Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Sensory and Motor Systems

Effects of Noise Exposure and Aging on Behavioral Tone Detection in Quiet and Noise by Mice

Kali Burke, Laurel A. Screven, Anastasiya Kobrina, Payton E. Charlton, Katrina Schrode, Dillan F. Villavisanis, Micheal L. Dent and Amanda M. Lauer
eNeuro 25 May 2022, 9 (3) ENEURO.0391-21.2022; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0391-21.2022
Kali Burke
1Department of Psychology, University at Buffalo, The State University of New York, Buffalo, NY 14260
2Department of Otolaryngology–Head and Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Kali Burke
Laurel A. Screven
1Department of Psychology, University at Buffalo, The State University of New York, Buffalo, NY 14260
2Department of Otolaryngology–Head and Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anastasiya Kobrina
1Department of Psychology, University at Buffalo, The State University of New York, Buffalo, NY 14260
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Payton E. Charlton
1Department of Psychology, University at Buffalo, The State University of New York, Buffalo, NY 14260
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Katrina Schrode
2Department of Otolaryngology–Head and Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Katrina Schrode
Dillan F. Villavisanis
2Department of Otolaryngology–Head and Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Dillan F. Villavisanis
Micheal L. Dent
1Department of Psychology, University at Buffalo, The State University of New York, Buffalo, NY 14260
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Amanda M. Lauer
2Department of Otolaryngology–Head and Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205
3Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Visual Abstract

Figure
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint

Abstract

Aging leads to degeneration of the peripheral and central auditory systems, hearing loss, and difficulty understanding sounds in noise. Aging is also associated with changes in susceptibility to or recovery from damaging noise exposures, although the effects of the interaction between acute noise exposure and age on the perception of sounds are not well studied. We tested these effects in the CBA/CaJ mouse model of age-related hearing loss using operant conditioning procedures before and after noise exposure and longitudinally measured changes in their sensitivity for detecting tones in quiet or noise backgrounds. Cochleae from a subset of the behaviorally tested mice were immunolabeled to examine organ of Corti damage relative to what is expected based on aging alone. Mice tested in both quiet and noise background conditions experienced worse behavioral sensitivity immediately after noise exposure, but mice exposed at older ages generally showed greater threshold shifts and reduced recovery over time. Surprisingly, day-to-day stability in thresholds was markedly higher for mice detecting signals in the presence of a noise masker compared with detection in quiet conditions. Cochlear analysis revealed decreases in the total number of outer hair cells (OHCs) and the number of ribbons per inner cell in high-frequency regions in aged, noise-exposed mice relative to aging alone. Our findings build on previous work showing interactions between age and noise exposure and add that background noise can increase the stability of behavioral hearing sensitivity after noise damage.

  • age-related hearing loss
  • cochlear pathology
  • hearing in noise
  • noise-induced hearing loss
  • operant conditioning
  • threshold instability

Significance Statement

Hearing in noisy environments is diminished after exposure to noise and with age. Age also increases the susceptibility of the auditory system to dysfunction and may increase susceptibility to or recovery from damaging noise exposures. We investigated behavioral detection of pure tones in quiet and noise by aged, noise-exposed mice. Our results demonstrate increased susceptibility to temporary threshold shifts and diminished long-term recovery after noise exposure with increased age. Surprisingly, we observed increased performance instability when listening in quiet compared with noisy backgrounds in aged, noise-exposed mice. Presumably, this result reflects the activation of top-down mechanisms (e.g., the auditory efferent system) that enhance hearing in noise.

Introduction

Age-related hearing loss is a major public health issue that is thought to be due to the interaction between natural aging processes, genetic susceptibility, diseases, and the accumulation of ototoxic and environmental exposures, although the effects of interactions between these various factors on age-related perceptual deficits have not been well characterized (Salvi et al., 2001). A history of noise exposure is associated with more severe cochlear pathology and poorer hearing outcomes in humans (PZ Wu et al., 2020, 2021; Gopinath et al., 2021). However, studies in humans report difficulty determining the overall effects of noise exposure with age because of several confounding factors related to the environment and differences in genetic susceptibility (Toppila et al., 2009; Sliwinska-Kowalska et al., 2006).

Susceptibility to damage from loud noise also depends on age in mouse models (Henry, 1982; Li, 1992; Li and Borg, 1993; Ohlemiller et al., 2000; Ho et al., 2014) and is increased in strains with genetic mutations associated with early-onset age-related hearing loss (Erway et al., 1996; Holme and Steel, 2004). Exposure to noise early in adulthood results in temporary threshold shifts that accelerate the progression of damage to the auditory periphery with age (Kujawa and Liberman, 2006; Fernandez et al., 2020). Yet, perception of tones in noise is not necessarily impaired by conditions that selectively damage the auditory nerve (Henry and Abrams, 2021; Resnik and Polley, 2021; Henry, 2022). Young-adult brains are adept at increasing central gain to restore sensitivity to sounds after noise-induced peripheral damage (Salvi et al., 2003; Gold and Bajo, 2014; Chambers et al., 2016; Clarkson et al., 2016; Zhao et al., 2016; Schrode et al., 2018), whereas compensatory central gain is impaired in aged animals exposed to noise, resulting in diminished subcortical responses to sound onsets (McFadden and Willott, 1994; Möhrle et al., 2016; Knipper et al., 2020).

Evoked potential measurements are typically used to quantify hearing loss in animal models; however, hearing loss is more typically assessed with behavioral measurements in humans. Hearing measured behaviorally is typically 5–30 dB more sensitive than what is measured physiologically (Dent et al., 2018; Burke et al., 2021), which may be due to anesthesia, strain, age, sex, and stimulus characteristics. Recent experiments have shown that changes in auditory brainstem response thresholds and waveform morphology precede behavioral hearing loss onset but correspond to substantial outer hair cell (OHC) and auditory nerve synapse degeneration in the CBA/CaJ mouse model (Henry, 2004; Kobrina et al., 2020). Furthermore, the long-term stability of hearing loss is rarely assessed behaviorally in animal models, but models of early-onset and late-onset age-related hearing loss suggest differential effects on the stability of hearing in quiet and noise over time (Prosen et al., 2003; Kobrina et al., 2020). The long-term effects of aging and noise interactions on auditory function have not been thoroughly studied in mammalian models (for review, see Lauer et al., 2019).

To investigate long-term behavioral recovery and stability after noise exposure in aging CBA/CaJ mice, we tracked behavioral detection of pure tones in quiet and masked environments after acute noise exposure in middle-aged and old-aged adult mice. We chose exposure conditions that were previously used to induce cochlear synaptopathy in CBA/CaJ mice with the intention of inducing increased synaptopathy beyond what occurs with natural aging and because this exposure is thought to primarily affect hearing in noise (Kujawa and Liberman, 2015). We hypothesized that the age of the mouse would affect the susceptibility to noise-induced hearing deficits, with middle-aged mice having fewer deficits than old-aged mice. We also hypothesized that hearing deficits would be worse in noise than in quiet after noise exposure. Cochlear specimens were examined for patterns of damage following the behavioral experiments. We show that detection of tones in quiet and noise is generally worse immediately after noise exposure, but middle-aged mice tend to recover over time more than old-aged mice. We also show that day-to-day variability is lower for tone detection in noise compared with quiet. Cochlear synaptopathy was greater than expected based on aging alone, but only in the high-frequency cochlear regions. Hair cell loss also occurred in the OHCs in high-frequency regions.

Materials and Methods

Experimental animals

Twenty-eight adult CBA/CaJ laboratory mice (Mus musculus, IMSR catalog #JAX:000654, RRID: IMSR_JAX:000654, obtained from: https://www.jax.org/strain/000654) ages 414–844 d old (d.o. hereafter) at noise exposure were used in this experiment. Mice were matched by sex (13 male and 15 female) to avoid sex bias (Lauer and Schrode, 2017; Villavisanis et al., 2018, 2020). Most mice entered the experiment after participating in pure tone behavioral discrimination studies in our laboratory. Some young mice entered the experiment at postnatal day (P)60 and did not participate in any other experiments. Training of all mice began when mice were approximately two to three months old and the experiments lasted up to 1000 d.o. (see Results). The original breeding pairs were acquired from The Jackson Laboratory. Our mice were bred in-house at the University at Buffalo, the State University of New York (SUNY), and all procedures were approved by the University at Buffalo (SUNY) Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. All animal experimentation was conducted in accordance with the Society for Neuroscience’s Policies on the Use of Animals and Humans in Neuroscience Research. All mice were singly housed and kept on a reverse 12/12 h light/dark cycle (with lights off at 6 A.M. and on at 6 P.M.). Mice were tested during the dark portion of this cycle for 1 h, at the same time of day, 6–7 d a week. Mice were water restricted and kept at ∼90% of their free feeding/drinking weights throughout the duration of the experiment. To maintain weight and performance, old mice (over 900 d.o.) were allowed free access to a water bottle. All mice had unrestricted access to food, except while participating in the experiment.

Behavioral apparatus

Mice were tested in one of six identically constructed testing booths. The mice were placed into a wire cage (23 × 39 × 15.5 cm) within a sound-attenuated chamber (53.5 × 54.5 × 57 cm) lined with 4-cm-thick Sonex sound attenuating foam (Illbruck Inc.). The chamber was illuminated at all times by a small lamp (Hampton Bay, SKU 208826) with an 8-W light bulb, and the behavior of the mice during test sessions was monitored by an overhead web camera (Logitech QuickCam Pro, Model 4000). The test cage consisted of an electrostatic speaker [Tucker-Davis Technologies (TDT), Model ES1], a response dipper (Med Associates Model ENV-302 M-UP), and two nose poke holes surrounded by infrared sensors (Med Associates Model ENV-254). The experiments were controlled by Dell Optiplex 580 computers operating TDT modules and software. Stimuli were sent through an RP2 signal processor, a PA5 programmable attenuator, an ED1 electrostatic speaker driver, and finally to the speaker. Inputs to and outputs from the testing cages were controlled via RP2 and RX6 processors. Power supplies were used to drive the dippers (Elenco Precision, Model XP-650). Custom MATLAB and TDT RPvds software programs were used to control the hardware. For masked noise experiments, a dome tweeter was placed next to the ES1 speaker for noise presentation (Model FT28D, Fostex, Madisound). The continuous white noise was generated using a noise generator (Model 3025, ACO Pacific Inc.).

Stimuli

The stimuli for this experiment were 14- and 20-kHz pure tones (500 ms in duration with 5-ms rise/fall cosine ramps). These pure tone stimuli were selected because they surround the peak sensitivity of hearing for this strain of mice (16 kHz; reported by Kobrina and Dent, 2016). The pure tone stimuli were created and edited in Adobe Audition (v.5; RRID:SCR_015796). Sound pressure levels for the 14-kHz pure tone were calibrated using a sound level meter (Larson Davis, System 824) with the microphone placed at the approximate location where the mouse’s head would be during testing. Calibrations were conducted weekly. Sound pressure levels for the 20-kHz pure tone were calibrated using an ultrasound recording system (Avisoft, Model USG 116-200) and a custom MATLAB script, again with the microphone (Avisoft Bioacoustics, Ultra Sound Gate CM116) placed at the approximate location where the mouse’s head would be during testing, and were conducted weekly.

The white noise masker was generated using an ACO Pacific Extremely Random white noise generator. This generator produces white noise that recycles every 2 d, making the repetition of a cycle of noise far enough apart to avoid mice potentially memorizing the noise content. The frequency range of this masker falls between 1.6 Hz and 39 kHz, completely overlapping with both tone frequencies tested here. The 1/3 octave band level surrounding 14 kHz was 40.8 dB and surrounding 20 kHz was 34.4 dB. The masker was presented at an overall intensity of 60 dB SPL and was calibrated weekly with the Larson Davis (System 824) sound level meter.

Behavioral procedure

Mice were trained using a Go/No-go operant conditioning procedure on a detection task. When the mouse initiated a trial by poking to the observation nose-poke hole, a variable waiting interval ranging from 1 to 4 s began. Following the waiting interval, a single test stimulus was presented. Using signal detection theory, a hit was recorded if the mouse poked in the report nose-poke hole within 2 s of the onset of the target and received a reinforcement of 0.01 ml of milk chocolate Ensure. If the mouse failed to report the presence of the sound when one was presented, a miss was recorded and the trial ended. To confirm that mice were not randomly receiving hits by chance, sham trials were presented 30% of the time to monitor whether mice remained under stimulus control. In this case, the mouse initiated a trial at the observation hole, but no stimulus was presented. If the mouse continued to remain in the observation hole, this was marked by a correct rejection and the next trial would commence. If the mouse moved to the report hole when no sound was presented, this was marked as a false alarm, and it was punished with a short blackout period, during which mice were unable to initiate a trial (1–6 s). Sessions in which mice did not maintain both a mean hit rate above 80% across all targets and a false alarm rate below 20% were excluded from the data analysis.

Mice typically completed between 50 and 400 trials per testing session, and shorter sessions were included in the analysis so long as hit and false alarm rates were both at criteria. A single pure tone frequency, presented in quiet or in a white noise background was selected for each mouse, and they were only tested on this stimulus for the entirety of the experiment. Throughout each session, only stimulus level varied from trial to trial. Within a session, all of the stimuli were presented to the mice in randomized blocks of 10 (seven targets of varying intensities and three shams per block). All stimuli were presented to mice according to the Method of Constant Stimuli. Once performance stabilized for very loud sounds, two or three out of the seven targets were attenuated in steps of 5–10 dB depending on the age and performance of the mouse, such that mice consistently missed one of the quietest two targets over 50% of the time and the second or third target was over 50% correct. Four hundred trials were then collected and pre-exposure thresholds were calculated from the final 200 trials. Once a stable threshold was calculated for that tone frequency, mice were exposed to the acute noise described below. Data collection continued for at least 45 d, until mice recovered to baseline or stabilized, after which point the mice were anesthetized with a 200 mg/kg ketamine and 10 mg/kg xylazine mixture diluted in sterile water with a volume corresponding to their body weight (e.g., a 20-g mouse received 0.2 ml of anesthetic solution) and perfused via the heart with 60 ml of either 4% paraformaldehyde or a 2% glutaraldehyde 2% paraformaldehyde mixture. Cochleae were re-perfused via the oval and round windows with these fixative agents and tissue was sent on for further anatomic analyses to measure the number of inner hair cells (IHCs) and OHCs, as well as the number of ribbons per IHC and OHC.

Noise exposure

After stable baseline pre-exposure thresholds were reached across 5 d as described above, mice were placed into a standard plastic mouse cage (30 × 19 × 13 cm) lined on the bottom with lab-grade shavings. All other enrichment was removed from the inside of the cage and the food was removed from the cage top. The mouse cage was then placed inside of a custom-built soundproof chamber (53.5 × 54.5 × 57 cm) lined with 4-cm-thick Sonex sound attenuating foam (Illbruck Inc.). A Fostex dome tweeter speaker (FT28D), mounted atop a chicken wire bridge (0.5 × 0.5-inch squares) was suspended over the center of the mouse cage in the booth. The Fostex speaker was wired to an Amplifier (Crown D-75A), which was hooked up to a Dell Optiplex computer (Model 580). The noise for the exposure was a narrowband white noise segment with a frequency band of 8–16 kHz, similar to what has been used in other studies (Kujawa and Liberman, 2015). The sound file was 5 min in duration and was presented on a continuous loop for the duration of the noise exposure (2 h). Mice were allowed to freely roam their cage throughout the exposure. Calibrations were conducted using a Larson Davis sound level meter (System 824) to ensure the noise was 100 dBA (fast scale) in all four corners and in the center of the cage. Calibrations took place before every noise exposure.

Inner ear immunohistochemistry

Mouse cochleae were fixed for 1 h and decalcified for at least 14 d in 1% EDTA. The organ of Corti was dissected into five to six flat turns according to methods from the Eaton Peabody Laboratories. Cochlear pieces were blocked for 1 h in the following solution to inhibit nonspecific binding: 5% normal goat serum, 10% bovine serum albumin, 0.5% Triton X-100 (Electron Microscopy Services), in PBS using routine procedures previously used by our laboratory (Schrode et al., 2018; Kobrina et al., 2020). The blocking buffer was removed and replaced with diluted primary antibody mixture for 24 h at 4°C with mouse mono-clonal anti-CTBP2 to label ribbons (1:200, BD Biosciences, catalog #612044, RRID:AB_399431), rabbit polyclonal anti-myo6 to label hair cells (1:500, Sigma-Aldrich, catalog #M5187, RRID:AB_260563). Cochlear pieces were rinsed 24 h later and incubated in secondary antibodies in blocking buffer for 2 h at room temperature, followed by an additional rinse. The secondary antibodies were Alexa Fluor 568 goat anti-rabbit (1:1000, Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #A-11036, RRID:AB_10563566) and Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-mouse (1:1000, Thermo Fisher Scientific catalog #A-10667, RRID:AB_2534057). The cochlear pieces were then mounted in Fluoromount-G (Southern Biotech) on subbed slides with coverslips.

The number of ribbons and hair cells were quantified at nine frequency regions (4, 5.6, 8, 11.2, 16, 22.6, 32, 45.2, and 64 kHz) along the cochlea in 100-μm sections. Locations were identified using low magnification ∼5× photographs and the ImageJ (RRID:SCR_003070) plugin Measure_Line, developed by the Eaton Peabody Laboratories. It should be noted that this reflects the cochlear place-frequency map in normal, young adult mice, and the noise exposure is likely to have shifted the place-frequency map (Müller et al., 2005). We collected z-stacks at 63× magnification using one of two confocal microscopes depending on availability (LSM 700 Axio Imager 2; Carl Zeiss or LSM 710 Zeiss Axio Observer, Carl Zeiss). Maximum intensity projections were created from confocal z-stacks at a step size of 0.25 μm to capture the entire thickness of each hair cell and its associated labeling. In Fiji (ImageJ) software (RRID: SCR_002285), we counted the number of hair cells and associated afferent ribbons within a 100-μm window corresponding to each frequency-specific point. Cells and their ribbons were only included if the entire cell was visible in the image frame. Total number of CTBP2-positive ribbons were counted and divided by the total number of hair cells within each frequency.

Data analysis

Behavior

Data for the detection of tones in quiet and in the presence of a masker were analyzed separately for each tone frequency. Given the longitudinal design of this study, as well as the repeated sampling for each mouse and several missing data points, a linear mixed-effects model was used to analyze the results. Data were pooled across the sexes due to the small sample size which would not allow us to draw valid conclusions about sex differences and to improve statistical power.

Thresholds (dB SPL) were used as the dependent variable to examine each tone frequency (14-20 kHz) and masker status (masked or quiet) separately. Each model was compared across middle-aged and old-aged mice, with middle-aged mice ranging from 365 to 730 d.o. and old-aged mice ranging from 731 to 1095 d.o. These age groups were selected based on previous work showing that with advanced age (e.g., over ∼700 d.o. in mice), hearing starts to decline rapidly (Kobrina and Dent, 2020; Kobrina et al., 2020). We then used the model to compare thresholds across our age groups for each stimulus over time. Time was binned into time periods for the analyses in six windows to minimize the number of critical comparisons and to avoid an inflation of Type I error by comparing every single day. Any days before the noise exposure were classified as baseline, then the results were classified as postexposure periods of 1–5, 6–10, 11–20, 21–40, and >40 d. Whether thresholds in our age groups differed across the time bins was assessed using the following model: lmer(threshold (dB SPL) ∼ age group × time period+ (1|Id)) (lmer in the lme4 R package, RRID: SCR_015654). We validated the use of our model by testing against an intercept-only model which uses mouse identity as a predictor. We then proceeded to test the goodness of fit of our model against other models using the step-up method of linear mixed-effects modeling. The goodness of fit for our model was measured using Akaike information criterion (AICc) with a criterion specifically used to evaluate goodness of fit for data with small sample sizes. The thresholds for quiet and masked tone detection were evaluated in separate models for 14 and 20 kHz. For the model predicting thresholds for 20 kHz in quiet, the factor of age group was excluded from the model as it did not improve the goodness of fit. Post hoc comparisons using Tukey’s method were performed to assess the relationships between age group and time period, with the p values adjusted to the number of family-based comparisons to reduce Type I error (corrected p = 0.0014 for 14 kHz, quiet; 14 kHz, masked; and 20 kHz, masked; corrected p = 0.003 for 20 kHz, quiet; emmeans R package, RRID: SCR_018734).

Additionally, daily thresholds for each mouse were used to calculate a mean deviation variability index for each mouse after noise exposure (which is preferable to reporting the standard deviation when data are not normally distributed). Using the interquartile range (IQR), outliers were identified as values outside the range Q1–1.5*IQR to Q3+1.5*IQR using boxplot.stats in base package R, which removed two mice. Comparisons before and after noise exposure were not conducted because of the extremely limited number of samples taken before noise exposure. In Kobrina et al. (2020), behavioral day-to-day detection performance stability in quiet and noise was assessed across the lifespan and revealed that performance stability generally varied between 0.1 and 10 dB across conditions, ages, and subjects, but that performance was generally more stable with age. Performance stability was not significantly different between quiet and noise background conditions. The Kobrina et al. (2020) study was performed in unexposed aging mice under similar conditions to this experiment and thus we did not expect differences across groups at baseline in performance stability. An ANOVA was conducted to examine how the day-to-day variability after noise exposure was affected by the interaction between masker status and stimulus. Linear regression analyses were also conducted to explore whether the age of the mouse could predict changes in day-to-day variability across conditions [similar to what is shown by Kobrina et al. (2020) without noise exposure].

Cochlear anatomy

To determine the effect of noise exposure on cochlear anatomy, we used linear mixed-effects models (lmer in the lme4 R package, RRID: SCR_015654) to assess the effects of age and frequency on CTBP2-positive ribbon counts per IHC and OHC and number of IHCs and OHCs per 100 μm. Frequency, age, and exposure group were all treated as categorical factors. Subjects in the exposed (N = 5) and unexposed groups (N = 10: N = 3 current study, N = 7; Kobrina et al., 2020) were age-matched and thus age did not improve the model fitness and results were averaged across age. We controlled for repeated measures dependencies in our data by including a random intercept for mouse identity. We present the results for the ANOVA based on each model, as well as post hoc tests controlling for multiple comparisons using the mvt adjustment (emmeans R package, RRID: SCR_018734). Tukey’s post hoc analyses were performed to assess significance (emmeans R package, RRID: SCR_018734).

Results

Behavior, quiet

14 kHz in quiet

Individual absolute thresholds (dB SPL) and threshold shifts (dB) for middle-aged and old-aged mice over time are shown in Figure 1 (see Table 1 for statistical table). The statistical model for the detection of 14 kHz in quiet revealed a significant main effect of age group (F = 10.42, p = 0.0098), time period (F = 16.61, p = 3.16 × 10−15), and interaction between age group and time period (F = 7.70, p = 5.39 × 10−7 (for results, see Fig 2A)). Post hoc comparisons across age groups reveal old-aged mice had significantly higher thresholds than middle-aged mice 1–5 d after noise exposure (p = 0.0004). Within age groups, middle-aged mice had significantly lower thresholds at baseline than at all other time points (p < 0.0001). Old-aged mice had significantly lower thresholds at baseline than at 1–5 d (p < 0.0001), and significantly higher thresholds at 1–5 d than at 11–20 d (p < 0.0001), 21–40 d (p < 0.0001), and over 40 d (p = 0.0002). These results reveal that for 14-kHz tones in quiet, detection thresholds for older mice were more affected by noise exposure than middle-aged mice (significant difference only at 1–5 d across age groups and a larger increase in threshold from baseline in old-aged mice than middle-aged mice), but old-aged mice recovered rapidly from that increase while middle-aged mice did not.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Statistical table for all behavioral modeling including F values, degrees of freedom (numerator, denominator), p values, and effect sizes (ηp2) for quiet (left) and masked (right) conditions separately for 14 kHz (top) and 20 kHz (bottom)

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Thresholds (dB SPL) for middle-aged mice (A) and old-aged mice (B), and threshold shifts (dB) for middle-aged mice (C) and old-aged mice (D), trained to detect 14-kHz tones in quiet for all test days after exposure. Each plot contains the first two letters of the subject identifier, sex, and age at exposure in d.o. with black symbols representing middle-aged mice and red symbols representing old-aged mice. The dashed vertical line at day 0 represents noise exposure day. The dashed horizontal line in C and D represents pre-exposure thresholds at 0 dB for each mouse.

20 kHz in quiet

Individual absolute thresholds (dB SPL) and threshold shifts (dB) for middle-aged and old-aged mice over time are shown in Figure 3. The statistical model for the detection of 20-kHz tones in quiet revealed a significant main effect of time period (F = 11.75, p = 3.80 × 10−10) since the model with the best fit only included time period as a factor (for results, see Fig. 2B). Post hoc comparisons revealed that thresholds at baseline were significantly lower than at 1–5 d (p < 0.0001), 21–40 d (p = 0.002), and over 40 d (p = 0.0005). Thresholds were significantly higher at days 1–5 than at 6–10 (p = 0.0004), 11–20 (p < 0.0001), 21–40 (p = 0.0013), and over 40 d (p = 0.0012). No other comparisons were significantly different. These findings reveal that the noise exposure led to a temporary increase in thresholds which improved marginally but did not return to baseline sensitivity, regardless of the age of the subject.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Predicted mean thresholds for tone detection in quiet from the mixed-effects model for 14 (A) and 20 (B) kHz across each time period (corrected p = 0.0014). Middle-aged mice are represented by black circles and within-group significant comparisons are designated with black lines and *. Old-aged mice are represented by red squares and within-group significant comparisons are designated with red lines and *. The blue * in A represents significant between age groups comparisons. Error bars represent SEM.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Thresholds (dB SPL) for middle-aged mice (A) and old-aged mice (B), and threshold shifts (dB) for middle-aged mice (C) and old-aged mice (D), trained to detect 20-kHz tones in quiet for all test days after exposure. Each plot contains the first two letters of the subject identifier, sex, and age at exposure in d.o. with black symbols representing middle-aged mice and red symbols representing old-aged mice. The dashed vertical line at day 0 represents noise exposure day. The dashed horizontal line in C and D represents pre-exposure thresholds at 0 dB for each mouse.

Behavior, masked

14 kHz in noise

The masked thresholds (dB SPL) and threshold shifts (dB) for middle-aged and old-aged mice over time are represented in Figure 4. The statistical model for the detection of a 14-kHz tone in a noise masker revealed a significant main effect of time period (F = 21.85, p < 2.2 × 10−16) and interaction between age group and time period (F = 3.56, p = 0.004; Fig. 5A). Across the age groups, there were no significant differences within each time period (p > 0.0014). In middle-aged mice, baseline thresholds were significantly lower than at 1–5 d (p = 0.001), 6–10 d (p < 0.001), and over 40 d (p = 0.001). Thresholds were significantly higher at 6–10 d than at 11–20 d (p = 0.0001), and 21–40 d (p < 0.0001). For old-aged mice, baseline thresholds were significantly lower than all other time periods (p < 0.0001), and thresholds were significantly higher at 1–5 d than over 40 d (p = 0.0001). No other contrasts were significantly different. These differences reflect that while thresholds did not return to baseline (evidenced by significant differences between baseline and all other time periods in the old-aged group and baseline and several intermediate and late time points in the middle-aged group), there was some recovery from the initial threshold shifts in both groups (evidenced by the significant decrease in thresholds between 6–10 and over 40 d in the old-aged group and the lack of a significant difference between baseline and 11–20 and 21–40 d in the middle-aged group).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Thresholds (dB SPL) for middle-aged mice (A) and old-aged mice (B), and threshold shifts (dB) for middle-aged mice (C) and old-aged mice (D), trained to detect 14-kHz tones in a masker for all test days after exposure. Each plot contains the first two letters of the subject identifier, sex, and age at exposure in d.o. with black symbols representing middle-aged mice and red symbols representing old-aged mice. The dashed vertical line at day 0 represents noise exposure day. The dashed horizontal line in C and D represents pre-exposure thresholds at 0 dB for each mouse.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Predicted mean thresholds for tone detection in noise from the mixed-effects model for 14 (A) and 20 (B) kHz across each time period (adjusted p = 0.0014). Middle-aged mice are represented by black circles and within-group significant comparisons are designated with black lines and *. Old-aged mice are represented by red squares and within-group significant comparisons are designated with red lines and *. Error bars represent SEM.

20 kHz in noise

The masked thresholds (dB SPL) and threshold shifts (dB) for middle-aged and old-aged mice over time are represented in Figure 6. The statistical model for the detection of 20 kHz in a masker revealed a significant main effect of time period (F = 10.01, p = 5.26 × 10−9), and a significant interaction between age group and time period (F = 9.77, p = 8.69 × 10−9; for model findings, see Fig. 5B). Post hoc comparisons were conducted comparing middle-aged and old-aged mice at each of the time periods. No significant differences emerged when comparing across the age groups for any time period (p > 0.0014). Within middle-aged mice, thresholds from 1 to 5 d were significantly higher than those over 40 d (p < 0.0001), and thresholds were significantly higher at 21–40 d than over 40 d (p < 0.0001). This result is striking because middle-aged mice were generally improving in thresholds over time while not experiencing much change in sensitivity after noise exposure. In contrast, old-aged mice had significantly lower thresholds at baseline than at all other time periods (p < 0.001) suggesting the noise exposure had a greater effect in this group. No other comparisons were significant. These results suggest that for detecting 20-kHz tones in a noise masker, old- but not middle-aged mice had mildly disrupted detection after noise exposure. The lack of a significant main effect of age group here suggests that although our old group was affected by the noise exposure and our middle-aged group was not, the effects of the noise exposure were minor in the old subjects for the detection of 20 kHz in a masker.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Thresholds (dB SPL) for middle-aged mice (A) and old-aged mice (B), and threshold shifts (dB) for middle-aged mice (C) and old-aged mice (D), trained to detect 20-kHz tones in a masker for all test days after exposure. Each plot contains the first two letters of the subject identifier, sex, and age at exposure in d.o. with black symbols representing middle-aged mice and red symbols representing old-aged mice. The dashed vertical line at day 0 represents noise exposure day. The dashed horizontal line in C and D represents pre-exposure thresholds at 0 dB for each mouse.

Behavior, threshold variability

An ANOVA comparing the interaction between masker status (quiet or noise) and frequency (14 or 20 kHz) revealed a significant main effect of masker status (F = 15.35, p = 0.00,074). A post hoc Tukey test revealed that there was significantly more variability when mice were detecting signals in quiet than in noise (p = 0.0007; Fig. 7; Table 2). Linear regression analyses examining the relationship between these factors and the age of the mouse did not reveal significant results, reflecting similar effects of noise exposure on threshold stability across the age ranges tested (see Table 2). The significant results of the threshold variability analyses indicate the background noise masker had a stabilizing effect on sound detection in noise-exposed mice.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2

Statistical table for all day-to-day variability measurements, including an ANOVA analysis examining the interaction between masker status and stimulus, a post hoc Tukey’s test, and linear regressions examining whether the age of the mouse (in d.o.) could predict changes in day-to-day variability

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Mean deviation scores after exposure for masked and quiet listening conditions (*p < 0.05). Box height represents the 75th percentile (Q3), the horizontal line within box represents the median, the bottom of the box represents the 25th percentile (Q1) and the error bars represent represent the maximum observation that falls within the upper limit (Q3+1.5(Q3−Q1)) and the minimum observation that falls within the lower limit (Q1−1.5 (Q3−Q1)).

Cochlear immunohistochemistry

We compared cochlear degeneration patterns to determine whether noise exposure further damaged the cochlea beyond what occurs with normal aging. Data from aged, unexposed mice contained a merged dataset of new subjects (N = 3) and our previously published data (Kobrina et al., 2020) in aged, unexposed subjects processed under similar conditions (N = 7) to increase power. All data from unexposed mice analyzed in the current experiment (N = 10) were from subjects tested under the same conditions, and all tissues were processed by the same experimenter using the same procedures and reagents.

On the full dataset (N = 15) of unexposed (N = 10) and exposed mice (N = 5), models were constructed to evaluate each dependent variable (number of IHCs per 100 μm, number of OHCs per 100 μm, number of ribbons per IHC, and number of ribbons per OHC) separately, as they are predicted by the frequency (4, 5, 8, 11, 16, 22, 32, 45, and 64 kHz) and noise-exposure status (exposed or unexposed), and taking into account the individual mouse ID and comparing to intercept-only models for significance and goodness of fit. For sample cochlear immunostained tissue, see the Figure 8A, top right corner. Age was included in the test models but the mean age in the exposed and unexposed groups did not differ (unexposed M = 775.5, range: 639–889 d.o., exposed M = 615, range: 424–844 d.o.). Therefore, models including age as a factor did not have the highest goodness of fit for the statistical model.

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

A, Predicted mean number of ribbons per hair HC across frequencies for unexposed (orange) and exposed (blue) mice. Error bars are standard error and *p < 0.0056. The top right corner includes sample cochlear immunohistochemistry with Myosin 6 (red) to visualize the hair cell and CTBP2 (green) to visualize ribbons. B, Predicted mean number of hair cells per 100 μm at each frequency for IHCs (closed symbols) and OHCs (open symbols) in unexposed (orange) and exposed mice (blue). Error bars are standard error and *p < 0.0056.

Number of ribbons per hair cell

The linear mixed-effects model comparing the number of ribbons per IHC revealed a significant main effect of frequency (F = 3.81, p = 0.0006) and a significant interaction between frequency and exposure group (F = 3.67, p = 0.0009; for summary of anatomic statistical tests, see Table 3). Post hoc analyses revealed that noise-exposed mice had significantly fewer ribbons per IHC than unexposed mice at 32 (t = −3.77, p = 0.0003) and 45 kHz (t = −2.8 90, p = 0.0048; see Fig. 8A). No other comparisons at specific frequency regions differed significantly between exposed and unexposed mice (p > 0.0056). The model comparing number of ribbons per OHC revealed a significant interaction between frequency and exposure group (F = 2.31, p = 0.028); however, none of the post hoc analyses were statistically significant (p > 0.0056). Given the small number and limited range of ribbons per OHC (approximately one to two), the biological significance of the interaction may be negligible.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3

Statistical table for all anatomic tests including F values, degrees of freedom (numerator, denominator), p values, and effect sizes (ηp2) for number of IHCs (top), number of OHCs (second down), number of puncta per IHC (third down), and number of puncta per OHC (bottom)

Total number of hair cells per 100 μm

The linear mixed-effects model comparing the total number of IHCs per 100 μm revealed a statistically significant interaction between frequency and exposure group (F = 3.14, p = 0.003). Post hoc analyses revealed that mice in the noise-exposed group had significantly more IHCs at 64 kHz than unexposed mice (t = 3.71, p = 0.0003; see Fig. 8B). No other comparisons at specific frequencies between exposed and unexposed mice differed significantly (p > 0.0056). The statistical model comparing the total number of OHCs per 100 μm revealed a statistically significant interaction between frequency and exposure group (F = 7.72, p = 7.0 × 10−8). Post hoc analyses revealed that noise-exposed mice had significantly more OHCs at 4 kHz than unexposed mice (t = 3.09, p = 0.0026) and significantly more OHCs at 32 kHz in unexposed than noise exposed mice (t = −4.03, p = 0.0001). No other comparisons at specific frequencies between exposed and unexposed mice differed significantly (p > 0.0056).

Discussion

Our data show that noise exposures used to induce auditory nerve synaptopathy in young adult mice lead to permanent changes in behavioral hearing sensitivity for pure tones in old-aged mice, and milder changes in middle-aged mice when listening in quiet and noise. Although the temporary hearing loss was overt and apparent in both quiet and masked conditions, behavioral hearing sensitivity was highly variable between mice and recovered to varying degrees over time. Unexpectedly, detection performance was more stable across time when listening in the presence of a noise masker compared with quiet listening conditions. Overall, greater high-frequency IHC ribbon loss and OHC loss was observed in exposed mice compared with degeneration associated with age alone, but this damage occurred in frequency regions that were outside the regions corresponding to the tone stimuli used in the behavioral detection experiments.

Hearing sensitivity in quiet

The behavioral experiments revealed interesting interactions between age group and time period after noise exposure. Similar noise exposure parameters have previously been shown to induce temporary auditory brainstem response threshold shifts and permanent loss of auditory nerve synapses in young adult mice without permanent threshold shifts (for review, see Kujawa and Liberman, 2015). We observed increased behavioral thresholds after noise exposure that generally returned to baseline or near-baseline sensitivity over time in middle-aged mice. In contrast, performance typically did not fully return to baseline in old-aged mice even if sensitivity improved relative to the initial shift. Similar effects have been shown previously in aged rats that sustained permanent auditory brainstem response thresholds shifts after noise exposure that only resulted in temporary threshold shifts in younger rats (Wang et al., 2011). These results suggest that age hinders recovery of hearing sensitivity after damaging noise exposure.

Hearing sensitivity in noise

For detection of tones in noise, there were significant differences across age and time period after exposure. Detection of 14-kHz tones in noise temporarily worsened after noise exposure in most of the mice tested, whereas performance worsened in old-aged but not middle-aged mice tested on the 20 kHz in noise condition. A previous behavioral study in NMRI mice showed that noise maskers were less effective in aged animals compared with young ones, and a similar effect may be at play in the present study (Ehret, 1979). Cortical reorganization may have occurred in these mice, although it begs the question of why it did not happen in other testing conditions or to the other mice tested in this condition. Regardless, 14-kHz tone detection was severely affected by noise exposure across the lifespan, whereas middle-aged mice experienced less severe deficits than old-aged mice for 20 kHz.

Performance stability

Mice showed high variability in day-to-day hearing sensitivity after noise exposure when tested in quiet. Animals’ performance was more stable in the presence of a noise masker than in the quiet condition, which was an unexpected result (Fig. 7). We are unaware of any other noise exposure studies examining the relationship between threshold stability after noise exposure, so we can only speculate about why this effect was observed. One possible explanation for our finding is that increased hearing stability emerged because of stochastic resonance, where the presence of a fluctuating noise increases detection performance by stabilizing internal noise (Chatterjee and Robert, 2001; McDonnell and Abbott, 2009). The so-called “noise benefit” observed in our behavioral detection data may also occur as central processes that are otherwise disengaged in quiet become engaged in the presence of a noise masker. This could reflect activation of medial olivocochlear pathways that enhance the neural representation of tones in the presence of background noise, although the perceptual hearing in noise benefits of olivocochlear activation remain uncertain (Lauer et al., 2021). Noise exposure induces plasticity in dopaminergic lateral olivocochlear neurons, and this may have a stabilizing effect on the representation of tones in noise in the auditory nerve (JS Wu et al., 2020). The noise background could also have activated medial olivocochlear neurons which may become hyperactive in the damaged auditory system (Knudson et al., 2014).

We did not observe the instability of detection in noise that reportedly occurs before overt threshold shifts in C57BL/6 mice with hereditary, early-onset hearing loss (Prosen et al., 2003), possibly because of the abrupt nature of noise-induced hearing damage. Intriguingly, the C56BL/6 strain shows weak medial olivocochlear activity compared with CBA mice even at young ages, although both strains exhibit medial olivocochlear degeneration with age (Frisina et al., 2007; Sinclair et al., 2017; Vicencio-Jimenez et al., 2021).

In humans, cognitive processes such as attention are critical for detecting a stimulus in masked environments (Strait and Kraus, 2011), thus the decreased day-to-day variability of detection performance in noise might reflect activation of attentional pathways by noise. This phenomenon has been observed in other species. High levels of anthropogenic masking noises that create difficult listening conditions lead to increased vigilance in birds (Evans et al., 2018). It is important to note that we avoided the use of louder background noise levels because, in our experience, mice are less inclined to engage in the operant task when ambient noise levels are too high. Relatedly, background noise can facilitate the acoustic startle response in mice (Ison, 2001; McGuire et al., 2015; Kim et al., 2022), presumably reflecting increased vigilance or fear. The role of attention and vigilance in hearing in noise should be investigated further in future studies.

Cochlear damage

Our anatomical analysis showed that noise-exposed mice experienced loss of OHCs and a reduction in the number of ribbons per hair cell in the high-frequency regions of the cochlea relative to what occurs with aging alone. OHCs in the base of the cochlea are most vulnerable to the effects of both noise and age, and impaired recovery from noise exposure in the aging auditory periphery has been previously reported (Möhrle et al., 2016). Thus, the confluence of noise and age resulting in more anatomical and perceptual damage in the present study is not surprising.

Increased vulnerability of the aged auditory system may be due to a reduced capacity for recovery or an increased susceptibility to damage in weakening cells (Ohlemiller et al., 2000). Although age and noise exposure have previously been shown to have additive effects (Sun et al., 1994), age was not statistically associated with cochlear damage in our study, likely because of the similarity in the ages of our exposed and unexposed groups and the range of ages (400–700 d.o.) of mice tested. Remarkably, the regions with increased damage were outside the mid-frequency regions of the tones used in the behavioral detection task. However, a change in the cochlear place-frequency map may have occurred in aged, noise-exposed mice. Noise exposure is known to shift the characteristic frequencies of auditory nerve fibers down by ∼0.6–1.2 octaves in young adult mice (Müller and Smolders, 2005). Whether or not similar shifts occur in aged, noise-exposed mice or aged mice alone is uncertain. Nevertheless, a down-shift in auditory nerve fiber tuning would presumably not have had a negative impact on the detection of tones in the mid-frequency range since the number of ribbons and hair cells was not affected by noise for regions that are 0.6–1.6 octaves below the tone frequencies.

One surprising finding regarding the cochlear damage patterns was the increased OHC survival in aged, noise-exposed mice in the apical-most region of the cochlea and the increased survival in aged, noise-exposed mice in the basal-most region of the cochlea relative to unexposed, aged controls. One possible explanation for these findings could be that experience with nondamaging sounds can promote cell survival with age (Dziorny et al., 2021). Thus, daily exposure to sounds in the test environment may have protected against apical OHC and basal IHC loss. Sound exposure may activate olivocochlear efferents, and the medial olivocochlear neurons, in particular, have protective effects in the lower frequency regions of the aging cochlea (Liberman et al., 2014). We can consider how experience with noise may alter the degree of damage sustained by the aging auditory system, but further research is necessary to understand the underlying mechanisms.

In conclusion, our experiments revealed that behavioral recovery from damaging noise exposure is dependent on age and listening conditions. Aged noise-exposed mice were more susceptible to hearing loss when detecting a tone in quiet than when detecting a tone embedded in a masker. Finally, post-exposure mice listening to 14-kHz tones experienced greater threshold shifts than mice listening to 20-kHz tones. Together, these results highlight the importance of employing behavioral methodologies and a longitudinal design in examining the complex interactions between noise-induced and age-related hearing losses, as well as assessing hearing in both quiet and noise.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgements: We thank the numerous undergraduate researchers for their assistance with the daily testing of mice in the operant experiments.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This work was supported by National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Grants R01-DC012302 (to M.L.D.), R01-DC016641 (to M.L.D. and A.M.L.), and T32-DC000023 (to K.B. and L.A.S.); the Acoustical Society of America James E. West Fellowship (D.F.V.), and the David M. Rubenstein Fund for Hearing Research.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Burke K, Manohar S, Dent ML (2021) Long term changes to auditory sensitivity following blast trauma in mice. Hear Res 403:108201. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2021.108201 pmid:33636682
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Chambers AR, Resnik J, Yuan Y, Whitton JP, Edge AS, Liberman MC, Polley DB (2016) Central gain restores auditory processing following near-complete cochlear denervation. Neuron 89:867–879. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.041 pmid:26833137
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Chatterjee M, Robert ME (2001) Noise enhances modulation sensitivity in cochlear implant listeners: stochastic resonance in a prosthetic sensory system? J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 2:159–171. doi:10.1007/s101620010079 pmid:11550525
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Clarkson C, Antunes FM, Rubio ME (2016) Conductive hearing loss has long-lasting structural and molecular effects on presynaptic and postsynaptic structures of auditory nerve synapses in the cochlear nucleus. J Neurosci 36:10214–10227. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0226-16.2016 pmid:27683915
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    Dent ML, Screven LA, Kobrina A (2018) Hearing in rodents. In: Rodent bioacoustics (Dent ML, Fay R, Popper A, eds), pp 71–105. Cham: Springer.
  6. ↵
    Dziorny AC, Luebke AE, Scott LL, Walton JP (2021) Rescuing auditory temporal processing with a novel augmented acoustic environment in an animal model of congenital hearing loss. eNeuro 8:ENEURO.0231-21.2021. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0231-21.2021
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Ehret G (1979) Correlations between cochlear hair cell loss and shifts of masked and absolute behavioral auditory thresholds in the house mouse. Acta otolaryngol 87:28–38. doi:10.3109/00016487909126384 pmid:760375
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Erway LC, Shiau YW, Davis RR, Krieg EF (1996) Genetics of age-related hearing loss in mice. III. Susceptibility of inbred and F1 hybrid strains to noise-induced hearing loss. Hear Res 93:181–187. doi:10.1016/0378-5955(95)00226-x pmid:8735078
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Evans JC, Dall SRX, Kight CR (2018) Effects of ambient noise on zebra finch vigilance and foraging efficiency. PLoS One 13:e0209471. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0209471 pmid:30596692
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    Fernandez KA, Guo D, Micucci S, De Gruttola V, Liberman MC, Kujawa SG (2020) Noise-induced cochlear synaptopathy with and without sensory cell loss. J Neurosci 427:43–57. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.11.051 pmid:31887361
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Frisina RD, Newman SR, Zhu X (2007) Auditory efferent activation in CBA mice exceeds that of C57s for varying levels of noise. J Acoust Soc Am 121:EL29–EL34. doi:10.1121/1.2401226 pmid:17297823
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Gopinath B, McMahon C, Tang D, Burlutsky G, Mitchell P (2021) Workplace noise exposure and the prevalence and 10-year incidence of age-related hearing loss. PLoS One 16:e0255356. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0255356 pmid:34329348
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Gold JR, Bajo VM (2014) Insult-induced adaptive plasticity of the auditory system. Front Neurosci 8:110. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00110 pmid:24904256
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Henry KR (1982) Age-related changes in sensitivity of the postpubertal ear to acoustic trauma. Hear Res 8:285–294. doi:10.1016/0378-5955(82)90020-x pmid:7153183
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Henry KR (2004) Males lose hearing earlier in mouse models of late-onset age-related hearing loss; females lose hearing earlier in mouse models of early-onset hearing loss. Hear Res 190:141–148. doi:10.1016/S0378-5955(03)00401-5 pmid:15051136
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Henry KS (2022) Animal models of hidden hearing loss: does auditory-nerve-fiber loss cause real-world listening difficulties? Mol Cell Neurosci 118:103692. doi:10.1016/j.mcn.2021.103692 pmid:34883241
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Henry KS, Abrams KS (2021) Normal tone-in-noise sensitivity in trained budgerigars despite substantial auditory-nerve injury: no evidence of hidden hearing loss. J Neurosci 41:118–129. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2104-20.2020 pmid:33177067
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    Ho MK, Li C, Wang J, Ohmen JD, Friedman RA (2014) FVB/NJ mice demonstrate a youthful sensitivity to noise-induced hearing loss and provide a useful genetic model for the study of neural hearing loss. Audiol Neurotol Extra 4:1–11. doi:10.1159/000357770 pmid:24707282
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Holme RH, Steel KP (2004) Progressive hearing loss and increased susceptibility to noise-induced hearing loss in mice carrying a Cdh23 but not a Myo7a mutation. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 5:66–79. doi:10.1007/s10162-003-4021-2 pmid:14648237
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Ison JR (2001) The acoustic startle response: reflex elicitation and reflex modification by preliminary stimuli. In: Handbook of mouse auditory research: from behavior to molecular biology, Ed 1 (Willott JF, ed), pp 59–82. Boca Raton: CRC Press.
  21. ↵
    Kim Y, Schrode KM, Engel J, Vicencio-Jimenez S, Rodriguez G, Lee H, Lauer AM (2022) Auditory behavior in adult-blinded mice. J Assoc Res Otolatyngol 23:225–239.
    OpenUrl
  22. ↵
    Knipper M, van Dijk P, Schulze H, Mazurek B, Krauss P, Scheper V, Warnecke A, Schlee W, Schwabe K, Singer W, Braun C, Delano PH, Fallgatter AJ, Ehlis A-C, Searchfield GD, Munk MHJ, Baguley DM, Rüttiger L (2020) The neural bases of tinnitus: lessons from deafness and cochlear implants. J Neurosci 40:7190–7202. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1314-19.2020 pmid:32938634
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    Kobrina A, Dent ML (2016) The effects of aging and sex on detection of ultrasonic vocalizations by adult CBA/CaJ mice (Mus musculus). Hear Res 341:119–129. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2016.08.014 pmid:27579993
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Kobrina A, Dent ML (2020) The effects of age and sex on the detection of pure tones by adult CBA/CaJ mice (Mus musculus). J Neurosci Res 98:1731–1744. doi:10.1002/jnr.24496 pmid:31304616
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Kobrina A, Schrode KM, Screven LA, Javaid H, Weinberg MM, Brown G, Board R, Villavisanis DF, Dent ML, Lauer AM (2020) Linking anatomical and physiological markers of auditory system degeneration with behavioral hearing assessments in a mouse (Mus musculus) model of age-related hearing loss. Neurobiol Aging 96:87–103. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2020.08.012 pmid:32950782
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Knudson IM, Shera CA, Melcher JR (2014) Increased contralateral suppression of otoacoustic emissions indicates a hyperresponsive medial olivocochlear system in humans with tinnitus and hyperacusis. J Neurophysiol 112:3197–3208. doi:10.1152/jn.00576.2014 pmid:25231612
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Kujawa SG, Liberman MC (2006) Acceleration of age-related hearing loss by early noise exposure: evidence of a misspent youth. J Neurosci 26:2115–2123. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4985-05.2006 pmid:16481444
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    Kujawa SG, Liberman MC (2015) Synaptopathy in the noise-exposed and aging cochlea: primary degeneration in acquired sensorineural hearing loss. Hear Res 330:191–199. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2015.02.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Lauer AM, Schrode KM (2017) Sex bias in basic and preclinical noise-induced hearing loss research. Noise Health 19:207–212. doi:10.4103/nah.NAH_12_17 pmid:28937014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Lauer AM, Dent ML, Sun W, Xu-Friedman MA (2019) Effects of non-traumatic noise and conductive hearing loss on auditory system function. Neurosci 407:182–191. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.01.020 pmid:30685543
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Lauer AM, Jimenez SV, Delano PH (2021) Olivocochlear efferent effects on perception and behavior. Hear Res 108207. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2021.108207
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  32. ↵
    Li HS (1992) Genetic influences on susceptibility of the auditory system to aging and environmental factors. Scand Audiol Suppl 75:521–524.
    OpenUrl
  33. ↵
    Li HS, Borg E (1993) Auditory degeneration after acoustic trauma in two genotypes of mice. Hear Res 68:19–27. doi:10.1016/0378-5955(93)90060-E pmid:8376211
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Liberman MC, Liberman LD, Maison SF (2014) Efferent feedback slows cochlear aging. J Neurosci 34:4599–4607. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4923-13.2014 pmid:24672005
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    McDonnell MD, Abbott D (2009) What is stochastic resonance? Definitions, misconception, debates, and its relevance to biology. PLoS Comput Biol 5:e10000348. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000348
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  36. ↵
    McGuire B, Fiorillo B, Ryugo DK, Lauer AM (2015) Auditory nerve synapses persist in ventral cochlear nucleus long after loss of acoustic input in mice with early-onset progressive hearing loss. Brain Res 1605:22–30. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.012 pmid:25686750
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    McFadden SL, Willott JF (1994) Responses of inferior colliculus neurons in C57BL/6J mice with and without sensorineural hearing loss: effects of changing the azimuthal location of an unmasked pure-tone stimulus. Hear Res 78:115–131. doi:10.1016/0378-5955(94)90018-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Möhrle D, Ni K, Varakina K, Bing D, Lee SC, Zimmermann U, Knipper M, Rüttiger L (2016) Loss of auditory sensitivity from inner hair cell synaptopathy can be centrally compensated in the young but not old brain. Neurobiol Aging 44:173–184. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.05.001 pmid:27318145
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Müller M, Smolders JW (2005) Shift in the cochlear place–frequency map after noise damage in the mouse. Neuroreport 16:1183–1187. doi:10.1097/00001756-200508010-00010 pmid:16012345
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Müller M, von Hünerbein K, Hoidis S, Smolders JW (2005) A physiological place-frequency map of the cochlea in the CBA/J mouse. Hear Res 202:63–73. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2004.08.011 pmid:15811700
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    Ohlemiller KK, Wright JS, Heidbreder AF (2000) Vulnerability to noise-induced hearing loss in ‘middle-aged’ and young adult mice: a dose-response approach in CBA, C57BL, and BALB inbred strains. Hear Res 149:239–247. doi:10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00191-X
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Prosen CA, Dore DJ, May BJ (2003) The functional age of hearing loss in a mouse model of presbycusis. I. Behavioral assessments. Hear Res 183:44–56. doi:10.1016/S0378-5955(03)00211-9
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    Resnik J, Polley DB (2021) Cochlear neural degeneration disrupts hearing in background noise by increasing auditory cortex internal noise. Neuron 109:984–996. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2021.01.015 pmid:33561398
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Salvi RJ, Ding D, Eddins AC, McFadden SL, Henderson D (2001) Age, noise and ototoxic agents. In: Functional neurobiology of aging (Hof PR, Mobbs CV, eds), pp 549–563. New York: Academic Press.
  45. ↵
    Salvi RJ, Ding D, Wang J, McFadden SL, Sun W (2003) Functional changes in peripheral and central auditory pathways following selective inner hair cell loss. Semin Hear 24:135–146.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  46. ↵
    Schrode KM, Muniak MA, Kim Y-H, Lauer AM (2018) Central compensation in auditory brainstem after damaging noise exposure. eNeuro 5:ENEURO.0250-18.2018–19. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0250-18.2018
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    Sinclair JL, Barnes-Davies M, Kopp-Scheinpflug C, Forsythe ID (2017) Strain-specific differences in the development of neuronal excitability in the mouse ventral nucleus of the trapezoid body. Hear Res 354:28–37. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2017.08.004 pmid:28843833
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Sliwińiska-Kowalska M, Pawelczyk M, Kowalski TJ (2006) Genetic factors in susceptibility to age- and noise-related hearing loss. Pol Merkur Lekarski 21:384–388. pmid:17205784
    OpenUrlPubMed
  49. ↵
    Strait DL, Kraus N (2011) Can you hear me now? Musical training shapes functional brain networks for selective auditory attention and hearing speech in noise. Front Psychol 2:113. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00113 pmid:21716636
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Sun JC, Bohne BA, Harding GW (1994) Is the older ear more susceptible to noise damage? Laryngoscope 104:1251–1258. doi:10.1288/00005537-199410000-00012 pmid:7934596
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Toppila E, Pyykkö I, Pääkkönen R (2009) Evaluation of the increased accident risk from workplace noise. Int J Occup Saf Ergon 15:155–162. doi:10.1080/10803548.2009.11076796 pmid:19534848
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Villavisanis DF, Schrode KM, Lauer AM (2018) Sex bias in basic and preclinical age-related hearing loss research. Biol Sex Differ 9:23. doi:10.1186/s13293-018-0185-7
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  53. ↵
    Villavisanis DF, Berson ER, Lauer AM, Cosetti MK, Schrode KM (2020) Sex-based differences in hearing loss: perspectives from non-clinical research to clinical outcomes. Otol Neurotol 41:290–298. doi:10.1097/MAO.0000000000002507 pmid:31789968
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Vicencio-Jimenez S, Weinberg M, Bucci-Mansilla G, Lauer AM (2021) Olivocochlear changes associated with aging predominantly affect the medial olivocochlear system. Front Neurosci 15:704805.
    OpenUrl
  55. ↵
    Wang H, Brozoski TJ, Ling L, Hughes LF, Caspary DM (2011) Impact of sound exposure and aging on brain-derived neurotrophic factor and tyrosine kinase B receptor level in dorsal cochlear nucleus 80 days following sound exposure. Neurosci 172:453–459. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.10.056 pmid:21034795
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Wu JS, Yi E, Manca M, Javaid H, Lauer AM, Glowatzki E (2020) Sound exposure dynamically induces dopamine synthesis in cholinergic LOC efferents for feedback to auditory nerve fibers. Elife 9:e52419. doi:10.7554/eLife.52419
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  57. Wu PZ, O’Malley JT, de Gruttola V, Liberman MC (2020) Age-related hearing loss is dominated by damage to inner ear sensory cells, not the cellular battery that powers them. J Neurosci 40:6357–6366. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0937-20.2020 pmid:32690619
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    Wu PZ, O’Malley JT, de Gruttola V, Liberman MC (2021) Primary neural degeneration in noise-exposed human cochleas: correlations with outer hair cell loss and word-discrimination scores. J Neurosci 41:4439–4447. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3238-20.2021
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  59. ↵
    Zhao Y, Song Q, Li X, Li C (2016) Neural hyperactivity of the central auditory system in response to peripheral damage. Neural Plast 2016:2162105. doi:10.1155/2016/2162105 pmid:26881094
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Christine Portfors, Washington State University

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Christine Köppl, Kenneth Henry.

The authors' responsive revision of their manuscript on behavioral tone detection in aging noise-exposed mice is greatly appreciated. The reviewers find the revised manuscript more clear and easier to follow. The presentation of the results however still has problems that need to be addressed. In particular, there is concern over the model generating data at mouse ages that were not directly evaluated and statistical concerns/errors that may affect the interpretation of the results.

Detailed comments:

The title is confusing to me; how can ‘behavioral tone detection’ have ‘temporary and permanent effects’ on something? Perhaps ‘Effects of noise exposure and aging on mouse behavioral tone detection in quiet and in noise’?

Line 234 Clarify that age^2 was a continuous independent variable.

Line 240. It seems like thresholds were “predicted” at different ages from the linear model, which can be dangerous when there is little or no data at the age for which the prediction is made (see more on this below). Some additional description of the procedure would be helpful here if it is retained, since post-hoc comparisons are more typically made between groups defined based categorical independent variables. How do results/inferences differ with a categorical variable?

Line 296 and 298. Here and elsewhere the P values are incorrect and do not match those presented in the tables at the end of the paper. The exponents appear to have dropped off. It’s a little unusual to see exponents on the P values. Oftentimes for p value below some very small criterion value (e.g, 0.0001), P values are simply reported as ‘P < 0.0001’.

Line 294-307. It is hard to see the support for the statement on line 304-307 that young mice were less affect by the noise exposure, with greater recovery. The model results in 2A show more of a main effect of age, but with more variability in the young ‘group’ resulting in no significant differences across time periods. But this is not actually a group (the text in this section needs to make this much clearer), it is the predicted pattern for 500 day-old mice. I don’t understand why the model would predict greater variability? Does this have to do with the fact that no mice in this experiment are that young, and only 2 are between 500 and 600 days at the age of exposure? It would be comforting to go back to the raw data and see what patterns the individual mice are showing, but this not possible due to the density of Fig 1B. One alternative would be to show data from 450-550, 551-650, and 651-750 day-old mice in separate panels. Perhaps 1A could be removed and the only lost information (mean baseline thresholds) inserted into the legend of the new panels.

Figure 2. ‘Predicted mean threshold’ might be a clearer title for the y axis here and elsewhere.

Line 322-346. First, there is another incorrect p value on line 325 that will be disconcerting to readers. Second, the same issue exists of having a detailed comparison of hearing loss at different age ranges based on a statistical model prediction, except here, it is much worse because 3 mice are 414-470 days old and two mice are old (>800 days). One would have to have extreme faith in age^2 to draw any conclusion about what is happening in 600 day-old mice, but a mean line appears in Fig 5A along with error bars and detailed discussion of the pattern in the text. An even larger issue is that the statistical model results (predicted thresholds) in 5A are totally different than the raw data in Fig. 4. The threshold shift immediately after noise is ∼10 dB in the raw data but the model predicted threshold is 20-40 dB

Line 348-364. It is a little surprising that so many pairwise comparisons are significant given the overlapping standard error bars in Fig. 5B, once again casting doubt on the validity of inferences from the model.

Line 367 Some introduction to this section would be helpful. Also, authors often report the p values for the non-significant main effects to establish why they were dropped from the statistical model

Please clarify the chemicals used:

Line 171: anaesthetic doses are given in mg/kg body weight. This is perfectly clear. But what is meant by “ ... at 100% of their body weight”?

line198-199: PBS (phosphate-buffered saline) is usually the carrier solution in which all other substances are contained at the concentrations given. As it reads now, the PBS itself is diluted to 10% of its original strength. Is this correct?

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 9 (3)
eNeuro
Vol. 9, Issue 3
May/June 2022
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Effects of Noise Exposure and Aging on Behavioral Tone Detection in Quiet and Noise by Mice
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Effects of Noise Exposure and Aging on Behavioral Tone Detection in Quiet and Noise by Mice
Kali Burke, Laurel A. Screven, Anastasiya Kobrina, Payton E. Charlton, Katrina Schrode, Dillan F. Villavisanis, Micheal L. Dent, Amanda M. Lauer
eNeuro 25 May 2022, 9 (3) ENEURO.0391-21.2022; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0391-21.2022

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Effects of Noise Exposure and Aging on Behavioral Tone Detection in Quiet and Noise by Mice
Kali Burke, Laurel A. Screven, Anastasiya Kobrina, Payton E. Charlton, Katrina Schrode, Dillan F. Villavisanis, Micheal L. Dent, Amanda M. Lauer
eNeuro 25 May 2022, 9 (3) ENEURO.0391-21.2022; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0391-21.2022
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Visual Abstract
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • age-related hearing loss
  • cochlear pathology
  • hearing in noise
  • noise-induced hearing loss
  • operant conditioning
  • threshold instability

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Dendritic Compartmentalization of Learning-Related Plasticity
  • Thalamocortical Projections Are Significantly Impaired in the R6/2 Mouse Model of Huntington’s Disease
  • Attention Cueing in Rivalry: Insights from Pupillometry
Show more Research Article: New Research

Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Optoception: perception of optogenetic brain perturbations
  • Attention Cueing in Rivalry: Insights from Pupillometry
  • Evidence for Independent Processing of Shape by Vision and Touch
Show more Sensory and Motor Systems

Subjects

  • Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2022 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.