Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Cognition and Behavior

Neural Networks Implicated in Autobiographical Memory Training

Dragoş Cȋrneci, Mihaela Onu, Claudiu C. Papasteri, Dana Georgescu, Cătălina Poalelungi, Alexandra Sofonea, Nicoleta Puşcaşu, Dumitru Tanase, Teofila Rădeanu, Maria-Yaelle Toader, Andreea L. Dogaru, Ioana R. Podină, Alexandru I. Berceanu and Ioana Carcea
eNeuro 21 November 2022, 9 (6) ENEURO.0137-22.2022; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0137-22.2022
Dragoş Cȋrneci
1CINETic Center, University of Theatre and Film “I.L. Caragiale” Bucharest, Bucharest, 020941, Romania
3Medical Imaging Department, Clinical Hospital Prof. Drive Th. Burghele, Bucharest, 061344, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mihaela Onu
1CINETic Center, University of Theatre and Film “I.L. Caragiale” Bucharest, Bucharest, 020941, Romania
3Medical Imaging Department, Clinical Hospital Prof. Drive Th. Burghele, Bucharest, 061344, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Claudiu C. Papasteri
1CINETic Center, University of Theatre and Film “I.L. Caragiale” Bucharest, Bucharest, 020941, Romania
4Department of Psychology, University of Bucharest, Bucharest, 050657, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dana Georgescu
5Provita Medical Center, Bucharest, 011403, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Cătălina Poalelungi
1CINETic Center, University of Theatre and Film “I.L. Caragiale” Bucharest, Bucharest, 020941, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Alexandra Sofonea
1CINETic Center, University of Theatre and Film “I.L. Caragiale” Bucharest, Bucharest, 020941, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nicoleta Puşcaşu
1CINETic Center, University of Theatre and Film “I.L. Caragiale” Bucharest, Bucharest, 020941, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dumitru Tanase
1CINETic Center, University of Theatre and Film “I.L. Caragiale” Bucharest, Bucharest, 020941, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Teofila Rădeanu
3Medical Imaging Department, Clinical Hospital Prof. Drive Th. Burghele, Bucharest, 061344, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Maria-Yaelle Toader
3Medical Imaging Department, Clinical Hospital Prof. Drive Th. Burghele, Bucharest, 061344, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Andreea L. Dogaru
3Medical Imaging Department, Clinical Hospital Prof. Drive Th. Burghele, Bucharest, 061344, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ioana R. Podină
1CINETic Center, University of Theatre and Film “I.L. Caragiale” Bucharest, Bucharest, 020941, Romania
4Department of Psychology, University of Bucharest, Bucharest, 050657, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Alexandru I. Berceanu
1CINETic Center, University of Theatre and Film “I.L. Caragiale” Bucharest, Bucharest, 020941, Romania
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ioana Carcea
1CINETic Center, University of Theatre and Film “I.L. Caragiale” Bucharest, Bucharest, 020941, Romania
6Department of Pharmacology, Physiology and Neuroscience, Rutgers Brain Health Institute, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Newark, NJ, 07103
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Training of autobiographical memory has been proposed as an intervention to improve cognitive function. The neural substrates for such improvements are poorly understood. Several brain areas have been previously linked to autobiographical recollection, including structures in the default mode network (DMN) and the sensorimotor network. Here, we tested the hypothesis that changes in connectivity within different neural networks support distinct aspects of memory improvement in response to training on a group of 59 human subjects. We found that memory training using olfactory cues increases resting-state intranetwork DMN connectivity, and this associates with improved recollection of cue-specific memories. On the contrary, training decreased resting-state connectivity within the sensorimotor network, a decrease that correlated with improved ability for voluntary recall. Moreover, preliminary data indicate that only the decrease in sensorimotor connectivity associated with the training-induced decrease in the tumor necrosis factor α (TNFα) factor, an immune modulation previously linked to improved cognitive performance. We identified functional and biochemical factors that associate with distinct memory processes improved by autobiographical training. Pathways which connect autobiographical memory with both high-level cognition and somatic physiology are discussed.

  • autobiographical memory
  • memory training
  • neural networks

Significance Statement

Our study shows for the first time that autobiographical memory training with olfactory cues changes connectivity in the default and sensorimotor networks to improve cue-evoked recollection and voluntary recall, respectively. Our findings related to the default mode network (DMN) add evidence to the scene construction theory of memory (Hassabis et al., 2007). The novel involvement we described for the sensorimotor network brings support to the embodied memory theory, where recalls rely on sensorimotor simulations of events (Iani, 2019). Together, our findings suggest that multiple mechanisms contribute to memory strengthening during training.

Introduction

Autobiographical memories represent the fabric of our identity. In several neurodegenerative disorders that manifest with memory loss, self-identity dissipates, inducing anxiety, confusion, and impaired social interactions. Training autobiographical recall might be a beneficial behavioral intervention that preserves the most defining memories. Several studies have investigated the effects of autobiographical memory training on emotional well-being (Köhler et al., 2015; Hitchcock et al., 2017; Serrano et al., 2004; Ricarte et al., 2012). However, unlike studies on training of working memory or of spatial memory (De Marco et al., 2016; Ten Brinke et al., 2017), very little is known about how autobiographical training changes the ability to recall autobiographical events and what neurophysiological substrates might be engaged by training.

Previous imaging studies have shown that brain regions activated during autobiographical recall form a network that largely overlaps with the default mode network (DMN), comprised of the medial prefrontal cortex, lateral and medial temporal lobe, precuneus, posterior cingulate cortex, retrosplenial cortex, and temporo-parietal junction (Addis et al., 2004; Buchanan, 2007; Spreng et al., 2009). These structures are also activated by imagining future events, navigation, theory of mind, and other mental processes that require scene construction (Hassabis and Maguire, 2007; Hassabis et al., 2007; Spreng et al., 2009). Scene construction theory views the retrieval of episodic memories, including autobiographical memories, as a reconstructive process (Mullally and Maguire, 2014; Clark et al., 2020). According to this theory, we expect that training autobiographical memory retrieval would lead to changes in the activity and connectivity of DMN structures.

A different theory for autobiographical memory recall (and more broadly for episodic memory) is that of embodied memory, where recalls rely on sensorimotor simulations of events (Iani, 2019). The embodied memory concept proposes that “cognition is strongly influenced by body” and introduced the sensorimotor model, which proposes that both perceptual and motor brain activity patterns are recorded during memory encoding and then reactivated during memory recall (Nyberg et al., 2001; Nyberg, 2002; Glenberg et al., 2013; Iani, 2019). This theory is supported by imaging studies that found activation of sensory and motor areas during episodic memory recall (Nilsson et al., 2000; Nyberg et al., 2001; Masumoto et al., 2006; Mineo et al., 2018). Based on this theory, we predict that autobiographical training changes connectivity within sensorimotor networks.

In addition to changes in neural activity, improvements in memory could also associate with biochemical changes. Autobiographical recalls have been shown to decrease the levels of tumor necrosis factor α (TNFα), of interleukin-2, and of interferon γ (Matsunaga et al., 2011, 2013). The relationship between inflammatory state and neural network activity and connectivity is complex. At baseline, blood levels of cytokines associate with changes in the activity of the DMN, limbic, ventral attention, and corticostriatal networks and with changes in connectivity within the DMN (Marsland et al., 2017; Kraynak et al., 2018). In relation to autobiographical recall, whereas for interferon γ an anticorrelation was found with activation of the orbitofrontal and posterior cingulate cortex (Matsunaga et al., 2013), for TNFα it remains to be determined whether such a functional association exists. Increased levels of TNFα associate with poor cognitive performance and aggravated Alzheimer’s dementia (Hennessy et al., 2017). It is therefore important to determine whether autobiographical training could be beneficial by decreasing cytokine levels.

Our hypothesis is that autobiographical memory training increases efficiency within brain networks involved in memory retrieval. To test this hypothesis, we used olfactory cues to induce recall of autobiographical memories. The choice of olfactory modality was dictated by unpublished data from our lab and by previous findings describing the effectiveness of odors in evoking memory (Matsunaga et al., 2013; Larsson et al., 2014; Herz, 2016). Odor-evoked autobiographical recall is a technique used in theater training for student actors to gain access to personal memories, an exercise inspired by the view on acting and memory of Method Acting (Cohen, 2010; Stanislavsky, 2010). To investigate changes in brain activity following autobiographical training that could explain lasting changes in memory performance, we performed resting-state functional MRI (fMRI) scanning at the beginning and at the end of training. We focused on the connectivity within functional networks. To determine whether autobiographical training can also change the levels of TNFα, we collected blood samples at the beginning and end of training. We then tested a possible association between cytokine levels and brain activity dynamics.

Our findings bring important scientific evidence to the translational use of a technique primarily employed in theatrical training. Our data support the notion that training of autobiographical memories could be used in therapy for the prevention and treatment of memory loss.

Materials and Methods

All methods and experiments have been approved by The Institutional Ethics Committee and followed the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent for their participation.

Subjects

An experimental group of 29 subjects (25 women and four men) with a mean age of 34.6 years and a control group of 30 subjects (24 women and six men) with a mean age of 32.5 years. Subjects were randomly assigned to the two groups. All subjects were volunteers selected from among the students. Subject inclusion criteria. A complete blood count and C-reactive protein (CRP) measurements were used to check for the presence of infection/inflammation. Only subjects without signs of infection or inflammation were included. From the same blood samples collected from them, the TNF-α level in lymphocytes have been measured with a high sensitivity ELISA kit. Exclusion criteria: rhinitis (or other medical problems that lead to impaired smell), depression, anxiety, chronic diseases that cause infection/inflammation, eyeglasses, metal implants, cardiac pacemakers, claustrophobia. The IQS of the subjects were not measured because their quality of being college students excludes a possible mental disability.

Materials

Fifteen odors were used: coffee, vinegar, vanilla, cocoa, wine, onion, fresh apples, cinnamon, orange, sanitary alcohol, paint, tobacco, diesel oil, jasmine fragrance, and chamomile. The odors were selected and adapted from the stimuli used in previous studies (Chu and Downes, 2002; Gardner et al., 2012). The odors were presented individually in small containers with perforated lids. A questionnaire containing three seven-point Linkert scales (1 is weakest, 7 is strongest) was used to measure the quality of retrieved memories: valence, vividness, and personal meaning. Also, a questionnaire containing two, seven-point Likert scales was used for measuring the subjective effects on memory after one month of training. One scale asks to what extent did the subject notice the onset of spontaneous memory during the day (outside of the experiment; where 1 means “none” and 7 means “to a very large extent”). The second scale asks whether the subject noticed a greater ease of voluntarily accessing memories in daily life (where 1 means “none” and 7 means “very easy”).

Procedure

Pre-training session

A venous blood sample was taken in the morning (between 9 and 10 A.M.). The same day, between noon and 2 P.M., subjects were exposed to an odor-triggered retrieval session, and the subjects were video monitored during the procedure (with their consent). The instruction was to recall and enunciate an episode from their lives that the current odor brings to memory. After each event retrieved, subjects completed the questionnaire measuring valance, vividness, and personal relevance. The procedure took ∼30 min. Six to 8 h after this session, all subjects have been scanned using resting-state functional connectivity MRI procedure.

Training session

After the Pre-training session, each subject from the experimental group underwent an autobiographical reminder training for 1 h, two times per week, for four weeks. The experimental group was stimulated to recall autobiographic memories using 15 odors. The participant took each container in his/her hand and smelled its contents through the holes in the lid. He/she waited for a maximum of 20 s to see whether a memory triggered by that smell appears. If a memory appeared, she/he described it in as much detail as possible. If not, he/she moved on to the next container. Subjects were encouraged to detail the memories as much as possible, insisting on the description of sensory, social, and emotional details. After the Pre-training session, each subject from the control group watched two short movies for 45 min, two times per week, for four weeks.

Post-training session

After four weeks, all subjects were exposed to the same procedure used in the Pre-training session. A venous blood sample was taken in the morning (between 9 and 10 A.M.). The same day, between noon and 2 P.M., subjects were exposed to an odor-triggered retrieval session and the subjects were video monitored during the procedure (with their consent). The instruction was to recall and enunciate an episode from their lives that the current odor brings to memory. After each event retrieved, subjects completed the questionnaire measuring valance, vividness, and personal relevance. Additionally, subjects completed the scales for spontaneous and voluntary memory retrieval in daily life, outside the lab. Six to 8 h after this session, all subjects have been scanned using resting-state functional connectivity MRI procedure.

Imaging, acquisition

A 3T Siemens Skyra-MR scanner was used to acquire a resting state functional acquisition with 281 axial volumes, by means of a two-dimensional multislice echo-planar imaging sequence (TR = 2500 ms, TE = 30 ms, FA = 90°, matrix size = 94 × 94, voxel size = 4 × 4 × 4.3 mm, 281 volumes of 40 axial images each). Each functional acquisition duration was 11 min and 42 s. Additionally, anatomic images were acquired (T1-weighted MP-RAGE, TRTR = 2200, TE = 2.51 ms, matrix size = 256 × 256, voxel size 0.9 × 0.9 × 0.9 mm). The first five volumes, acquired to allow the longitudinal magnetization to reach a steady state, were discarded.

Data analysis was performed using FMRIB Software Library (FSL) package (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/). Head motion in the fMRI data was corrected using multiresolution rigid body co-registration of volumes with 12-DOF, as implemented in the MCFLIRT software, brain extraction with BET, spatial smoothing [Gaussian kernel full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) 5 mm] and denoising using nonlinear filtering (SUSAN). For one experimental and one control subjects, the movement was too substantial to be corrected (mean scan-to–to-scan displacement larger than 0.2 mm, maximum displacement larger than 2 mm), and data from these subjects were excluded from the rest of the analysis. Brain image extraction was conducted for motion corrected BOLD volumes with optimization of the deforming smooth surface model, as implemented in the BET software. Rigid body registration as implemented in the FLIRT software was used to co-register fMRI volumes to T1-MPRAGE (brain-extracted) volumes of the corresponding subjects and subsequently, to the MNI152 standard space. The images were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel FWHM of 5 mm. Image denoising was performed using nonlinear filtering (SUSAN), and a temporal high-pass filtering (with a cutoff frequency of 0.01 Hz) was applied,

Three subjects in the control group were excluded from the fMRI analysis: two of them refused to participate to the POST scanning session, and one of them had substantial movement in the scanner that rendered the data unusable.

Imaging, analysis

Independent component analysis (ICA)–the multivariate exploratory linear decomposition into independent components (MELODIC) tool was used to perform spatial group ICA using multisession temporal concatenation. Given previous studies showing that a minimum of 50 ICA components are needed to separate known networks (Szewczyk-Krolikowski et al., 2014), we chose to perform a 50 component ICA. Resting-state networks were identified by evaluating the spatial cross-correlation and overlap between the group ICA maps and the corresponding BOLD ICA template (https://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/datasets/brainmap+rsns/), at a thresholded of z > 3. The similarities between ICA outputs were investigated by using FSL function (fslcc), which allows running cross-correlations between every volume in one 4D dataset with every volume in another. The ICA maps with a correlation coefficient above 0.3, were selected for further analysis (Kiviniemi et al., 2011). ICA maps associated with motion or which were localized primarily in the white matter or ventricles were excluded from further study (Kelly et al., 2010). We also considered ICA prominent low-frequency power of fast Fourier transformation (FFT) spectra and slow fluctuation in time courses. The remaining 15 networks were identified as classical ICA maps as previously reported (Smith et al., 2009; Zuo et al., 2010). These networks are: motor, attention, posterior default mode network (pDMN), higher visual, anterior default mode network (aDMN), fronto-parietal left, primary visual, temporal, fronto-parietal right, executive, somatosensory, basal ganglia, anterior salience, hippocampal, and pontine. The Juelich histologic atlas and Harvard–Oxford cortical and subcortical atlases (Harvard Center or Morphometric Analysis) were used to identify the anatomic location and NeuroSynth 100 top terms atlas (http://neurosynth.org) was used to identify the functional components of the resulting ICA maps.

An intranetwork connectivity analysis was performed. This analysis involves comparing the subject-specific spatial maps between experimental and control conditions. To determine subject-specific spatial maps, dual regression analysis was performed on the obtained neural networks using variance normalization (with variance normalization, the dual regression reflects differences in both activity and spatial spread of the resting-state networks), similar to previous studies (Onu et al., 2015; Emerson et al., 2016). We performed the following statistical analysis for each of the 15 ICA maps. For the paired two-group difference (two-sample paired t test), different component maps were collected across subjects into single 4D files (one per original ICA map) and tested voxelwise by nonparametric permutation using the FSL randomize tool (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/Randomise) with 5000 permutations and a threshold-free cluster enhanced (TFCE) technique to control for multiple comparisons. As we tested a multitude of resting state networks, we addressed the issue of multiple testing corrections by controlling the false discovery rate (FDR) at p < 0.05.

To evaluate the relationship between intranetwork connectivity changes as effect of the training program and cognitive performance, mean z-corrected parameter estimates extracted from the clusters of significant experimental versus control condition differences, which were correlated with behavioral variables. The subject-specific z-corrected parameter estimated spatial maps are the outputs of stage 2 of the dual regression.

The intranetwork connectivity quantified indices (mean z-corrected parameter estimate scores) calculated as a result of these procedures were then analyzed in GraphPad Prism.

Biochemistry

TNFα levels were measured in lymphocytes. Blood samples were collected between 10 A.M. and noon for all subjects. They were obtained by venipuncture using EDTA-coated tubes; 2.5-ml fasting venous blood were used to obtain lymphocytes, which were separated by density gradient centrifugation (Biocoll separating solution, Biochrom GmbH). After separation, the lymphocytes were resuspended in 1-ml RPMI culture media (Biochrom GmbH) and ultrasonicated. The supernatant was then aliquoted and stored at −20°C. Because of technical problems, many of the stored probes were compromised. We were able to use PRE and POST probes from nine subjects that underwent training and from five subjects in the control group. TNFα was measured in these samples using a high sensitivity ELISA kit (IBL International GmbH) with the detection limit of 0.13 pg/ml. The calculated intra-assay coefficient of variation was 8.5% and the interassay coefficient of variation was 9.8%. TNFα concentrations were measured using the Tecan Reader, with Magellan Reader software (Tecan Group, Ltd). For the calculation of results, we used a four-parameter curve.

Testing generalization effect

A second group of 10 subjects (eight women, two men, mean age of 41.6 years) were used to test whether memory improvement after training generalizes to odors other than the ones used in training. In this case, training was done with 10 odors (orange, alcohol, paint, tobacco, gasoline, jasmine, dry basil, lemon, fresh mint, chloride), whereas the PRE and POST testing was done with other eight odors (coffee, vinegar, vanilla, cacao, wine, onion, fresh apples, cinnamon). On this set of subjects we did not perform MRI scanning and we did not collect blood samples.

Statistics

Our study reports descriptive, inferential, and estimation statistics. GraphPad Prism and Estimation Statistics Beta were used. In the case of estimation statistics, 5000 bootstrap samples were taken; the confidence interval is bias-corrected and accelerated.

The p value(s) reported are the likelihood(s) of observing the effect size(s), if the null hypothesis of zero difference is true. For each permutation p value, 5000 reshuffles of the control and test labels were performed (Ho et al., 2019).

Results

To determine how training might improve autobiographical memory, we evaluated several aspects related to autobiographical recollection in 29 subjects that underwent four weeks of training and in 30 control subjects. The autobiographical memories were triggered with olfactory cues (orange, coffee, etc.) that were presented to experimental subjects in small vials, each at a time. At baseline (Experimental Session 1, PRE) and at the end of the experiment (Experimental Session 10, POST), subjects were presented the cues and asked to recollect an episode from their own life (Fig. 1A). In POST, they were asked to score on an analog scale if they observed a change since the start of the experiment in two mnemonic aspects outside of laboratory settings: voluntary recollection and spontaneous recollection. During the eight sessions of training, the experimental subjects came to the lab and underwent a similar procedure, where they were asked to recollect autobiographical memories in response to the olfactory cues presented. Control subjects visited the lab the same amount of time, but instead of autobiographical training, they were asked to watch and score a series of videoclips, a control activity meant to match the level of engagement of the experimental group. PRE and POST intervention, both experimental and control subjects underwent resting-state fMRI imaging for 11.7 min to investigate changes in neural network activity induced by training (Fig. 1A).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Behavioral effects of autobiographical training. A, Diagram of the experimental design. B, Training increases the number of odor-evoked recalls. Black symbols are PRE data and red symbols are POST data. White-filled symbols are control subjects and gray-filled symbols are training subjects. C, Post-training recall improvement generalizes to untrained odors. D, Training improves voluntary recalls in a significant proportion of subjects. *p < 0.05.

At the behavioral level, we observed that autobiographical training but not the control condition had a significant positive effect on the number of odor-evoked recalls (“Control PRE”: 10 ± 0.4 recalls/session, “Control POST”: 10 ± 0.6, Cohen’s d = 0.01 [95% CI: −0.24, 0.29], p = 0.857, N = 30; “Training PRE”: 9.4 ± 0.6 recalls/session, “Training POST”: 11.5 ± 0.4, Cohen’s d = 0.7 [95% CI: 0.15, 1.11], p = 0.001, N = 29; Fig. 1B). Based on these data, we infer that autobiographical training can improve odor-evoked memory (repeated-measure two-way ANOVA shows an effects of interaction F(1,57) = 8.119, p = 0.0061, and Sidak’s multiple comparison test shows significant difference POST vs PRE for the Training group, p = 0.0003 but not for the Control group, p = 0.997; Fig. 1B). To investigate whether the training procedure induces generalized improvement of odor-evoked memory recall, on a separate group of 10 subjects we tested autobiographical memories in response to different odors than the ones included in the training protocol. We found that training strongly increased the number of autobiographical recalls even for odors not practiced during training (PRE: 6.6 ± 0.3, POST: 7.8 ± 0.1, Cohen’s d = 1.47, [95% CI: 0.73, 2.09], p = 0.007, two-sided paired t test, N = 10; Fig. 1C). Training did not affect the vividness, valance and personal relevance of odor-evoked recalls (no interaction effect between experimental condition and PRE versus POST timing for valence, p = 0.732, vividness, p = 0.319, or personal relevance, p = 0.175).

Autobiographical training also improves the ability to recall voluntary memories outside of the laboratory setting. In the control group, only 33.4% of subjects reported an improvement of voluntary memory (score above 4, on a scale from 1 to 7), whereas in the training group 72.4% subjects reported improved voluntary recollection (p = 0.02, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test; Fig. 1D). The score for spontaneous memory was not affected by training (training: 44.8%, subjects reported improved spontaneous memory, control: 50%, p = 0.7).

Intranetwork connectivity: between-group differences

To investigate whether changes in brain connectivity associate with changes in memory observed after autobiographical training, we acquired resting-state BOLD activity in PRE and POST in all experimental and control subjects. In order to investigate stimulus-independent changes in neural function, no odors were presented immediately before or during resting-state scanning. Subjects were instructed to relax with eyes closed for 11.7 min. In our dataset, we identified 15 independent component analysis (ICA) maps that had significant cross-correlation (r > 0.3) with established resting-state neural networks (Kiviniemi et al., 2011). We then determined whether for these 15 networks the strength of intranetwork connectivity, as measured by mean z scores (see Materials and Methods), changes with training. The only significant finding was related to circuitry within the DMN, between the medio-dorsal thalamus, and the anterior parts of DMN (anterior and posterior divisions of the cingulate gyrus, paracingulate gyrus, precuneus, and left angular gyrus; Fig. 2A). In a recent review study, the mediodorsal thalamic nuclei are considered part of the DMN, having a role in mnemonic processes along with other limbic formations, such as the hippocampus (Alves et al., 2019). Unlike the control condition, training had a significant positive effect on DMN-thalamus connectivity in POST compared with PRE (“Control PRE” connectivity: −0.07 ± 0.2, “Control POST” connectivity: −0.44 ± 0.2, paired Cohen’s d = −0.34 [95% CI: −0.92, 0.21], p = 0.154, N = 27; “Training PRE” connectivity: 0.19 ± 0.14, “Training POST” connectivity: 0.55 ± 0.16, paired Cohen’s d = 0.47 [95% CI: −0.03, 1.0], p = 0.048, N = 29; Fig. 2B). Our data indicate that, compared with control, autobiographical training increases recruitment of the right medio-dorsal thalamus into the anterior part of the DMN in POST (repeated-measure two-way ANOVA shows an effect of interaction between time (PRE vs POST) and training F(1,54) = 5.61, p = 0.021; Sidak’s multiple comparison correction shows significant difference between training and control in POST, p = 0.0003, but not in PRE, p = 0.494). The change of DMN-thalamus connectivity (ΔConnectivity = POST-PRE) positively correlates with the change in the number of recalls during autobiographical session (ΔAutobiographical recalls = POST-PRE; r = 0.3, p = 0.015; Fig. 2C), indicating that it could serve as a mechanism for improved odor-evoked autobiographical memory retrieval. The change in DMN-thalamus connectivity did not correlate with the increase in voluntary memory after training (r = 0.05, p = 0.789).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Autobiographical training increases anterior default mode network connectivity. A, Horizontal, coronal, and sagittal sections showing the part of right medio-dorsal thalamus (yellow/red) with increased connectivity with anterior DMN (blue) after training. The standard coordinates of the identified cluster in the medio-dorsal thalamus are: MAX X (vox):20, MAX Y:27, MAX Z: 22, COG X: 19.2, COG Y: 27.2, COG Z: 22. Range for z scores is between 3 and 10, and range for p values is between 0.05 and 0.001. B, Summary data showing increased connectivity between right thalamus and anterior DMN after training (gray-filled symbols) compared with controls (white-filled symbols). C, Positive correlation between change in resting-state connectivity and change in the number of odor-evoked recalls.

Intranetwork connectivity: voxel-wise correlation with voluntary memory score

The changes in anterior DMN did not explain the post-training improvement we observed in voluntary memory (Fig. 1D). To understand what patterns of connectivity might explain the increase in voluntary recall after training, we performed voxel-wise correlations with behavioral scores for all subjects in post-training conditions. Connectivity within the sensorimotor network was the only one significantly correlated with the voluntary memory score after correction for multiple comparisons. More exactly, the connectivity of clusters within the juxtapositional lobule cortex (formerly supplementary motor cortex) were negatively correlated with the voluntary memory scores (Fig. 3A). We extracted these clusters and went back to the individual connectivity maps (for PRE and POST conditions) and calculated subject-wise connectivity strength between the identified clusters in the juxtapositional lobule cortex and the entire sensorimotor network. Using estimation statistics, we found that the strength of connectivity decreased after the training procedure but not after the control intervention (“Control PRE” connectivity: 0.67 ± 0.49, “Control POST” connectivity: 0.70 ± 0.35, paired Cohen’s d = 0.015 [95% CI: −0.53, 0.56], p = 0.957, N = 27; “Training PRE” connectivity: 0.50 ± 0.38, “Training POST” connectivity: −0.45 ± 0.37, paired Cohen’s d = −0.46 [95% CI: −0.82, −0.09], p = 0.013, N = 29; Fig. 3B). Consistent with the group analysis performed for Figure 2A, we did not find a significant interaction between PRE versus POST sessions and training condition (repeated-measure two-way ANOVA shows no effect of interaction F(1,54) = 2.044, p = 0.158), suggesting that in future studies the experiment should be repeated on a larger cohort.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Sensorimotor network connectivity and improved voluntary memory after training. A, Clusters (green) within the sensorimotor network (blue) that negatively correlate with voluntary memory score. B, Subject-specific z score connectivity within the sensorimotor network in the training group and the control group. C, Invers (negative) correlation between sensorimotor connectivity and voluntary memory scores across both training and control groups, r = −0.5, p < 0.0001. Gray symbols represent experimental (training) subjects.

In previous work, it has been shown that exercising sensorimotor behavior leads to decreased BOLD activity in sensory, motor, and premotor structures, which was interpreted as a possible facilitation of neural functions, with more proficient sensorimotor skills associated with lower representation in sensory and motor structures (Kelly and Garavan, 2005). Consistent with these previous studies, we now find that exercising mental sensorimotor simulations of autobiographical memories leads to a decrease in intranetwork connectivity that facilitates voluntary recall, as connectivity in the sensorimotor network negatively correlates with the scores of voluntary memory for both experimental and control groups (r = 0.5, p < 0.0001; Fig. 3C).

Immunologic correlates of autobiographical training

Previous studies documented the role played by certain immunologic factors, primarily TNFα, in memory and other cognitive processes (Besedovsky and del Rey, 2011; Liu et al., 2017; Morimoto and Nakajima, 2019). We investigated whether such biological processes could be implicated in autobiographical training (Fig. 4). Unlike the control condition, we found a significant negative effect of training on the levels of blood TNFα in POST compared with PRE (“Control PRE” TNFα: 328.4 ± 85.3 pg/ml, “Control POST” TNFα: 238.9 ± 54.04 pg/ml, paired Cohen’s d = −0.56 [95% CI: −7.37, 1.32], p = 0.488, N = 5; “Training PRE” TNFα: 355.1 ± 60.54 pg/ml, “Training POST” TNFα: 199.1 ± 43.03 pg/ml, paired Cohen’s d = −0.99 [95% CI: −1.67, −0.34], p = 0.008, N = 9; Fig. 4B). However, we did not find a significant interaction (repeated-measure two-way ANOVA shows no effect of interaction between time and training F(1,12) = 0.339, p = 0.57), suggesting that in future studies the experiment should be repeated on a larger cohort of subjects. We performed a voxel-wise correlation with TNFα values, for subjects in post-training conditions. The TNFα values positively correlated with connectivity for clusters located in the left motor area (Fig. 4A). We extracted these clusters and went back to subject-specific connectivity maps to collect connectivity z scores for both experimental and control groups in PRE and POST conditions. The level of circulating TNFα was positively correlated with this subject-specific connectivity in the sensorimotor network (r = 0.5, p = 0.002; Fig. 4C). These findings could indicate a potential relationship between the decrease in blood TNFα and the neural correlates of improved voluntary recall following autobiographical training, but, given the sample size, these data should be considered with caution.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Correlation between TNFα and connectivity in the sensorimotor network. A, Horizontal, coronal, and sagittal sections showing the part of motor cortex (green) for which the connectivity with the rest of the sensorimotor network (blue) correlates with TNFα levels. B, Summary data showing decreased TNFα levels after training (p = 0.01) but not after control intervention (p = 0.6). C, Positive correlation between resting-state connectivity in the sensorimotor network and TNFα levels. Red symbols, training; gray symbols, control.

Discussion

In this study, we set out to determine whether autobiographical training can improve mnemonic functions by changing connectivity in functional neural networks. We found that autobiographical memory training increased the number of odor-evoked retrieved memories. This confirmed previous studies in older subjects, showing that retrieval practice enhanced successful recall of personal events (Xu et al., 2020). We also found that autobiographical memory training increased the ease of voluntarily accessing memories outside the laboratory, consistent with previous clinical studies in schizophrenic patients (Ricarte et al., 2012).

A priori, we expected to find changes in the DMN network that would support the scene construction theory of memory, or in the sensorimotor network that would support the embodied cognition theory of memory. We found that autobiographical training leads to changes in the connectivity of both of these networks, which argues that both mnemonic theories have physiological relevance. None of the other thirteen neural networks considered showed significant changes with training.

In our study, autobiographical training increases connectivity between the mediodorsal thalamic nuclei and the rest of the anterior DMN and decreases connectivity within the sensorimotor network. Both of these changes could increase recall efficiency. Other studies using different training procedures found similar changes in DMN connectivity. Experienced mindfulness meditators (with >1000 h of training) compared with beginner meditators (one week of training) had increased connectivity between anterior DMN region (dorso-medial prefrontal cortex) and posterior DMN region (inferior parietal lobule), supporting the hypothesis that meditation training leads to functional connectivity changes between these two DMN hubs (Taylor et al., 2013). An extensive study reviewing functional imaging data on practice-related brain changes found that, as performance improves, a “process switch” allows for more efficient processing: as connectivity in specific brain circuits reorganizes, metabolically costly brain activity decreases (Kelly and Garavan, 2005). This phenomenon is reminiscent of the synaptic plasticity of neural circuits described in animal models (Carcea and Froemke, 2013). The increased connectivity between the right mediodorsal thalamus and the anterior DMN found in our study could indicate that training enhances thalamic engagement in the recall process. In animal models, the mediodorsal thalamus has been identified as an important component of memory systems (Hsiao et al., 2020), and its main role could be to amplify and sustain representation in prefrontal structures (Parnaudeau et al., 2017; Schmitt et al., 2017). It is also possible that the increased connectivity that we detect after training reflects a stronger filtering input from prefrontal structures onto the thalamus (Nakajima et al., 2019), a process that would limit the interference of external sensory stimuli on the autobiographic recall.

The training-induced decrease in connectivity that we observe within the sensorimotor network is consistent with the “neural efficiency” hypothesis that posits that the training response reduces activity in sensorimotor areas (Guo et al., 2017). In addition to decreases in activity, previous reports also found decreased connectivity following various types of training (McGregor and Gribble, 2017; Yue et al., 2020). However, to the best of our knowledge, we are the first to report decreased sensorimotor connectivity following autobiographical training. The inverse correlation between sensorimotor connectivity and voluntary memory improvement post-training, supports the notion that this change represents a mechanism for “neural efficiency.”

We showed that training modulates the connectivity in both DMN and the sensorimotor network but in opposite directions. This finding is consistent with a large body of prior literature showing that connectivity in DMN, the task-negative network, inversely correlates with connectivity in task-positive networks, including the sensorimotor network (Blumenfeld, 2016; Cheng et al., 2020).

Our data indicate that the observed changes in network connectivity are not specific to the trained odors, rather they could have a generalized positive effect on recall. Our arguments rely on the fact that we intentionally separated in time the PRE/POST recall testing and the PRE/POST resting-state imaging procedure by 6–8 h. Thus, at the time we acquired the fMRI data, the subjects had not been exposed to any of the trained odors for many hours, and the changes in connectivity that we observe likely represent a state change more than an exercised response to an odor.

During the memory training procedure, subjects were instructed to recall autobiographical events, but we did not check the veracity of their expressed memories. It is possible that some subjects imagined episodes that did not actually happened. However, given previous work showing that performance in autobiographical recollection correlates with imagination (Addis et al., 2010) and given the shared neural substrates for recall and imagination (Hassabis et al., 2007), we predict that similar changes in connectivity of brain networks regardless of how much the odor-evoked recalled episodes overlap with reality.

The exploratory data on the decrease in circulating immune factor TNFα indicate that autobiographical training might exert effects on bodily tissues as well. Immune response can be adjusted by the activity of the sympathetic nervous system and by hormonal activity especially linked to the adrenal gland (Segerstrom and Miller, 2004; Nance and Sanders, 2007; Kenney and Ganta, 2014). Two broad networks in the cerebral cortex have access to the control of adrenal gland function (Dum et al., 2016). The larger network includes all cortical motor areas in the frontal lobe and portions of the somatosensory cortex, indicating that specific circuits exist to connect movement, cognition, and emotions with the function of the adrenal gland. A systematic review of 24 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies investigated brain regions and networks associated with peripheral inflammation in humans and found a so-called “posterior putamen loop,” which comprises also the sensorimotor cortex and is implicated in sensorimotor processes (Kraynak et al., 2018). Taken together, these results indicate possible bidirectional interactions between peripheral inflammatory processes and various cognitive, affective, and sensorimotor contexts.

A recent study suggests that mental health and physical health are linked by neural systems that regulate both somatic physiology and high-level cognition (Koban et al., 2021). The study proposes a “self-in-context” model which hypothesizes that events with personal meaning guide learning from experience and constructs narratives about the self and the environment (autobiographical memories), but at the same time can control peripheral physiology in a predictive way, including autonomic, neuroendocrine, and immune functions (Kleckner et al., 2017). This model is in line with our findings and with previous research which demonstrated that cortical areas involved in the control of movement, cognition, and affect are sources of central commands to influence sympathetic arousal (Dum et al., 2016). This means that cognitive operations like action planning, but also recalling significant actions from past events, may be linked to the regulation of the adrenal function and of the immune system.

In the future, it will be important to determine the mechanism by which autobiographical training could impact the levels of TNFα. Given the correlation between TNFα levels and sensorimotor connectivity, structures within the sensorimotor network could be part of the mechanism for autobiographical immune control. Limitations of this study are represented by the little success in recruiting male subjects into the study, and by the small sample of reliable immunologic data collected from the participants.

Limitations

Each subject likely has certain preferences for the odors used during testing and training. We have not tested how this preference might affect the behavioral and neural outcomes of training. However, this should be addressed in future studies. The number of subjects for which we were able to analyze blood samples in very small. Therefore, the TNFα results should be considered exploratory and confirmation from future studies is needed. Also, we collected blood samples in the morning and the resting-state brain imaging in the evening, a temporal separation that could obscure further correlations between TNFα concentrations and brain activity.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgments: We thank Prof. Nicolae Mandea, Prof. Liviu Lucaci, and Prof. Radu Apostol for their administrative support, Dr. Robert C. Froemke and Dr. Justin S. Riceberg for consultation, and Doina Strat for her technical support.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • The project “Developing a methodology of therapy through theatre with an effect at the neurochemical and neurocognitive levels” is co-financed by the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) through Competitiveness Operational Programme 2014-2020, SMIS code 106688, and implemented by University of Theater and Film “I.L. Caragiale,” CINETic Centre. Additionally, this work was also supported by the European Economic Area (EEA)/Norway Grant EEA-RO-NO-2018-0606.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Addis DR, Moscovitch M, Crawley AP, McAndrews MP (2004) Recollective qualities modulate hippocampal activation during autobiographical memory retrieval. Hippocampus 14:752–762. doi:10.1002/hipo.10215
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Addis DR, Musicaro R, Pan L, Schacter DL (2010) Episodic simulation of past and future events in older adults: evidence from an experimental recombination task. Psychol Aging 25:369–376. doi:10.1037/a0017280 pmid:20545421
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Alves PN, Foulon C, Karolis V, Bzdok D, Margulies DS, Volle E, de Schotten MT (2019) An improved neuroanatomical model of the default-mode network reconciles previous neuroimaging and neuropathological findings. Commun Biol 2:370. doi:10.1038/s42003-019-0611-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Besedovsky HO, del Rey A (2011) Central and peripheral cytokines mediate immune-brain connectivity. Neurochem Res 36:1–6. doi:10.1007/s11064-010-0252-x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Blumenfeld H (2016) Neuroanatomical basis of consciousness. In: The neurology of consciousness, Ed 2. San Diego: Academic Press.
  6. ↵
    Buchanan TW (2007) Retrieval of emotional memories. Psychol Bull 133:761–779. pmid:17723029
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Carcea I, Froemke RC (2013) Cortical plasticity, excitatory-inhibitory balance, and sensory perception. Prog Brain Res 207:65–90.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Cheng X, Yuan Y, Wang Y, Wang R (2020) Neural antagonistic mechanism between default-mode and task-positive networks. Neurocomputing 417:74–85. doi:10.1016/j.neucom.2020.07.079
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  9. ↵
    Chu S, Downes JJ (2002) Proust nose best: odors are better cues of autobiographical memory. Mem Cognit 30:511–518.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    Clark IA, Monk AM, Maguire EA (2020) Characterizing strategy use during the performance of hippocampal-dependent tasks. Front Psychol 11:2119. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02119 pmid:32982868
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Cohen L (2010) The Lee Strasberg notes, pp 14–20. New York: Routledge.
  12. ↵
    De Marco M, Meneghello F, Duzzi D, Rigon J, Pilosio C, Venneri A (2016) Cognitive stimulation of the default-mode network modulates functional connectivity in healthy aging. Brain Res Bull 121:26–41.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Dum RP, Levinthal DJ, Strick PL (2016) Motor, cognitive, and affective areas of the cerebral cortex influence the adrenal medulla. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113:9922–9927.
    OpenUrl
  14. ↵
    Emerson RW, Gao W, Lin W (2016) Longitudinal study of the emerging functional connectivity asymmetry of primary language regions during infancy. J Neurosci 36:10883–10892. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3980-15.2016
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    Gardner RS, Vogel AT, Mainetti M, Ascoli GA (2012) Quantitative measurements of autobiographical memory content. PLoS One 7:e44809. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0044809
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Glenberg AM, Witt JK, Metcalfe J (2013) From the revolution to embodiment: 25 years of cognitive psychology. Perspect Psychol Sci 8:573–585. doi:10.1177/1745691613498098
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Guo Z, Li A, Yu L (2017) “Neural efficiency” of athletes’ brain during visuo-spatial task: an fMRI study on table tennis players. Front Behav Neurosci 11:72. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00072
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Hassabis D, Maguire EA (2007) Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends Cogn Sci 11:299–306.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Maguire EA (2007) Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. J Neurosci 27:14365–14374. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4549-07.2007
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    Hennessy E, Gormley S, Lopez-Rodriguez AB, Murray C, Cunningham C (2017) Systemic TNF-α produces acute cognitive dysfunction and exaggerated sickness behavior when superimposed upon progressive neurodegeration. Brain Behav Immun 59:233–244.
    OpenUrl
  21. ↵
    Herz RS (2016) The role of odor-evoked memory in psychological and physiological health. Brain Sci 6:22. doi:10.3390/brainsci6030022
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. ↵
    Hitchcock C, Werner-Seidler A, Blackwell SE, Dalgleish T (2017) Autobiographical episodic memory-based training for the treatment of mood, anxiety and stress-related disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 52:92–107.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. ↵
    Ho J, Tumkaya T, Aryal S, Choi H, Claridge-Chang A (2019) Moving beyond P values: data analysis with estimation graphics. Nat Methods 16:565–566. doi:10.1038/s41592-019-0470-3 pmid:31217592
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Hsiao K, Noble C, Pitman W, Yadav N, Kumar S, Keel GR, Terceros A, Kanke M, Conniff T, Cheleuitte-Nieves C, Tolwani R, Sethupathy P, Rajasethupathy P (2020) A thalamic orphan receptor drives variability in short-term memory. Cell 183:522–536.e19. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2020.09.011
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    Iani F (2019) Embodied memories: reviewing the role of the body in memory processes. Psychon Bull Rev 26:1747–1766.
    OpenUrl
  26. ↵
    Kelly AMC, Garavan H (2005) Human functional neuroimaging of brain changes associated with practice. Cereb Cortex 15:1089–1102.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Kelly RE Jr., Alexopoulos GS, Wang Z, Gunning FM, Murphy CF, Morimoto SS, Kanellopoulos D, Jia Z, Lim KO, Hoptman MJ (2010) Visual inspection of independent components: defining a procedure for artifact removal from fMRI data. J Neurosci Methods 189:233–245. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.03.028
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Kenney MJ, Ganta CK (2014) Autonomic nervous system and immune system interactions. Compr Physiol 4:1177–1200.
    OpenUrl
  29. ↵
    Kiviniemi V, Vire T, Remes J, Elseoud AA, Stark T, Tervonen O, Nikkinen J (2011) A sliding time-window ICA reveals spatial variability of the default mode network in time. Brain Connect 1:339–347.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Kleckner IR, Zhang J, Touroutoglou A, Chanes L, Xia C, Simmons WK, Quigley KS, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF (2017) Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nat Hum Behav 1:0069. doi:10.1038/s41562-017-0069
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  31. ↵
    Koban L, Gianaros PJ, Kober H, Wager TD (2021) The self in context: brain systems linking mental and physical health. Nat Rev Neurosci 22:309–322. doi:10.1038/s41583-021-00446-8
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  32. ↵
    Köhler CA, Carvalho AF, Alves GS, McIntyre RS, Hyphantis TN, Cammarota M (2015) Autobiographical memory disturbances in depression: a novel therapeutic target? Neural Plast 2015:759139. doi:10.1155/2015/759139 pmid:26380121
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Kraynak TE, Marsland AL, Wager TD, Gianaros PJ (2018) Functional neuroanatomy of peripheral inflammatory physiology: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 94:76–92. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.07.013
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  34. ↵
    Larsson M, Willander J, Karlsson K, Arshamian A (2014) Olfactory LOVER: behavioral and neural correlates of autobiographical odor memory. Front Psychol 5:312. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00312
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  35. ↵
    Liu Y, Zhou LJ, Wang J, Li D, Ren WJ, Peng J, Wei X, Xu T, Xin WJ, Pang RP, Li YY, Qin ZH, Murugan M, Mattson MP, Wu LJ, Liu XG (2017) TNF-α differentially regulates synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus and spinal cord by microglia-dependent mechanisms after peripheral nerve injury. J Neurosci 37:871–881. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2235-16.2016
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    Marsland AL, Kuan DC-H, Sheu LK, Krajina K, Kraynak KE, Manuck SB, Gianaros PJ (2017) Systemic inflammation and resting state connectivity of the default mode network. Brain Behav Immun 62:162–170.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Masumoto K, Yamaguchi M, Sutani K, Tsuneto S, Fujita A, Tonoike M (2006) Reactivation of physical motor information in the memory of action events. Brain Res 1101:102–109. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.05.033
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Matsunaga M, Isowa T, Yamakawa K, Kawanishi Y, Tsuboi H, Kaneko H, Ohira H (2011) Psychological and physiological responses to odor-evoked autobiographic memory. Neuro Endocrinol Lett 32:774–780.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  39. ↵
    Matsunaga M, Bai Y, Yamakawa K, Toyama A, Kashiwagi M, Fukuda K, Oshida A, Sanada K, Fukuyama S, Shinoda J, Yamada J, Sadato N, Ohira H (2013) Brain–immune interaction accompanying odor-evoked autobiographic memory. PLoS One 8:e72523. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0072523
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Mineo L, Concerto C, Patel D, Mayorga T, Chusid E, Infortuna C, Aguglia E, Sarraf Y, Battaglia F (2018) Modulation of sensorimotor circuits during retrieval of negative autobiographical memories: exploring the impact of personality dimensions. Neuropsychologia 110:190–196. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.04.016
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  41. ↵
    McGregor HR, Gribble PL (2017) Functional connectivity between somatosensory and motor brain areas predicts individual differences in motor learning by observing. J Neurophysiol 118:1235–1243.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Morimoto K, Nakajima K (2019) Role of the immune system in the development of the central nervous system. Front Neurosci 13:916. doi:10.3389/fnins.2019.00916
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  43. ↵
    Mullally SL, Maguire EA (2014) Memory, imagination, and predicting the future: a common brain mechanism? Neuroscientist 20:220–234. doi:10.1177/1073858413495091
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Nakajima M, Schmitt LI, Halassa MM (2019) Prefrontal cortex regulates sensory filtering through a basal ganglia-to-thalamus pathway. Neuron 103:445–458.e10. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.026
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Nance DM, Sanders VM (2007) Autonomic innervation and regulation of the immune system (1987-2007). Brain Behav Immun 21:736–745.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Nilsson LG, Nyberg L, Klingberg T, Åberg C, Persson J, Roland PE (2000) Activity in motor areas while remembering action events. Neuroreport 11:2199–2201.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  47. ↵
    Nyberg L (2002) Levels of processing: a view from functional brain imaging. Memory 10:345–348. doi:10.1080/09658210244000171
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Nyberg L, Petersson KM, Nilsson LG, Sandblom J, Åberg C, Ingvar M (2001) Reactivation of motor brain areas during explicit memory for actions. Neuroimage 14:521–528. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0801
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    Onu M, Badea L, Roceanu A, Tivarus M, Bajenaru O (2015) Increased connectivity between sensorimotor and attentional areas in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroradiology 57:957–968. doi:10.1007/s00234-015-1556-y
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  50. ↵
    Parnaudeau S, Bolkan SS, Kellendonk C (2017) The mediodorsal thalamus: an essential partner of the prefrontal cortex for cognition. Biol Psychiatry 83:648–656.
    OpenUrl
  51. ↵
    Ricarte JJ, Hernandez-Viadel JV, Latorre JM, Ros L (2012) Effects of event-specific memory training on autobiographical memory retrieval and depressive symptoms in schizophrenic patients. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 43 [Suppl 1]:S12–S20.
    OpenUrl
  52. ↵
    Schmitt LI, Wimmer RD, Nakajima N, Happ M, Mofakham S, Halassa MM (2017) Thalamic amplification of cortical connectivity sustains attentional control. Nature 545:219–223. doi:10.1038/nature22073
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Serrano JP, Latorre JM, Gatz M, Montanes J (2004) Life review therapy using autobiographical retrieval practice for older adults with depressive symptomatology. Psychol Aging 19:270–277. pmid:15222820
    OpenUrlPubMed
  54. ↵
    Segerstrom SC, Miller GE (2004) Psychological stress and the human immune system: a meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychol Bull 130:601–630. pmid:15250815
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, Filippini N, Watkins KE, Toro R, Laird AR, Beckmann CF (2009) Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:13040–13045.
    OpenUrl
  56. ↵
    Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim ASN (2009) The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci 21:489–510.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Stanislavsky KS (2010) An actor’s work on a role (translated and edited by Jean Benedetti). New York: Routledge.
  58. ↵
    Szewczyk-Krolikowski K, Menke RAL, Rolinski M, Duff E, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Filippini N, Zamboni G, Hu MTM, Mackay CE (2014) Functional connectivity in the basal ganglia network differentiates PD patients from controls. Neurology 83:208–214. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000000592 pmid:24920856
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Taylor VA, Daneault V, Grant J, Scavone G, Breton E, Roffe-Vidal S, Courtemanche J, Lavarenne AS, Marrelec G, Benali H, Beauregard M (2013) Impact of meditation training on the default mode network during a restful state. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8:4–14. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr087
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    Ten Brinke LF, Davis JC, Barha CK, Liu-Ambrose T (2017) Effects of computerized cognitive training on neuroimaging outcomes in older adults: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr 17:139. doi:10.1186/s12877-017-0529-x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  61. ↵
    Xu Q, Zhang J, Grandjean J, Tan C, Subbaraju V, Li L, Lee KJ, Hsieh PJ, Lim JH (2020) Neural correlates of retrieval-based enhancement of autobiographical memory in older adults. Sci Rep 10:1447. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-58076-6
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  62. ↵
    Yue C, Zhang Y, Jian M, Herold F, Yu Q, Mueller P, Lin J, Wang G, Tao Y, Zhang Z, Zou L (2020) Differential effects of tai chi chuan (motor-cognitive training) and walking on brain networks: a resting-state fMRI study in Chinese women aged 60. Healthcare 8:67. doi:10.3390/healthcare8010067
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  63. ↵
    Zuo XN, Kelly C, Adelstein JS, Klein DF, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2010) Reliable intrinsic connectivity networks: test-retest evaluation using ICA and dual regression approach. Neuroimage 49:2163–2177. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.080
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Ifat Levy, Yale University School of Medicine

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Chang-Hao Kao.

The authors have done a good job addressing reviewers comments, and the mansucript is much improved,

Please address the last remaining comment:

In the control condition, there are 30 subjects in the behavior analysis but only 27 subjects in the fMRI analysis. It would be good to describe why 3 subjects were not included in the fMRI analysis.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 9 (6)
eNeuro
Vol. 9, Issue 6
November/December 2022
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Neural Networks Implicated in Autobiographical Memory Training
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Neural Networks Implicated in Autobiographical Memory Training
Dragoş Cȋrneci, Mihaela Onu, Claudiu C. Papasteri, Dana Georgescu, Cătălina Poalelungi, Alexandra Sofonea, Nicoleta Puşcaşu, Dumitru Tanase, Teofila Rădeanu, Maria-Yaelle Toader, Andreea L. Dogaru, Ioana R. Podină, Alexandru I. Berceanu, Ioana Carcea
eNeuro 21 November 2022, 9 (6) ENEURO.0137-22.2022; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0137-22.2022

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Neural Networks Implicated in Autobiographical Memory Training
Dragoş Cȋrneci, Mihaela Onu, Claudiu C. Papasteri, Dana Georgescu, Cătălina Poalelungi, Alexandra Sofonea, Nicoleta Puşcaşu, Dumitru Tanase, Teofila Rădeanu, Maria-Yaelle Toader, Andreea L. Dogaru, Ioana R. Podină, Alexandru I. Berceanu, Ioana Carcea
eNeuro 21 November 2022, 9 (6) ENEURO.0137-22.2022; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0137-22.2022
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • autobiographical memory
  • memory training
  • neural networks

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Opponent Learning with Different Representations in the Cortico-Basal Ganglia Circuits
  • Cardiac and Gastric Interoceptive Awareness Have Distinct Neural Substrates
  • Nonspiking Interneurons in the Drosophila Antennal Lobe Exhibit Spatially Restricted Activity
Show more Research Article: New Research

Cognition and Behavior

  • Hippocampal neuronal activity preceding stimulus predicts later memory success
  • Absence of VGLUT3 expression leads to impaired fear memory in mice
  • Gender impacts the relationship between mood disorder symptoms and effortful avoidance performance
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.