace-Python 500, Basler) running at 400 Hz and 640 × 352 pixels and using a telecentric lens (model TitanTL, Edmund Optics). Illumination was provided by a pulsed 940 nm LED (model SL162, Advanced Illumination) operating in synchrony with the camera (typical exposure and illumination duration, 200 μs). Seven to nine seconds of each trial were recorded, including 1 s before pole movement, the period when the pole was in reach, and several seconds after the pole was retracted. Data were processed at the High Performance Computing cluster at New York University. First, candidate whiskers were detected using the Janelia Whisker Tracker (Clack et al., 2012). Next, whisker identity was refined and assessed across a single session using custom MATLAB software (Peron et al., 2015, 2020). Following whisker assignment, whisker curvature (κ) and angle (θ) were calculated at specific locations along the length of the whisker.
Change in curvature, Δκ, was calculated relative to a resting angle-dependent baseline curvature value obtained during periods when the pole was out of reach. Next, automatic touch detection was performed. Touch assignment was manually curated using a custom MATLAB user interface (Peron et al., 2015). As per convention, protractions were assigned negative Δκ values. The angle (θ) was decomposed into whisking setpoint, amplitude, and phase via the Hilbert transform (Kleinfeld and Deschênes, 2011).
Lesions
Cortical lesions were performed using a 1040 nm laser (Fidelity HP, Coherent) focused at a depth of 200–300 μm for 10–20 s at 1–1.5 W power. Lesions were either centered on the target barrel (experimental lesions) or in visual cortex (posterior and medial relative to the target barrel; sham lesions). Lesions targeted a single site. In Ai162 × Slc17a7-Cre mice, the desired barrel was found using coarse resolution two-photon imaging (4×), and lesion success was visually confirmed by an increase in GCaMP6s fluorescence in the target area. In C57BL/6J mice, the target barrel was found using epifluorescence imaging of the vasculature and the intrinsic imaging map as a reference. Lesion efficacy was often validated by performing an identical lesion in an Ai162 × Slc17a7 mouse immediately before lesioning of trained C57BL/6J mice. Because these mice expressed GCaMP6s, lesion extent could be calibrated based on the post lesion fluorescence. Animals were awake and head fixed in the behavioral apparatus during lesioning and were monitored for signs of distress or discomfort. Most animals were lesioned 5–30 min before the start of behavior, although a few animals were lesioned in the middle of a behavioral session.
Following behavioral training and lesioning, all animals were perfused with paraformaldehyde (4% in PBS) and postfixed overnight. Coronal sections 70–80 μm thick were cut on a vibratome (Leica) and mounted on glass slides with VECTASHIELD antifade mounting media containing DAPI (Vector Laboratories). Sections were imaged using a fluorescent light microscope (model VS120, Olympus).
To quantify lesion volume, we used DAPI (C57BL/6J mice) or GCAMP6s fluorescence (Ai162 × Slc17a7-Cre mice). Serial images of the lesion were collected using a fluorescent light microscope (model VS120, Olympus). All images in which a lesion was present were registered to the Allen Mouse Brain Common Coordinate framework using the SHARP-Track (Slice Histology Alignment, Registration, and Probe-Track) pipeline (Shamash et al., 2018). In this way, lesions from each animal were normalized to the standard brain atlas and lesions could be compared across animals. Lesion volume was quantified by measuring the area of manually delimited lesion borders across adjacent sections and calculating the volume as follows: V = (A1 + A2 + … + An) * t, where An is the lesion area in the final slice containing a lesion, and t is the thickness at which sections were sliced (Shih et al., 2013). For animals where some lesion slices were not recoverable, we estimated the lesion volume from the available slices. The slice with the largest lesion area was taken to be the maximum area the lesion reached across all slices. We linearly fit known volumes and maximal area across the animals for which all slices were present and used this fit along with the maximal area to estimate lesion volume. Animals for which volume was estimated in this manner are denoted with a dagger symbol in Table 1.
Immunohistochemistry
Lesions were performed as described previously in two Ai162 × Slc17a7-Cre mice. Perfusion was performed 24 h after lesioning. The 50-μm-thick sections were then cut on a vibratome (Leica), and the sections that included the lesion were incubated overnight with primary antibodies made in 1% bovine serum albumin and 0.05% sodium azide under continuous agitation. Alternating slices were labeled for Iba1 and glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP; Podgorski and Ranganathan, 2016). Slices were washed three times and then incubated in a secondary antibody (1:500) conjugated to Alexa Fluor 647. Slices were rinsed and mounted using antifade mounting media (Vector Laboratories). Slices were imaged using a microscope (model VS120, Olympus) and a confocal microscope (model SP5, Leica).
The following primary antibodies were used: rabbit anti-Iba1 (1:500 dilution; catalog #019–19 741, Wako), mouse monoclonal anti-GFAP (1:1000 dilution; catalog #G3893, Sigma-Aldrich). The following secondary antibodies used were used: goat anti-mouse, Alexa Fluor 647 (Iba1 and HSP; catalog #A-21244, Thermo Fisher Scientific); and goat anti-rabbit, Alexa Fluor 647 (GFAP; catalog #A-21235, Thermo Fisher Scientific).
Experimental design and statistical analyses
For comparisons between distinct samples, two-tailed unpaired t tests were used. For longitudinal comparisons within the same animals, paired t tests were used. For correlation tests, a Pearson’s correlation was used to identify a linear correlation coefficient (R) and test for significance. Ranges indicate mean ± SEM, unless indicated otherwise. All statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB. An exact list of animals used for each experiment is provided in Table 1.
Results
Mice use touch to solve a single whisker go/no-go object location discrimination task
We trained water-restricted mice implanted with a cranial window over vS1 to perform a head-fixed go/no-go object location discrimination task using a single whisker (Fig. 1A; Materials and Methods). Mice received a water reward for licking when a vertical pole was presented in a series of proximal positions (“go” trials). On “no-go” trials, the pole appeared in a series of distal positions and licking responses resulted in a timeout. Mice became proficient at this task in 12.9 ± 2.0 d (mean ± SD; n = 10 mice; Fig. 1B).
Once mice were proficient at the task, we used high-speed videography to assess the impact of vibrissal-object touch on behavior (Materials and Methods; Fig. 1C). Mice were sensitive to the number of object touches, licking more often on trials with a higher number of touches (Fig. 1D). The number of touches was position dependent, with anterior positions typically eliciting fewer touches (Fig. 1E,F). Across all pole positions, a higher number of touches was more likely to elicit a lick response.
Vibrissal S1 touch neurons show increasing responses to higher contact forces (Peron et al., 2015). We therefore examined the impact of touch force on behavior, using the net whisker curvature change on each trial as a proxy for force acting on the whisker follicle (Birdwell et al., 2007; Pammer et al., 2013). Force on the whisker follicle drives primary sensory afferent responses (Severson et al., 2017). Mice licked more frequently on trials with greater contact forces (Fig. 1G). At more posterior positions, contact force was higher, and, for any given pole position, higher contact force increased the likelihood of licking (Fig. 1H,I). Thus, both the frequency and intensity of touch impacted behavior in our task.
Mice often exhibit vibrissal foveation in object localization tasks, restricting whisker movement to the proximal positions, rather than whisking through their full whisking extent (O’Connor et al., 2010). This transforms the behavior from a location discrimination task that likely requires knowledge of whisker position and sensorimotor integration to a task where the animal simply must lick on any detected touch. To counteract this tendency, we used a series of proximal and distal pole positions and presented these positions at different frequencies tailored to individual mice (Fig. 2A). This resulted in a high number of object contacts at both proximal and distal pole positions (Fig. 2B,C). Most mice showed performance decrements on proximal trials where no touch occurred, although a few mice showed equal performance for proximal pole positions on touch and nontouch trials (Fig. 2D). Mice consistently performed worse on distal trials where touch occurred compared with distal trials without touch. Therefore, mice naturally tend toward a detection strategy, but our multiposition approach ensures that they continue to touch across both position ranges, necessitating that they perform an object location discrimination task.
Columnar-scale vS1 lesions via prolonged femtosecond laser exposure
To determine whether individual vS1 columns are necessary for object location discrimination, we lesioned a target barrel along with portions of adjacent barrels in well trained animals and tracked performance for several days following the lesion (Fig. 3A; Materials and Methods). Barrels were first identified using intrinsic signal imaging (Fig. 3B), and their locations were mapped onto the vasculature (Materials and Methods). Lesions were performed by subjecting the target barrel to prolonged femtosecond laser exposure immediately before the first post lesion behavioral session. Lesions did not require any surgery or anesthesia as they were performed on mice previously implanted with cranial windows. Lesion volumes were comparable to the volume of two barrels: 0.17 ± 0.09 mm3 (mean ± SEM; n = 7 mice; volume of a single barrel: 0.09 mm3, assuming a ∼300 μm diameter cylinder spanning 1.288 mm in cortical depth; Lefort et al., 2009; exact size: 1.9 ± 0.1 barrels or 12,300 ± 647 neurons; Fig. 3C,D). Lesions did not evoke distal microglial or astrocytic reactions (Fig. 3E) and did not penetrate the white matter (Fig. 3F). Thus, our approach allows for lesions on the scale of individual cortical columns.
Columnar-scale vS1 lesions impair performance on go/no-go object location discrimination task
We next examined the impact of columnar-scale lesions on performance for the go/no-go object location discrimination task. We recorded high-speed whisker video for at least 2 d before the lesion and 3 d following the lesion (Fig. 3A). To assess behavior on trials where the animal was performing object location discrimination, we restricted our analyses to trials on which touch occurred (73.7 ± 2.7% of trials on pre lesion sessions, 71.7 ± 4.2% of trials post lesion, n = 7 mice). Following a lesion, performance on touch trials declined from 74.4 ± 2.5% correct to 62.4 ± 0.8% (Fig. 4A; p = 0.002, paired t test comparing day before lesion to day of lesion; n = 7 mice). We checked for recovery in the days following the lesion, but performance remained low on the third postlesion session (day 2 post lesion: 60.3 ± 1.6%; p = 0.221, lesion day vs day 2 post lesion; n = 7). The decline in performance was not sensitive to the size of the lesion (R = 0.48, p = 0.468, Pearson correlation; Fig. 4B). This decline was primarily because of an increase in the fraction of false alarms (from 29.2 ± 3.1% to 70.2 ± 5.6%; p < 0.001, paired t test comparing day before to the day after lesion; n = 7 mice; Fig. 4C). Sham lesions of comparable size (0.17 ± 0.13 mm3; n = 5 mice; p = 0.989, unpaired t test comparing sham to vS1 lesion volumes; Fig. 3D) in visual areas posterior and medial to vS1 performed in a subset of animals did not produce behavioral effects (performance before: 74.5 ± 2.8%; after: 74.8 ± 2.9%; p = 0.945; n = 5; Fig. 4D). Thus, columnar-scale vS1 lesions drive irreversible declines in performance on object location discrimination tasks.
The increase in false alarm rate on no-go (distal position) trials suggested that post lesion, mice adopted a strategy of licking on object contact regardless of object position and withholding licking when touch did not occur. To test this hypothesis, following the first 3 post lesion days, mice were transitioned to a go/no-go lick-on-touch detection task in which the distal pole positions were completely out of reach and mice simply had to lick on trials where the pole was in reach (Fig. 5A). Upon moving lesioned mice to this task, their performance improved from 58.4 ± 1.8% to 88.9 ± 2.5% (Fig. 5B; p < 0.001, paired t test comparing last post lesion location discrimination day to first detection day; n = 7 mice). This improvement was mostly because of a decline in false alarm rate (from 71.6 ± 5.1% to 10.0 ± 2.7%; p < 0.001; n = 7 mice; Fig. 5C), implying that lesions had not impaired the capacity of mice to perform at high levels within this go/no-go task structure but instead had interfered with sensory input. In all mice, performance on this detection task exceeded the performance on the final pre lesion object location discrimination task day (p < 0.001; n = 7), implying that mice performing the object location discrimination task are fundamentally perceptually limited relative to mice performing the detection task.
If mice had indeed adopted a lick-on-touch strategy following the lesion, mice experiencing no touch should withhold licking. To evaluate this possibility, we cut the remaining whisker after 2 days of the detection task and examined the impact on behavior. Performance dropped to chance (performance before trimming, 86.5 ± 4.6%; performance after trimming, 50.0 ± 0.5%; p < 0.001; n = 7; Fig. 5D). All mice adopted a constant lick rate, as evidenced by a matched hit and false alarm rate (Fig. 5E), consistent with a guessing strategy. Most mice licked on <25% of trials, substantially less than before trimming, despite their being highly motivated to do so. This is consistent with a lick-on-touch strategy.
Thus, columnar-scale vS1 lesions persistently disrupted performance on an object location discrimination task. Mice adopted a lick-on-touch strategy following the lesion, as demonstrated by their ability to immediately perform a detection task and their near cessation of licking following whisker trimming.
Columnar-scale lesions do not impact vibrissal kinematics
Area scale lesions and optogenetic silencing of vS1 alter vibrissal kinematics, with mice exhibiting reduced whisking vigor and lower touch intensities (Hong et al., 2018). Do columnar-scale lesions drive shifts in vibrissal kinematics? To address this question, we compared kinematics before and after lesions (Fig. 6A). In mice performing the go/no-go location discrimination task, columnar-scale lesions did not show any effect on the kinematic parameters we measured. Mice made 3.6 ± 1.4 touches per trial before the lesion, and 3.9 ± 2.4 touches per trial after (Fig. 6B; p = 0.872, paired t test; n = 7 mice). The intensity of the touches also did not change, quantified using the net curvature change per trial (net Δκ; Fig. 6C; before lesion, 0.014 ± 0.006 mm−1; after lesion, 0.023 ± 0.013 mm−1; p = 0.079; Materials and Methods). Peak curvature change was not impacted by lesions (Fig. 6D; before lesion, 0.0011 ± 0.0006 mm−1; after lesion, 0.0013 ± 0.0005 mm−1; p = 0.395). Whisking intensity also remained constant (Fig. 6E–G; Materials and Methods), quantified using peak whisking amplitude (before lesion, 3.5 ± 2.1°; after lesion, 3.4 ± 1.5°; p = 0.780), peak setpoint (before lesion, 4.1 ± 6.4°; after lesion, 6.7 ± 5.9°; p = 0.325), and peak velocity (before lesion, 332.6 ± 177.8°/s; after lesion, 291.54 ± 74.3°/s; p = 0.579). Thus, whisking kinematics were not altered by columnar-scale lesions, implying that observed behavioral changes were not because of changes in afferent input.
Columnar-scale lesions interfere weakly and transiently with simple and complex vibrissal object detection tasks
Mice with vS1 lesions show degraded performance on an object location discrimination task but attain high performance on a lick-on-touch detection task (Fig. 5B). We therefore asked whether columnar-scale lesions to vS1 would perturb performance in mice trained only on a detection task. Following area-scale vS1 lesions, mice exhibit temporary declines in performance on a go/no-go touch detection task (Hong et al., 2018). We therefore trained a separate cohort of mice on a go/no-go vibrissal object detection task (Fig. 7A). Mice learned this task more readily than the discrimination task, reaching above-threshold performance in 3.4 ± 0.5 d (Fig. 7B; n = 10 mice; p < 0.001, t test comparing detection and discrimination cohorts). We observed a small decline in performance on the first day following the lesion, despite lesions taking place within an hour of the start of the post lesion behavioral session (performance before lesion, 87.4 ± 2.9%; performance after legion, 82.5 ± 2.4%; p = 0.035; n = 7 mice; Fig. 7C). Lesion size (0.18 ± 0.06 mm3; n = 7 mice; Fig. 7F,G) did not predict the effect on performance on the first post lesion day (Fig. 7D; R = 0.105, p = 0.843, Pearson correlation) and did not differ from the location discrimination task (p = 0.857, unpaired t test comparing go/no-go detection to location discrimination lesion volumes). Behavior quickly recovered to pre lesion levels (p = 0.165, t test comparing the day before the lesion to the second post lesion day). We confirmed that this was a whisker-dependent behavior by trimming the whisker after 3 post lesion days. Following whisker trimming, mice performed at chance levels (performance before trimming, 88.8 ± 1.6%; performance after trimming, 51.1 ± 1.1%; p < 0.001, n = 7; Fig. 7E).
Tasks with higher cognitive load are believed to be more dependent on cortical activity than simpler tasks (Stüttgen and Schwarz, 2018). Thus, we examined the impact of columnar-scale lesions on a more complex detection behavior. We trained mice to perform a lick-left/lick-right touch detection behavior that required mice to report touch by licking one of two lickports and the lack of touch by licking the other lickport (Fig. 8A). In contrast to the go/no-go behaviors, this task required mice to withhold licking until 0.5 s after the pole became inaccessible, at which point an auditory cue indicated to the animal that it was time to respond. Thus, this task had both two response contingencies and a working memory component. Mice learned this task more slowly than the go/no-go detection task, reaching above-threshold performance in 19.0 ± 2.5 d (Fig. 8B; n = 9 mice; p < 0.001, t test comparing go/no-go and lick-left/lick-right detection cohorts). Despite the greater difficulty in learning this task, mice still showed only a very weakly significant performance decline immediately following a columnar-scale lesion in vS1, which recovered by the next day (performance before lesion, 73.9 ± 2.2%; performance after lesion, 69.8 ± 2.5%; p = 0.039; n = 7; performance on second post lesion day, 77.7 ± 1.4%, p = 0.080, n = 7; Fig. 8C). Lesion size (0.10 ± 0.01 mm3; n = 7 mice; Fig. 8F,G) did not correlate with performance (Fig. 8D; R = 0.193, p = 0.678, Pearson correlation) and did not differ from the location discrimination task (p = 0.090, unpaired t test comparing lick-left/lick-right detection to location discrimination lesion volumes). Performance fell to chance levels following whisker trimming (performance before trimming, 77.2 ± 2.2%; performance after trimming, 52.7 ± 1.7%; p < 0.001; n = 7; Fig. 8E). Thus, vS1 was still not necessary for task performance on a detection task, even after adding a working memory component and a second response contingency.
Discussion
We tested the behavioral role of vS1 columns in a range of vibrissa-dependent tasks and found that lesioning one to two barrels in vS1 degraded performance when the mouse had to report whether it touched a proximal or distal object. Lesions only slightly degraded performance when the mouse had to report whether it touched an object or not. The deficit in the location discrimination task performance did not recover for several days following the lesion. In contrast, detection performance recovered by the second post lesion day, even in a task with a working memory component. Thus, neurons within the principal whisker column in vS1 are necessary for vibrissal object location discrimination, but not for touch detection.
Identifying neurons that underpin perception ultimately requires identifying structures whose removal permanently perturbs behavior. Transient inactivation of a structure can often lead to a behavioral deficit, even if permanent removal of that structure does not influence behavior or produces rapid recovery (Otchy et al., 2015; Hong et al., 2018). Finding structures that are indispensable to specific behaviors thus requires permanent inactivation followed by longitudinal observation (LaMotte and Mountcastle, 1979). Lesions are well suited for this purpose. Our work presents a spatially precise lesioning approach which allows us to measure the behavioral role of small cortical structures and track recovery over time. Our use of this lesioning method to target individual vS1 barrels demonstrates that small cortical volumes play a crucial role in the perception of object location, as the behavioral impact of permanent, columnar-scale lesions persisted for several days (Wolff and Ölveczky, 2018).
Vibrissal S1 has been implicated in many whisker behaviors using a range of inactivation approaches. These approaches, both transient and permanent, often inactivate not only vS1 but also adjacent structures. In the case of transient inactivation, the radius of effect is rarely measured, but when it is, it usually extends to at least 1 mm (Krupa et al., 1999; Long and Fee, 2008; Hong et al., 2018; Li et al., 2019). Spatially extensive inactivation can impact adjacent structures, which can lead to misattribution of the behavioral role. For example, lesions of vS1 that extend to striatum can produce permanent performance degradation in touch detection tasks where lesions confined to vS1 produce behavioral recovery (Hong et al., 2018). Area-scale lesions of vS1 have led to degraded performance in tasks requiring discrimination of aperture size (Krupa et al., 2001), object location (O’Connor et al., 2010), distance (Hutson and Masterton, 1986), and texture (Guic-Robles et al., 1992). In contrast, whisker touch detection tasks show either no sensitivity to lesions (Hutson and Masterton, 1986) or transient sensitivity (Hong et al., 2018), with rapid recovery back to pre lesion performance. Local pharmacological inactivation degrades performance in a single-vibrissa object location discrimination task (O’Connor et al., 2010) as well as tasks that require comparison of vibrissal vibration across two sides of the head or detection of vibration on one side (Miyashita and Feldman, 2013). Optogenetic silencing of vS1 interferes with object location discrimination (Guo et al., 2014b) and object detection (Sachidhanandam et al., 2013; Hong et al., 2018). Spatial dissociation of function in auditory cortex demonstrates that specific regions subserve specific functions (Lomber and Malhotra, 2008). Consequently, whether these tasks require large portions of vS1, surrounding areas that are likely impacted by past inactivation approaches, or just the relevant whisker barrels, remains unclear. Our work shows that relatively small populations of neurons can underpin specific behavioral functions.
Gap crossing with a single whisker, which is impeded following large-scale vS1 lesions (Hutson and Masterton, 1986), is susceptible to microstrokes on the scale of a single barrel (Shih et al., 2013). These strokes are generated by occluding blood vessels either via laser illumination of a sensitizer or via amplified laser pulses. Columnar-scale lesions are comparable in size to microstrokes but are produced without the need for targeting blood vessels or the use of special reagents. Both approaches demonstrate that relatively small (10,000–20,000) populations of neurons can contribute substantially to behavior, raising the intriguing possibility that an even smaller subset of these neurons may act as a bottleneck in the generation of perception. In the case of gap crossing, it is unclear whether performance would have recovered after several days as performance was only evaluated in a single post lesion session (Shih et al., 2013).
Primary sensory cortices are thought to be more important for behaviors involving more complex stimuli and higher cognitive loads. In the auditory cortex, many discrimination behaviors require cortex (Lomber and Malhotra, 2008; Slonina et al., 2022). At the same time, simple auditory features such as frequency can be discriminated even without cortex (Ohl et al., 1999). Lesions to V1 in humans result in “blindsight”—the ability to perform certain vision-dependent tasks despite the lack of conscious visual awareness (Leopold, 2012). Complex visual tasks, however, can no longer be performed. In vS1, performance on tasks where a relatively strong stimulus (e.g., active touch) needs to be detected recovers or does not degrade after inactivation (Hutson and Masterton, 1986; Hong et al., 2018), while performance on object location discrimination (O’Connor et al., 2010) or gap crossing (Hutson and Masterton, 1986) tasks is affected. Tasks with a higher cognitive load are also thought to be more dependent on activity from primary sensory cortices than those with a lower cognitive load (Stüttgen and Schwarz, 2018). We find that individual vS1 barrels are necessary for object location discrimination but are only transiently necessary in both our go/no-go detection task and the more complex lick-left/lick-right delayed response detection task. Thus, stimulus complexity but not cognitive load predicts the importance of individual vS1 barrels.
What is the minimal set of neurons needed for object location discrimination in vS1? In layer (L) 2/3, ∼10% of neurons show robust responses to touch (Crochet et al., 2011; Peron et al., 2015). L4 excitatory neurons produce fewer spikes, on average, than L2/3 neurons, and L5 neurons produce comparable spike counts (Yu et al., 2019). This suggests that a sparse minority of the ∼6000 excitatory neurons in a barrel (Lefort et al., 2009) contribute to the touch response. It is thus possible that several hundred neurons act as a perceptual bottleneck in vS1 for single whisker object location discrimination. In contrast to columnar-scale lesions, however, cellular resolution lesions in vS1 targeting tens of neurons have not produced behavioral effects (Peron et al., 2020). This result is unsurprising given that those animals were performing a detection task, which we have here shown to be insensitive to vS1 lesions. Cellular resolution lesion experiments in mice performing object location discrimination tasks will thus be crucial in determining whether small populations of neurons constitute a perceptual bottleneck in vS1.
In vS1, mice can readily detect the activation of arbitrary groups of tens of neurons (Dalgleish et al., 2020). Even single neurons can drive behavioral report (Houweling and Brecht, 2008); among excitatory neurons in vS1, touch-sensitive pyramidal neurons show the greatest ability to drive a single-neuron perceptual report (Tanke et al., 2018). In mouse primary visual cortex, activating a handful of neurons can perturb discrimination between vertical and horizontal gratings (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Marshel et al., 2019). These experiments do not, however, demonstrate that naturalistic percepts only require tens of neurons. The introduced activity is likely amplified via the strong recurrence present in cortex (Douglas and Martin, 2007; London et al., 2010), enabling perceptual detection. Consistent with this argument, in tongue premotor cortex, activating ∼10 neurons can perturb the chosen licking direction (Daie et al., 2021), but silencing does not exert an effect unless hundreds of thousands of neurons are inactivated (Guo et al., 2014a). Moreover, adding activity to hundreds of L4 vS1 neurons only partially increases the report of touch (O’Connor et al., 2013). Thus, while gain-of-function experiments are crucial to our understanding of the neural basis of perception, loss-of-function experiments are needed to establish that a given population plays a role in a naturalistic behavior. Our experiments establish a ceiling of ∼10,000 excitatory neurons as a perceptual bottleneck for the discrimination of naturalistic touch.
In sum, we present a spatially precise lesioning method that can be used to study the role of cortical areas across a variety of task types. Applying this method, we find that individual vS1 barrel columns contribute to single-whisker vibrissal object location discrimination. At the same time, object detection using active whisking is not dependent on individual barrel columns. Thus, small (∼10,000) populations of primary sensory cortical neurons can contribute to specific behaviors.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgments: We thank Dan Sanes for comments on this manuscript; and Dora Angelaki, Rob Froemke, and Bettina Voelcker for discussion.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by the Whitehall Foundation and the National Institutes of Health (Grant R01-NS-117536).
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.