Abstract
Respiration is strongly linked to internal states such as arousal, emotion, and even cognitive processes and provides objective biological information to estimate these states in humans and animals. However, the measurement of respiration has not been established in macaque monkeys, which have been widely used as model animals for understanding various higher brain functions. In the present study, we developed a method to monitor the respiration of behaving monkeys. We first measured the temperature of their nasal breathing, which changes between inspiration and expiration phases, in an anesthetized condition and estimated the respiration pattern. We compared the estimated pattern with that obtained by a conventional chest band method that has been used in humans and applied to anesthetized, but not behaving, monkeys. These respiration patterns matched well, suggesting that the measurement of nasal air temperature can be used to monitor the respiration of monkeys. Furthermore, we confirmed that the respiration frequency in behaving monkeys monitored by the measurement of nasal air temperature was not affected by the orofacial movement of licking to obtain the liquid reward. We next examined the frequency of respiration when they listened to music or white noise. The respiratory frequency was higher when the monkeys listened to music than the noise. This result is consistent with a phenomenon in humans and indicates the accuracy of our monitoring method. These data suggest that the measurement of nasal air temperature enables us to monitor the respiration of behaving monkeys and thereby estimate their internal states.
Significance Statement
While respiration is linked with internal processing, methods to measure respiration for physiological research on monkeys have not been established. We developed a novel method that obtained respiration signals by measuring the nasal air temperatures of behaving monkeys. Our method was able to continuously track the respiration pattern without distortions evoked by orofacial movements to lick the liquid reward. The respiratory frequency increased while listening to music than when listening to white noise for all the monkeys. These results demonstrate that nasal air temperature measurements can be used to monitor the respiration patterns of aroused monkeys, allowing us to understand their internal state. This will be useful for investigating the underlying neuronal mechanism of neuropsychiatric disorders using monkeys.
Introduction
While humans can verbally explain their internal states such as emotions and feelings, animals cannot. However, it is possible to estimate the internal states of animals by monitoring their behavior and physiological parameters. Since an altered internal state is a remarkable characteristic of psychiatric disorders such as depression and schizophrenia (Boiten et al., 1994), techniques to monitor physiological parameters have made significant contributions to animal model studies on mechanisms underlying these disorders (Lang et al., 2000; Rudebeck et al., 2014). In previous studies using animals, physiological parameters, including heart rate (Maeda et al., 2018; Wascher, 2021), pupil diameter (Suzuki et al., 2016; Maeda et al., 2018; Kuraoka and Nakamura, 2022), and skin temperature (Nakayama et al., 2005; Kuraoka and Nakamura, 2022), were measured to estimate emotion and arousal. Among them, respiration is a well-measured physiological parameter, the pattern of which changes depending on internal states such as anxiety and stress (Suess et al., 1980; Boiten, 1998; Homma and Masaoka, 2008). In contrast to other physiological parameters that are involuntarily regulated, respiration is controlled not only involuntarily but also voluntarily. Thus, this physiological parameter is thought to reflect richer internal information than the other parameters. Consistent with this idea, brain regions that affect respiration are widely distributed over the cortical and subcortical areas. Several regions in the medulla are known to directly control respiration (Del Negro et al., 2018). In addition, respiration is modulated by neural oscillations in the limbic brain regions, such as the amygdala and hippocampus, that are involved in emotion (Frysztak and Neafsey, 1991; Zelano et al., 2016). Human studies have also reported that neural oscillations in the prefrontal cortex, a center of cognitive processes, are synchronized with the rhythm of voluntary respiration (McKay et al., 2003). These data suggest that brain activities regulating not only emotion but also cognitive processes affect respiratory control, and thereby respiration measure is likely to be useful to estimate these internal states.
Behavioral and neuroscience studies on emotion and cognitive processes have used macaque monkeys as experimental animals because of their similarities to humans in behavioral capacities and brain architecture. However, these studies did not monitor their respiration because there is no established method to monitor respiration in behaving monkeys. Body and air movements evoked by respiration (e.g., chest movement) were measured to monitor respiration in humans (Porges and Byrne, 1992) and rodents (Grimaud and Murthy, 2018). The measuring equipment (e.g., chest band) was directly attached to the body. These conventional methods are hard to apply to behaving macaque monkeys because they have dexterous fingers like humans and often remove the measuring equipment. To monitor the respiration of behaving macaque monkeys, the present study developed a method to measure the temperature of their nasal breath (nasal air) that changes between inspiration and expiration phases. The equipment did not touch any part of the body, to ensure that the monkeys were not disturbed, and it was attached to a location where the monkeys could not approach it. We successfully reconstructed the respiration pattern of behaving monkeys from the measured nasal air temperature. This methodology enables us to estimate internal states of behaving macaque monkeys and facilitates research on neuronal mechanisms underlying respiration, motivation, and cognitive processes in primates.
Materials and Methods
Animal preparation
Four female Japanese monkeys (monkeys E, P, M, and Y; four, nine, five, and four years old; 5.0, 7.0, 6.2, and 6.5 kg, respectively) and two male rhesus monkeys (monkeys B and S; 10 and nine years old; 6.2 and 5.9 kg, respectively) were used in the experiment. All the procedures for animal care and experimentation were approved by the University of Tsukuba Animal Experiment Committee and were conducted according to the guidelines described in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals published by the Institute for Laboratory Animal Research. Initially, the monkeys were habituated to sitting on a primate chair. Using sterile procedures and general isoflurane anesthesia, a head holder was implanted, which was embedded in dental acrylic covering the top of the skull and firmly anchored using plastic screws. Analgesics were administered during each surgery and for several days afterward. The experimental sessions began after the monkeys had fully recovered from the surgery.
Respiration and licking behavior monitoring
To monitor the respiration of the monkeys, we measured the temperature of nasal air, which changes between inspiration and expiration phases, using a thermosensor probe (IT-18, Physitemp; time constant, 100 ms; lead length, 91.4 cm; Fig. 1) with a thermometer (Model BAT-12R, Physitemp) not only in the anesthetized condition (medetomidine and midazolam) but also in the awake condition. The thermosensor probe (Φ0.65 mm) was supported with a metal wire (Φ0.70 mm) and these were covered with a polyolefin heat shrink tube (Fig. 1A). The nasal air temperature is maintained uniform by the ambient temperature during the inspiration phase but increases during the expiration phase in which warm air is expired. We also measured changes in the monkeys’ chest diameters using a movement sensor (TN1132/ST, AD Instruments). During the experiment, the monkeys sat in a primate chair in a sound-attenuated room, and their heads were fixed using the head holder. We measured the nasal air temperature with the thermosensor that was located at three different positions: 5 mm outside, 0 mm, and 5 mm inside from the entrance of the nasal cavity. When we measured the temperature in the awake condition, the thermosensor was located inside. Licking movements were measured with a vibration sensor (AE-9922, NF Corporation) that was attached to the spout for reward delivery. These analog data were collected using a data acquisition system (Omniplex, Plexon).
Sound stimulation and reward delivery
In the awake condition, sound stimuli and a liquid reward were presented to the monkeys. These sound stimulation and reward delivery were controlled with a personal computer running the MonkeyLogic MATLAB toolbox (Asaad and Eskandar, 2008). We used three different tempos of music for sound stimulation (“Master of Puppets” by Metallica, 212 beats/min; “Berceuse” by Chopin, 85 beats/min; and “Can’t stop the feeling” by Justin Timberlake, 113 beats/min; 65.0–70.0 dB SPL). The introductions of the music were removed to match their volume levels. We also used three pieces of white noise (40.0 dB SPL) to measure the resting respiration frequency. Each music and white noise were presented for 3 min with a 1-min interval. The recording session was executed once a day and was repeated for 3 d. The music and white noise were presented using a speaker that was located 50 cm away from the monkey’s face. A liquid reward (0.2 ml) was delivered randomly (once in 30 s) during the sound stimulation.
Analysis
The respiration and licking data were analyzed offline using a commercial software (MATLAB 2018b, MathWorks). We calculated the chest diameter by integrating the chest movement signals. To calculate the interpeak interval (IPI) and the inspiration/expiration time ratio, we identified peaks of the respiratory waveform that corresponded to the time when the phase of respiration changed (Fig. 2A). To count the number of licks, we detected the onset of each lick as the time when the licking signal measured by the vibration sensor reached a threshold that was manually adjusted for each day (Fig. 3B).
Results
Accuracy of the respiration signal monitored by nasal air temperature
To develop a noninvasive respiration monitoring system for macaque monkeys, the present study focused on the temperature of nasal air that changes between the inspiration and expiration phases. This thermosensor detected the resistance of the material at its tip that changed with the temperature, with a high temporal resolution (100 ms). Because the thermosensor probe was thin (Φ0.65 mm) and flexible, it was supported with a metal wire (Φ0.70 mm; Fig. 1A) and placed at the entrance of the nasal cavity without being attached to the wall of the nasal cavity (mean ± SD; horizontal, 8.80 ± 1.31 mm; vertical, 5.10 ± 0.89 mm; n = 5; Fig. 1B,C). The monkeys sat in a conventional primate chair, an acrylic box from which only the monkey’s head stuck out, so that they could not approach the thermosensor probe by the hands.
We first applied this system to the three anesthetized monkeys (monkeys P, M, and S) and observed an oscillation of nasal air temperature that coincided with the alternation between inspiration and expiration phases (Fig. 2A, red curve). The peaks of the oscillating temperature signal (Fig. 2A, red triangles) were corresponding to the times when the phase of respiration changed from inspiration to expiration. To examine how accurately the temperature signal reflected the pattern of respiration, we simultaneously monitored the respiration of the anesthetized monkeys using a conventional chest-band method that had also been used in humans and was applicable to anesthetized monkeys (Fig. 2A, blue curve). We observed that the temperature and chest movement signals were well synchronized. We obtained similar ratios for inspiratory to expiratory times (inspiration/expiration time ratio) from the signals in each method (mean ± SD; temperature, 0.88 ± 0.01; chest band, 0.84 ± 0.19; p = 0.11, unpaired t test). However, the temperature signal obtained by the thermosensor was slightly delayed compared with the respiration signal obtained by the chest band (see Fig. 2A, enlarged panel).
The position of the thermosensor in the nasal cavity may have caused this delay. To find the best position, we measured the nasal air temperature at three different positions: −5 mm inside, 0 mm, and 5 mm outside the entrance of the nasal cavity. We then calculated the delay in the temperature signal compared with the respiration signal calculated by the chest band for each position (i.e., the time difference between the peaks of the temperature signal and those of the respiration signal; Fig. 2B). We observed that the delay in the temperature signal decreased when the thermosensor was located inside the nostril. Although a significant decrease in the delay was observed only in one of the three monkeys (regression analysis; monkey M, r = −0.49, p = 0.01; monkey P, r = −0.13, p = 0.51; monkey S, r = −0.28, p = 0.11), this trend was significant as a whole (regression analysis; all monkeys, r = −0.23, p = 0.03). Hence, the thermosensor should be located on the inside rather than the outside or entrance of the nostril to reduce the temperature signal delay.
Although the delay in the temperature signal obtained by the thermosensor compared with the respiration signal calculated by the chest band was small (0–100 ms in the “inside” condition), researchers that need a fine temporal resolution may have to correct this delay. A factor that could influence this delay is the body weight of the monkeys because the volume of airflow evoked by respiration changes depending on the weight. Figure 2C shows the relationship between the temperature signal delay and body weight. We observed that the delay tended to increase as the body weight increased (regression analysis; r = 0.47, p = 2.58 × 10−6). Thus, to analyze respiration with a fine temporal resolution (e.g., phase-triggered average), the delay of the temperature signal obtained by the thermosensor may need to be corrected in accordance with the body weight.
Respiration monitored by nasal air temperature in behaving monkeys
The results indicate that the respiration of anesthetized monkeys can be monitored by measuring the nasal air temperature. We next applied this method to behaving monkeys. The nasal air temperature of the three monkeys was measured while sound stimuli and a liquid reward were presented. Three music and white noise were sequentially presented for 3 min each with a 1-min interval (see Fig. 3A for the schedule).
Similar to the anesthetized condition, we observed an oscillation of nasal air temperature that reflected the alternation between inspiration and expiration phases during the presentation of the music (Fig. 3B, lower curve). We examined whether the temperature signal was affected by orofacial movements to lick the liquid reward. Liquid rewards are commonly used in neuroscience and behavioral studies on macaque monkeys. Therefore, to disseminate our method in these fields, it is useful to show that respiration patterns monitored by our method are not distorted by orofacial movements to lick the liquid. Figure 3B, upper curve, shows a typical example of the licking signal measured by a vibration sensor that was attached to the spout for reward delivery. To quantify the effect of licking on the temperature signal, we aligned the signal at the onset of lick events (shown in Fig. 3B, inverted triangles). We observed that the temperature signal did not change before and after the lick events (Fig. 3C). This result suggests that respiration patterns monitored by nasal air temperature are not distorted by orofacial movements to lick the liquid and that our method can be used in conjunction with liquid rewards.
Rhythmic auditory stimuli changing the frequency of respiration
Previous studies have reported that, in humans, while rhythmic auditory stimuli such as music are presented, the frequency of respiration changes depending on the rhythm (Bernardi et al., 2006; Salimpoor et al., 2009). Here, we tested whether the same phenomenon occurs in monkeys. We monitored the respiration of three monkeys (monkeys M, P, and S) by measuring their nasal air temperature while they listened to the three types of music with different tempos (fast, 212 beats/min; medium, 113 beats/min; and slow, 85 beats/min) as well as white noise, and compared the respiration frequency during the presentation of the rhythmic auditory stimuli (i.e., music) with that of the nonrhythmic auditory stimulus (i.e., white noise). Figure 4A,B shows the interpeak interval of the measured temperature signal, which reflects the respiration frequency, of monkey S in the 1-d experiment. The interpeak interval during music presentation (including the three music type data) was significantly smaller than that during white noise presentation (unpaired t test, p = 2.02 × 10−5; Fig. 4A). However, the interpeak interval was not significantly different among the three types of music (ANOVA, p = 0.32; Fig. 4B).
We repeated the same experiment five times for each monkey and compared the interpeak interval between each music and white noise (Fig. 4C). We observed that many comparisons exhibited a significant difference (paired t test; monkey M, fast music vs noise, p = 0.02, medium music vs noise, p = 0.10, slow music vs noise, p = 1.6 × 10−3; monkey P, fast music vs noise, p = 0.09, medium music vs noise, p = 1.6 × 10−3, slow music vs noise, p = 0.04; monkey S, fast music vs noise, p = 0.06, medium music vs noise, p = 5.9 × 10−3, slow music vs noise, p = 5.0 × 10−3; Fig. 4C). Yet, the interpeak interval was not significantly different among the three music in any of the monkeys (ANOVA, monkey M, p = 0.98; monkey P, p = 0.60; monkey S, p = 0.93). Taken together, these results suggest that the respiration of macaque monkeys is accelerated by rhythmic auditory stimuli but is not influenced by the tempo.
Limitation of conventional chest band method in behaving monkeys
As mentioned above, we succeeded in monitoring the respiration of behaving macaque monkeys by measuring the temperature of their nasal airflow. As far as we know, this is the first report of the respiration of behaving macaque monkeys. Indeed, we failed to monitor their respiration using the conventional chest band method. In this experiment, we used five monkeys (monkeys E, B, M, S, and Y) and they were attached with the chest band. Because the chest band needs to be attached tightly to monitor the chest movement, it may physically and emotionally affect natural respiration. So, we compared the respiration frequency of the five monkeys before and 15 min after the chest band attachment (5 min each). The respiration signal was obtained by the thermosensor which was attached to the monkey just before the experiment. We repeated the same procedure five times (i.e., five sessions) for all five monkeys. Note that whereas the temperature probe was easily attached, we needed to temporarily hold the monkeys’ hands to prevent them from scratching the investigators when attaching the chest band. The results are summarized in Table 1. All five monkeys removed the chest band using their hands either immediately (monkey E) or within 15 min after attaching it (mean ± SD; monkey B, 129.0 ± 63.5 s; monkey M, 658.0 s; monkey S, 690.0 ± 63.6 s; monkey Y, 172.4 ± 93.7 s), whereas none of the monkeys removed the temperature probe. Although two of the monkeys removed the chest band in only a few sessions (monkey M, 1/5; monkey S, 3/5), their respiration frequency was faster 15 min after attachment compared with before attachment (mean ± SD; monkey M, −254.8 ± 135.1 ms; monkey S, −993.0 ± 122.6 ms), which indicates that the chest band disturbed natural respiration. Taken together, because the chest band seemed to cause stress to the monkeys, the condition in which the chest band method can be used is limited in awake monkeys.
Discussion
In this study, we succeeded in monitoring the respiration pattern of behaving macaque monkeys, which reflects their internal states such as emotion and cognition, by measuring the temperature of their nasal airflow. We also show that the conventional method, the chest band, does not work well in behaving monkeys because it caused of the physical stress (Table 1). Our method is simple and noncontact (Fig. 1) but has several features that make it useful in various physiological experiments. First, measuring the nasal air temperature allowed us to obtain accurate respiration patterns that were only slightly delayed from those obtained by the conventional method using the chest band (Fig. 2). Second, our method was able to continuously track the respiration pattern without distortions evoked by orofacial movements to lick the liquid reward (Fig. 3). Third, our method does not disturb the monkey’s natural respiration changes. We succeeded in recording the physical response to the change in respiration frequency because of rhythmic auditory stimulus (Fig. 4). Thus, our respiration monitoring method could be a strong tool for estimating the internal state of behaving macaque monkeys.
Other methods have been used to monitor the respiration pattern of humans and animals (Grimaud and Murthy, 2018). Respiration is led by volume changes in the lungs induced by respiratory muscle activity. Therefore, the electromyography of inspiratory and expiratory muscles can be used to monitor respiration patterns (Murphy et al., 2001). Pleural pressure, which increases during the inspiration phase and decreases during the expiration phase, is also a physiological parameter that reflects the respiration pattern and therefore can be used as its monitor. However, the measurements of respiratory muscle activity and pleural pressure require sophisticated surgical skills to implant electrodes and pressure sensors, respectively. It is difficult to apply these invasive measurements to behaving macaque monkeys because they can detach these probes with their dexterous fingers. In humans, measuring the chest diameter, the carbon dioxide of respiratory airflow, and the breathing flow rate are standard noninvasive methods to monitor respiration patterns (Porges and Byrne, 1992; Wientjes, 1992; Al-Khalidi et al., 2011). These methods provide accurate respiration signals, but the obtained signals may be distorted and may not reflect natural respiration, especially in behaving animals, because the instruments necessary for these methods, such as chest band and plastic facial mask, are directly attached to the body and disturb their behavior.
This study compared the respiratory signals from the chest diameter and nasal air temperature of anesthetized monkeys and revealed that both respiration signals correlated. We also found that the time difference between these signals is related to body weight (Fig. 2). This might be because the volume of airflow evoked by respiration changes depending on weight. However, to determine whether the relationship between the delay and body weight is linear, it is necessary to obtain the data from many other monkeys with a range of different body weights. Our study also had other limitations. First, it is difficult to obtain quick changes in respiration signals through our method because temperature changes are gradual. Furthermore, there is a time lag (∼100 ms) in the inhalation onset detection between the methods presented here (Fig. 2). The main reasons for the delay may be the time constant of the thermosensor and the nasal air temperature affecting signal detection. The time lag is affected by the position of the sensor in the nasal cavity (Fig. 2B). Thus, although measuring the nasal air temperature provides adequate information to monitor the respiration rate and tidal volume, it does not reflect the quick changes in respiration. To examine the rapid response of the internal state, it might be useful to measure the pupil diameter (Suzuki et al., 2016; Kuraoka and Nakamura, 2022) or skin temperature (Nakayama et al., 2005; Kuraoka and Nakamura, 2022) concurrently with respiration. Second, our method uses the head-fix condition, which is commonly used in primate neurophysiology experiments, to enable stable measurements. Although we can obtain respiration signals in a head-free condition if the sensor is attached to the nasal area (e.g., by using surgical tape), when the monkeys move their heads, there may be some signal changes. Hence, measuring the temperature of nasal airflow is suitable for respiratory monitoring in head-fixed aroused monkeys.
Respiration has a strong link with the internal state, such as cognitive and emotional processing. In humans, the respiration frequency changes depending on the tempo of the music regardless of the habituation, i.e., it increases with fast tempo and decreases with slow tempo (Haas et al., 1986; Bernardi et al., 2006; Gomez and Danuser, 2007; Salimpoor et al., 2009; but see Sato et al., 2012). This suggests that faster rhythms may concentrate the attention and pauses or slower rhythms may induce relaxation. Conversely, our study revealed that the respiration rate in monkeys increased with both slow and fast music tempos (Fig. 4C). The arousal in the animals may have occurred because of the musical sound, regardless of the tempo. Alterations in respiration patterns are also found in psychiatric disorders such as anxiety and schizophrenia, neurologic disorders, and aging (Boiten et al., 1994; Caldirola et al., 2004; Ramirez et al., 2013). For example, irregular respiration rates were found in patients with anxiety disorders, particularly when they were angry (Stevenson and Ripley, 1952). Research on the mechanisms of these emotion-respiration and cognitive-respiration relationships is still limited. Tracking the connection between respiration patterns and performance in cognitive tasks and neurophysiological correlates provides an invaluable objective measure of the internal state. Subsequently, our method is a strong and useful tool for investigating the underlying neuronal mechanism of psychiatric disorders using nonhuman primates.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements: We thank H. Yamada and T. Koganezawa for the valuable comments on the study; M. Isoda, A. Uematsu, T. Ninomiya, and A. Noritake for the technical advice; S. Nishino and Y. Narita for the animal care; and Y. Suwa and Y. Yabana for their administrative help. The Japanese monkeys were provided by the National Bio-Resource Project.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported partly by grants from the Japan Science and Technology Agency (JST), PRESTO Grant Number JPMJPR21S4; the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED) Grant Number JP21zf0127005; Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT) Grants 21H05800 and 21H05036; the Takeda Science Foundation; and the Brain Science Foundation.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.