Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: Negative Results, Disorders of the Nervous System

14-3-3θ Does Not Protect against Behavioral or Pathological Deficits in Alzheimer’s Disease Mouse Models

Mary Gannon, Bing Wang, Sara Anne Stringfellow, Stephan Quintin, Itzel Mendoza, Thanushri Srikantha, A. Claire Roberts, Takashi Saito, Takaomi C. Saido, Erik D. Roberson and Talene A. Yacoubian
eNeuro 13 June 2022, 9 (3) ENEURO.0368-21.2022; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0368-21.2022
Mary Gannon
1Center for Neurodegeneration and Experimental Therapeutics, Department of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Mary Gannon
Bing Wang
1Center for Neurodegeneration and Experimental Therapeutics, Department of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sara Anne Stringfellow
1Center for Neurodegeneration and Experimental Therapeutics, Department of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294
2University of South Alabama College of Medicine, Mobile, AL 36688
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Stephan Quintin
1Center for Neurodegeneration and Experimental Therapeutics, Department of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294
3Medical Scientist Training Program, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL 32611
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Itzel Mendoza
1Center for Neurodegeneration and Experimental Therapeutics, Department of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Thanushri Srikantha
1Center for Neurodegeneration and Experimental Therapeutics, Department of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
A. Claire Roberts
1Center for Neurodegeneration and Experimental Therapeutics, Department of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Takashi Saito
4RIKEN Center for Brain Science, Saitama 351-0198, Japan
5Department of Neurocognitive Science, Institute of Brain Science, Nagoya City University Graduates School of Medical Sciences, Nagaya 467-8601, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Takaomi C. Saido
4RIKEN Center for Brain Science, Saitama 351-0198, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Erik D. Roberson
1Center for Neurodegeneration and Experimental Therapeutics, Department of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Erik D. Roberson
Talene A. Yacoubian
1Center for Neurodegeneration and Experimental Therapeutics, Department of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is characterized by progressive cognitive impairment associated with synaptic dysfunction and dendritic spine loss and the pathologic hallmarks of β-amyloid (Aβ) plaques and hyperphosphorylated tau tangles. 14-3-3 proteins are a highly conserved family of proteins whose functions include regulation of protein folding, neuronal architecture, and synaptic function. Additionally, 14-3-3s interact with both Aβ and tau, and reduced levels of 14-3-3s have been shown in the brains of AD patients and in AD mouse models. Here, we examine the neuroprotective potential of the 14-3-3θ isoform in AD models. We demonstrate that 14-3-3θ overexpression is protective and 14-3-3θ inhibition is detrimental against oligomeric Aβ-induced neuronal death in primary cortical cultures. Overexpression of 14-3-3θ using an adeno-associated viral (AAV) vector failed to improve performance on behavioral tests, improve Aβ pathology, or affect synaptic density in the J20 AD mouse model. Similarly, crossing a second AD mouse model, the AppNL-G-F knock-in (APP KI) mouse, with 14-3-3θ transgenic mice failed to rescue behavioral deficits, reduce Aβ pathology, or impact synaptic density in the APP KI mouse model. 14-3-3θ is likely partially insolubilized in the APP models, as demonstrated by proteinase K digestion. These findings do not support increasing 14-3-3θ expression as a therapeutic approach for AD.

  • β-amyloid
  • 14-3-3
  • Alzheimer’s disease
  • cortex
  • hippocampus
  • mouse model

Significance Statement

Despite being the most common form of neurodegeneration, effective treatments for Alzheimer’s disease (AD) remain elusive and therefore novel therapeutic targets are needed. 14-3-3 proteins, a highly conserved family of chaperone proteins involved in regulating synaptic function and neuronal architecture, are reduced in human AD patients, making them a promising novel target. In this study, we examined the effects of increasing the 14-3-3θ isoform in AD models. We observed that 14-3-3θ overexpression reduces oligomeric β-amyloid (Aβ) toxicity in cortical cultures. However, increasing the 14-3-3θ expression in two different AD mouse models failed to rescue behavioral or pathologic phenotypes, indicating a more complicated relationship between 14-3-3s and AD.

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common neurodegenerative disease, with progressive neuronal loss leading to gradual cognitive decline and ultimately death. With the anticipated rise in AD cases in the next several decades, the expected societal burden and financial costs of AD are skyrocketing, with predicted health care costs for AD reaching $1.2 trillion in 2050 (Alzheimer’s Association, 2014). In addition to neuronal loss in the hippocampus and frontotemporal cortex, AD is characterized pathologically by two major hallmarks, β-amyloid (Aβ) plaques and tau tangles. However, cognitive impairment correlates more strongly with synaptic dysfunction and spine loss than with plaque or tangle burden (DeKosky and Scheff, 1990; Terry et al., 1991; Hardy and Selkoe, 2002; Selkoe, 2011; Pozueta et al., 2013; Tu et al., 2014; Boros et al., 2017).

Several pieces of evidence point to a potential role for the 14-3-3 proteins in AD. 14-3-3 proteins are a highly conserved family of proteins that act via protein-protein interactions to regulate several cellular functions important to neuronal function (Dougherty and Morrison, 2004; Gehler et al., 2004; Mackintosh, 2004; Chen and Roche, 2009; Lee et al., 2009; Ramser et al., 2010; Yoon et al., 2012; Chung et al., 2015; Foote et al., 2015; Workman et al., 2015; Lavalley et al., 2016; Kaplan et al., 2017; Pennington et al., 2018). 14-3-3s interact with Aβ and tau and are found in Aβ plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in human AD brains (Hashiguchi et al., 2000; Liao et al., 2004; Umahara et al., 2004; Sumioka et al., 2005). Genetic variants in the 14-3-3 genes are associated with AD risk (Mateo et al., 2008a,b). 14-3-3s are important regulators of neuronal architecture: they stabilize synaptic spines and promote axonal and dendritic growth (Gohla and Bokoch, 2002; Gehler et al., 2004; Ramser et al., 2010; Yoon et al., 2012; Foote et al., 2015; Lavalley et al., 2016; Kaplan et al., 2017). Additionally, 14-3-3s regulate NMDA receptor (NMDAR) trafficking to the synapse and impact cerebellar and hippocampal long-term potentiation (Simsek-Duran et al., 2004; Chen and Roche, 2009; Qiao et al., 2014; Chung et al., 2015). Indeed, the functional 14-3-3 knock-out mice expressing the pan 14-3-3 inhibitor, difopein, show deficits mimicking several aspects of AD mouse models, including reduced spine density, reduced NMDAR subunits at the synapse, impaired long-term potentiation, and hippocampal-dependent learning and memory deficits (Qiao et al., 2014; Foote et al., 2015). Aged rats have reduced 14-3-3θ in hippocampal synapses, and lower 14-3-3θ levels in aged rats correlate with decreased cognitive function (VanGuilder et al., 2010, 2011). Recently, a dramatic 40% reduction in total soluble 14-3-3 protein levels in AD brains was observed, relative to age-matched controls, with a converse increase in total 14-3-3 levels in the insoluble fraction (McFerrin et al., 2017). This is consistent with recent data showing reduced solubility of 14-3-3s, including the 14-3-3θ isoform, in the APP-presenilin (APP/PS1) mouse model compared with controls (G. Xu et al., 2013).

Taken together, this evidence suggests that 14-3-3θ dysfunction contributes to cognitive deficits in AD. In this study, we used primary neuronal cultures and two AD mouse models, the J20 APP transgenic model and the AppNL-G-F knock-in (APP KI) mouse, to test the impact of 14-3-3θ in AD. While we found that 14-3-3θ reduced Aβ-induced neuron loss in culture, 14-3-3θ failed to reduce Aβ plaque load, alter synaptic density, or improve cognitive deficits in the AD mouse models.

Materials and Methods

Mice

All mice used in these studies followed the guidelines of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) and University of Alabama at Birmingham Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC), and all work performed was approved by University of Alabama at Birmingham IACUC. Equal numbers of male and female mice were used for all studies. Mice were group housed under 12/12 h light/dark cycle. The 14-3-3θ line used expressed human 14-3-3θ under the neuronal promoter Thy1.2 (Lavalley et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2018; Underwood et al., 2021). The difopein line used expressed difopein-enhanced yellow fluorescent protein (eYFP) under the promoter Thy1.2 and was initially obtained from Yi Zhou at Florida State University (Qiao et al., 2014). To obtain both transgenic and littermate controls for the primary culture experiments, both hemizygous 14-3-3θ and difopein mice were crossed separately with C57BL/6J mice from The Jackson Laboratory (catalog #000664; RRID: IMSR_JAX:000664). J20 transgenic mice carry both the APP Swedish (KM670/671NL) and APP Indiana (V717F) mutations and were on a C57BL6/J background (Mucke et al., 2000). Homozygous AppNL-G-F/NL-G-F (APP KI) mice (Saito et al., 2014) were provided by the Riken BRC through the National Bio-Resource Project of the MEXT, Japan. Hemizygous 14-3-3θ transgenic mice (14-3-3θ tg) were crossed with homozygous APP KI mice (AppNL-G-F/NL-G-F) to create AppWT/NL-G-F/14-3-3θ tg and AppWT/NL-G-F/WT. These mice were then crossed with each other to yield, among other genotypes, the desired AppNL-G-F/NL-G-F/14-3-3θ tg and AppNL-G-F/NL-G-F/WT mice. Crossing these two mice with each other produced the AppNL-G-F/NL-G-F/14-3-3θ tg (APP KI/14-3-3θ) and AppNL-G-F/NL-G-F/WT (APP KI) mice used in experiments, without the additional possible genotypes. Control mice (nTg) without either the mutant APP or the overexpressed 14-3-3θ were created by crossing a heterozygous APP KI mouse (AppWT/NL-G-F) with a nTg C57BL/6 mouse.

Adeno-associated virus (AAV) preparation and injection

AAV2/CBA-IRES2-eGFP-WPRE (AAV-GFP) and AAV2/CBA-14-3-3θ-V5-his-IRES-eGFP-WPRE (AAV-14-3-3θ/GFP) were constructed as previously described (St Martin et al., 2007; Ding et al., 2015). Male and female J20 transgenic mice and littermate controls were deeply anesthetized with 5% isoflurane and maintained at 0.25−4% during surgery. Mice were bilaterally injected with AAV at 8–11 weeks of age using a stereotactic setup into the dentate gyrus [anteroposterior (AP): − 2.0 from bregma; mediolateral (ML): ±1.3 from midline; dorsoventral (DV): − 2.2 below dura]. Mice were injected with 2 μl of either AAV-GFP (titer: 1.8E + 12 vg/ml, viral genomes/ml) or AAV-14-3-3θ/GFP (titer: 7.0E + 11 vg/ml) at a rate of 0.25 μl/min using a microinjection pump.

Aβ oligomer (Aβo) preparation

Aβ (rPeptide, A-1163-2) was reconstituted in HFIP (Sigma-Aldrich, 105228) and aliquoted. After full evaporation of HFIP in fume hood, aliquots were stored at −80°C until ready for use. One day before experiments, an Aβ aliquot was resuspended to 100 μm. The Aβ was first resuspended in a small volume in sterile DMSO (Sigma D2650) by vortexing and brought to the final volume using HBSS (Invitrogen14,175–095). After pipette trituration, the Aβ was sonicated on high for 15 cycles of 45 s on, 15 s off. The Aβ was then left overnight at 4°C to form oligomers.

Aβ toxicity assay

Hippocampal or cortical regions were dissected from male and female postnatal (P)0 mice and incubated at 37°C for 25 min in papain. Cells were then washed with neurobasal-A media containing B-27 supplement, Glutamax, and 5% FBS and then triturated using fire polished glass pipettes. Cells were pelleted by centrifugation at 700 rpm for 5 min and were resuspended in neurobasal-A media containing B-27 supplement, Glutamax, and 5% FBS and plated on 18 mm poly-d-lysine-coated coverslips. After 12–16 h, media were replaced by neurobasal-A media containing B-27 supplement, Glutamax, and Arabinose C at 6 μm. Fifty percent media changes were made every 7 d. After 14 d, 1 or 5 μm of Aβ oligomers (Aβo) or vehicle control were added. After 24 h of Aβo treatment, cells were incubated with ethidium homodimers to stain dead cells and Hoechst dye to stain all cells, and then mounted onto slides using VectaShield (Vector Laboratories). Total cells stained with both ethidium homodimers and Hoechst were then counted and a percentage of dead cells calculated.

Behavior

All behavior tests were performed with approximately equal numbers of male and female mice.

Open field

Mice were placed in a 48 × 48-inch open arena with clear plexi-glass walls and allowed to freely move for 4 min while being recorded using EthoVision software. Video was then analyzed for overall velocity and distance moved across all planes (vertical and horizontal).

Elevated plus maze (EPM)

Mice were placed in the center of plus shaped maze elevated above the floor. Two oppositely placed arms of the maze are open and the remaining two arms are closed. Mice were allowed to freely explore the maze for 4 min while being recorded using EthoVision software. Video was then analyzed to calculate the percentage of time that mice spent in the open arm.

Morris water maze (MWM)

Mice were placed in a pool of water with a hidden platform submerged under the surface of the water. During the acquisition phase of the test, the time taken for the mice to find the platform (latency to platform) was recorded for four trials daily for 5 d. Immediately following the final trial on day 5 of the acquisition phase, a probe trial was conducted where the platform was removed. The percentage of time spent in the quadrant where the platform had been located (target quadrant) was then calculated. All trials were recorded and analyzed using EthoVision software.

Passive avoidance

The experiment was performed using a two-compartment box in which one chamber was darkened, and a grid floor of stainless-steel bars was connected to an internal shock source (Gemini II Avoidance System). On the training day, each mouse was placed in the lighted chamber with the interchamber door closed. After 60 s the door was opened and when the mouse crossed to the darkened side the door was closed and a 0.5 mA × 2 s foot shock was administered. Mice were left in the dark chamber for 30 s before being returned to their home cage. Mice were tested 24 h later and the protocol was repeated but without application of a foot-shock, with a cutoff time of 5 min. For both training and testing, the latency was measured as the time when all four paws entered into the darkened chamber.

Immunohistochemistry

Immediately following the conclusion of behavior testing, mice were perfused with PBS before 4% paraformaldehyde using a forced pump system. The brains were then processed and embedded in paraffin and were sliced in 7-μm-thick sections. Sections were dewaxed and rehydrated followed by antigen retrieval done by autoclaving in citrate buffer. Sections were then quenched with 0.3% hydrogen peroxide in methanol before blocking in 5% NGS with 0.1% Triton X-100. Sections were incubated overnight with primary antibody [mouse GFP (abcam ab1218; RRID: AB_298911), rabbit V5 (Sigma V8137; RRID: AB_261889), rabbit NMDA receptor 2a (NR2A; Millipore 07–632; RRID: AB_310837), guinea pig vesicular glutamate transporter 1 (VGLUT1) (Synaptic Systems 135 304; RRID: AB_887878), mouse 14-3-3θ (Santa Cruz SC-69720; RRID: AB_2218224), or mouse HA (Biolegend 901501; RRID: AB_2565006)]. After washing with TBS, sections were incubated with Cy3-conjugated goat anti-mouse, Cy3-conjugated goat anti-rabbit, or Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-guinea pig secondary antibody. Slides were coverslipped using ProLong Diamond Antifade mounting solution (Thermofisher Scientific) and imaged using an Olympus BX51 epifluorescence microscope or Nikon Eclipse Ti2 confocal microscope.

Aβ plaque load quantification

Every fourth section through the hippocampus was stained for Aβ, using the TSA Plus amplification kit (Akoya Biosciences, NEL763001KT). Sections were de-waxed and rehydrated and antigen retrieval was done by autoclaving in citrate buffer. Sections were quenched with 0.3% hydrogen peroxide in methanol before blocking and overnight incubation with primary antibody (mouse anti-Aβ 82E1, IBL 10 326). After washing, sections were incubated with goat anti-mouse IgG biotinylated secondary antibody (Vector Laboratories, BA-9200). Sections were then incubated in HRP-conjugated streptavidin (Thermo Scientific) and finally with the FITC-conjugated Tyramide. Slides were coverslipped using ProLong Diamond Antifade mounting solution (Thermofisher Scientific) and imaged using an Olympus BX51 epifluorescence microscope. Images were then quantified using the ‘analyze particles’ function in ImageJ to calculate percent plaque load in both hippocampus and cortex.

Synapsin quantification

To estimate synaptic density, paraffin embedded sections were stained for the presynaptic marker synapsin. Paraffin embedded sections were stained as described for the Aβ immunohistochemistry methods using a rabbit synapsin primary antibody (Invitrogen 51-5200), followed by the TSA Plus amplification kit as described above. 60× images of each hippocampal section were acquired using a Nikon Eclipse Ti2 confocal microscope. The area positive for synapsin staining was quantified using ImageJ software. Images were converted to 8 bit in ImageJ, and the threshold was set with a lower limit of 15 and upper limit of 255. The area fraction and total area of the image was obtained through the “measure” feature of ImageJ. Any blank spaces in the image because of tears in the tissue were then circled as ROIs and the total area of the empty space was obtained using the “measure” feature again. The empty space area was subtracted from the initial total area of the image, and the percent area positive for synapsin staining was calculated. For each mouse, two images from each of three to four hippocampal sections were analyzed and averaged.

Proteinase K digestion

Sections were dewaxed and rehydrated and then treated for 10 min at 37°C with 20 μg/ml of proteinase K diluted in TBS or TBS only. Antigen retrieval was done by autoclaving in citrate buffer and sections were quenched with 0.3% hydrogen peroxide in methanol. Sections were then blocked in 5% NGS with 0.1% Triton X-100 and incubated overnight with primary antibody (mouse 14-3-3θ; Santa Cruz SC-69720; RRID: AB_2218224). After washing with TBS, sections were incubated with Cy3-conjugated goat anti-mouse secondary antibody. Slides were coverslipped using ProLong Diamond Antifade mounting solution (Thermofisher Scientific) and imaged using a Nikon Eclipse Ti2 confocal microscope.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses and graphing were performed in Prism 9.2 (GraphPad Software, Inc., La Jolla, CA). Details of statistical tests are outlined in Table 1.

Results

14-3-3 overexpression protects while inhibition exacerbates oligomeric Aβ-induced cell death in vitro

We first examined the effects of 14-3-3s on oligomeric Aβ (Aβo) toxicity in neuronal cultures. We treated day in vitro (DIV)14 primary cortical neurons from 14-3-3θ transgenic mice, which demonstrate 14-3-3θ overexpression in the cortex and hippocampus (Fig. 1A,E; Underwood et al., 2021), and their nontransgenic (nTg) littermate controls with Aβo for 24 h. Aβo treatment induced neuronal toxicity in a dose-dependent manner in primary cortical neurons from nTg mice (Fig. 1B,F). At both 1 and 5 μm Aβ concentrations, we saw a reduction in Aβo-induced toxicity in 14-3-3θ cortical neurons compared with nTg neurons (two-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,33) = 20.28, p < 0.0001; Aβ dose F(2,33) = 38.67, p < 0.0001; interaction F(2,33) = 15.40, p < 0.0001; Fig. 1B,F). Interestingly, 14-3-3θ overexpression was minimally protective against Aβo toxicity in hippocampal cultures (two-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,35) = 5.801, p = 0.0214; Aβ dose F(2,35) = 27.00, p < 0.0001; interaction F(2,35) = 1.626, p = 0.2112; Fig. 1G).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

14-3-3θ reduces oligomeric Aβ toxicity in primary cortical cultures. A, Immunocytochemistry for exogenous HA-tagged 14-3-3θ in nontransgenic (nTg) and 14-3-3θ transgenic mouse neuronal cultures. Scale bar: 100 μm. B, Representative images of primary cortical neurons treated with oligomeric Aβ from nontransgenic or 14-3-3θ mice. Ethidium D (EthD) labels the nuclei of dying cells, while Hoechst 33342 stains the nuclei of all cells. Scale bar: 100 μm. C, Immunocytochemistry for eYFP-difopein in nontransgenic and difopein transgenic mouse neuronal cultures. Scale bar: 100 μm. D, Representative images of primary cortical neurons treated with oligomeric Aβ from nontransgenic or difopein mice. Ethidium D labels the nuclei of dying cells, while Hoechst 33342 stains the nuclei of all cells. Scale bar: 100 μm. E, Western blotting for 14-3-3θ levels in Triton X-100 soluble fractions from hippocampal (H) and cortical (C) cultures from nontransgenic and 14-3-3θ mice. F, Quantification of cell death in primary cortical neurons from nTg or 14-3-3θ littermate mice treated with Aβ oligomers for 24 h. n = 6 per condition; **p ≤ 0.01, ****p ≤ 0.0001 (Tukey’s multiple comparison test). Error bars represent standard error of the mean (SEM). G, Quantification of cell death in primary hippocampal neurons from nTg or 14-3-3θ littermate mice treated with Aβ oligomers for 24 h. n = 7 per condition. Error bars represent SEM. H, Quantification of cell death in primary cortical neurons from nTg or difopein littermate mice treated with Aβ oligomers for 24 h. n = 4 per condition, **p ≤ 0.01, ****p ≤ 0.0001 (Tukey’s multiple comparison test). Error bars represent SEM. I, Quantification of cell death in primary hippocampal neurons from nTg or difopein littermate mice treated with Aβ oligomers for 24 h. n = 4 per condition. Error bars represent SEM.

To test the impact of 14-3-3 inhibition on Aβo toxicity, we used a functional knock-out mouse expressing the pan 14-3-3 inhibitor, difopein, tagged with eYFP under the Thy1.2 promoter. Difopein (dimeric fourteen-three-three peptide inhibitor) is a high-affinity 14-3-3 competitive antagonist peptide that disrupts 14-3-3/ligand interactions by binding within the amphipathic groove of all 14-3-3 isoforms (Masters et al., 2001). This mouse (line 132) expresses difopein-eYFP in neurons in the cortex and hippocampus (Fig. 1C; Qiao et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2018; Underwood et al., 2021). We treated DIV14 primary neurons from difopein mice with Aβo for 24 h. Cortical neurons from difopein mice showed an exacerbation of Aβo-induced toxicity in cortical neurons compared with nTg controls at both 1 and 5 μm (two-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,18) = 25.85, p < 0.0001; Aβ dose F(2,18) = 42.68, p < 0.0001; interaction F(2,18) = 9.481, p = 0.0015; Fig. 1D,H). However, difopein expression did not impact Aβo toxicity in hippocampal cultures (two-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,18) = 3.952, p = 0.0622; Aβ dose F(2,18) = 16.47, p < 0.0001; interaction F(2,18) = 0.6126, p = 0.5529; Fig. 1I). Taken together, these data show that 14-3-3θ is protective against Aβo-induced toxicity in cortical but not hippocampal neurons in culture.

14-3-3θ does not reduce cognitive decline in AD mouse models

We next examined the effects of 14-3-3θ overexpression in two different AD mouse models: (1) the J20 APP mouse (Mucke et al., 2000), and (2) the AppNL-G-F knock-in (APP KI) mouse (Saito et al., 2014). The J20 APP mouse model expresses human APP with the Swedish (K670N, M671L) and Indiana (V717F) mutations under the PDGFβ promoter and demonstrates memory deficits and Aβ plaque deposition (Mucke et al., 2000; Chin et al., 2004, 2005). The newer APP KI model expressing human APP carrying the Swedish, Beyreuther/Iberian (I716F), and Arctic (E22G) mutations also demonstrates memory deficits and Aβ plaque deposition (Saito et al., 2014; Masuda et al., 2016).

We used both viral-mediated overexpression and transgenic means to test the impact of 14-3-3θ overexpression in vivo. We first used stereotactic injections of an AAV vector to overexpress 14-3-3θ in the hippocampi of nTg and J20 mice. Male and female 8- to 11-week-old mice were injected with either AAV-GFP or AAV-14-3-3θ/GFP into the hippocampi bilaterally. We confirmed expression of GFP or V5-tagged 14-3-3θ in the hippocampi of AAV-injected mice at the immediate conclusion of the behavioral experiments, approximately six months after viral injection (Fig. 2A–D). At two and six months after viral injection, we performed behavioral tests, including the open field, EPM, and MWM tests. The J20 mice have been previously shown to demonstrate behavioral alterations in these tests with increased exploratory movement on the open field test, altered anxiety on the EPM, and impaired memory on the MWM (Chin et al., 2005; Kobayashi and Chen, 2005; Ognibene et al., 2005; Cheng et al., 2007; Roberson et al., 2007; Meilandt et al., 2009; Harris et al., 2010).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

14-3-3θ overexpression does not rescue behavioral deficits in J20 mice. A, Representative image of GFP immunostaining of an AAV-GFP-injected nontransgenic (nTg) mouse, indicating correct targeting and expression of the injected virus in the hippocampus of the mice. Scale bar: 100 μm. B, Representative image of V5 immunostaining of an AAV-V5-tagged 14-3-3θ-injected nontransgenic mouse, indicating correct targeting and expression of the injected virus in the hippocampus of the mice. Scale bar: 100 μm. C, Representative image of GFP and 14-3-3θ immunostaining of an AAV-GFP-injected J20 mouse. 14-3-3θ immunostaining detects the endogenous 14-3-3θ expressed in these mice. Scale bar: 100 μm for GFP image and 50 μm for 14-3-3θ image. D, Representative image of V5 and 14-3-3θ immunostaining of an AAV-V5-tagged 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mouse. 14-3-3θ immunostaining detects the endogenous and exogenous 14-3-3θ expressed in these mice, while V5 immunostaining detects only the exogenous V5-tagged 14-3-3θ. Scale bar: 100 μm for GFP image and 50 μm for 14-3-3θ image. E, Quantification of distance traveled in the open field task at 2 MPI. n = 8–10 mice per group; *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01, ****p ≤ 0.0001 (Tukey’s multiple comparison test). Error bars represent SEM. F, Quantification of time spent in the open arm on the EPM at 2 MPI. n = 8–10 mice per group. Error bars represent SEM. G, Quantification of latency to platform on the MWM at 2 MPI. n = 7–10 mice per group; ****p < 0.0001 (Tukey’s multiple comparison test). Error bars represent SEM. H, Quantification of the probe trial in the MWM at 2 MPI. n = 7–10 mice per group; *p ≤ 0.05 (Tukey’s multiple comparison test). Error bars represent SEM. I, Quantification of mortality for GFP and 14-3-3θ-injected nTg and J20 mice. J, Quantification of distance traveled in the open field task at 6 MPI. n = 5–10 mice per group; **p ≤ 0.01, ****p ≤ 0.0001 (Tukey’s multiple comparison test). Error bars represent SEM. K, Quantification of time spent in the open arm on the EPM at 6 MPI. n = 5–10 mice per group. Error bars represent SEM. L, Quantification of latency to platform on the MWM at 6 MPI. n = 5–10 mice per group. Error bars represent SEM.

On open field testing at two months postinjection (MPI) of the virus, when the mice were four to five months old, J20 mice showed more exploratory behavior than nTg mice (Fig. 2E, greater distance traveled), as previously described (Chin et al., 2005; Kobayashi and Chen, 2005; Ognibene et al., 2005; Cheng et al., 2007; Roberson et al., 2007; Meilandt et al., 2009; Harris et al., 2010), while the 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice traveled significantly further than the GFP-injected J20 mice (two-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,33) = 45.57, p < 0.001; virus F(1,33) = 5.910, p = 0.0207; interaction F(1,33) = 3.746, p = 0.0615; Fig. 2E). There was no difference between any of the groups in the EPM, indicating no difference in levels of anxiety at 2 MPI (two-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,33) = 2.451, p = 0.1270; virus F(1,33) = 0.8338, p = 0.3678; interaction F(1,33) = 0.8565, p = 0.3614; Fig. 2F). The J20 mice, regardless of type of virus injection, were impaired in the MWM task at 2 MPI, showing less ability to learn the platform location over the course of training (three-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,85) = 105, p < 0.0001; time F(4,85) = 16.33, p < 0.0001; virus F(1,79) = 2.544, p = 0.1147; time × genotype F(4,85) = 3.84, p = 0.0064; time × virus F(4,79) = 0.7289, p = 0.5749; genotype × virus F(1,79) = 2.266, p = 0.1362; time × genotype × virus F(4,79) = 2.713, p = 0.0357; Fig. 2G). On the final testing day, the 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice were significantly slower at finding the platform than either nTg group (Tukey’s post hoc test: p < 0.0001 for J20 14-3-3θ vs nTg GFP and for J20 14-3-3θ vs nTg 14-3-3θ), but there was no statistically significant difference between the GFP-injected J20 and the 14-3-3θ-injected J20 groups (Tukey’s post hoc test: p = 0.5494). During the probe trial, the 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice performed worse than the 14-3-3θ-injected nTg mice, but no other comparisons were statistically significant (two-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,32) = 3.675, p = 0.0642; virus F(1,32) = 3.170, p = 0.5773; interaction F(1,32) = 4.622, p = 0.0392; Fig. 2H).

Behavioral tests were repeated on the same cohort of mice at six months after AAV injection, when the mice were eight to nine months old, though the sample sizes for these later behavioral tests were limited because of J20 mouse mortality. While not statistically significant, the 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice showed a higher mortality rate than the GFP-injected J20 mice (p = 0.201; log-rank/Mantel–Cox test; Fig. 2I). Premature mortality rates around 15% have been described in the J20 mouse line (Palop et al., 2007). We observed that 2 out of 10 (20%) GFP-injected J20 mice died compared with 5 out of 11 (45%) 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice by the conclusion of the experiment (Fig. 2I).

Similar to the earlier time point, the J20 mice showed greater distance moved in the open field test 6 MPI, regardless of type of virus injection (two-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,27) = 40.84, p < 0.0001; virus F(1,27) = 0.1557, p = 0.9016; interaction F(1,27) = 0.7530, p = 0.3932; Fig. 2J). While the J20 genotype was associated with increased time in the open arm of the EPM, indicating decreased anxiety in these mice, there was no statistical difference between GFP-injected J20 mice and 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice (two-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,27) = 4.821, p = 0.0369; virus F(1,27) = 0.1706, p = 0.6828; interaction F(1,27) = 0.3644, p = 0.5511; Fig. 2K). On the MWM, the J20 mice did significantly worse than nTg mice at learning the location of the platform, but there were no differences between 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice and GFP-injected J20 mice on the final day of testing (three-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,75) = 46.69, p < 0.0001; time F(4,75) = 12.86, p < 0.0001; virus F(1,60) = 6.839, p = 0.0113; time × genotype F(4,75) = 0.5912, p = 0.67; time × virus F(4,60) = 0.6958, p = 0.5978; genotype × virus F(1,60) = 0.3838, p = 0.5379; time × genotype × virus F(4,60) = 0.2333, p = 0.9186; Fig. 2L). Based on these findings, we concluded that AAV-mediated overexpression of 14-3-3θ expression into the hippocampus was not protective in terms of behavioral decline.

We also examined the effect of 14-3-3θ in the APP KI model by crossing 14-3-3θ transgenic mice with APP KI mice. Unlike the J20 mice, we did not observe any mortality of these mice by the end of the experiment (Fig. 3B). As APP KI mice demonstrate much more subtle cognitive changes at later time points, we tested these mice at eight to nine months by open field, elevated zero maze, and the passive avoidance test (Masuda et al., 2016; Saito et al., 2014). The APP KI mice do not demonstrate deficits on the MWM, and thus we did not perform this test (Saito et al., 2014). We examined nTg (AppWT/WT/WT), APP KI (AppNL-G-F/NL-G-F/WT), and APP KI/14-3-3θ (AppNL-G-F/NL-G-F/14-3-3θ tg) mice on these behavioral tests. After the behavior was concluded and the mice were killed, we confirmed exogenous HA-tagged 14-3-3θ expression in the cortex and hippocampus of APP KI/14-3-3θ mice, which was not observed in APP KI mice (Fig. 3A). NTg, APP KI, and APP KI/14-3-3θ mice showed no difference in distance traveled in the open field (one-way ANOVA: F(2,34) = 0.9122, p = 0.4112; Fig. 3C). APP KI and APP KI/14-3-3θ mice spent more time in the open arm in the elevated zero maze compared with the nTg mice, indicating decreased anxiety, but there was no difference between APP KI and APP KI/14-3-3θ mice (one-way ANOVA: F(2,31) = 11.17, p = 0.0002; Fig. 3D). On the passive avoidance test, none of the three groups showed any delay in crossing to the dark side of the shuttle box on day 2, and there was no difference between the groups (one-way ANOVA: F(2,29) = 0.1705, p = 0.8441; Fig. 3E). Of note, the passive avoidance test may have been impacted by an unexpected and uncontrollable alarm that randomly went off in the animal facility during housing of the APP KI cohort. As with the J20 mice, 14-3-3θ overexpression did not rescue behavioral changes in the APP KI mice.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

14-3-3θ overexpression does not modify behavior in APP KI mice. A, Representative images of HA immunostaining in the cortex and hippocampus of APP KI mice demonstrates expression of HA-tagged 14-3-3θ in 14-3-3θ-overexpressing mice (APP KI/14-3-3θ) but not mice without overexpression (APP KI). 14-3-3θ immunostaining detects both endogenous and exogenous 14-3-3θ expressed in APP KI and APP KI/14-3-3θ mice. Scale bar: 100 μm for HA images and 50 μm for 14-3-3θ images. B, Quantification of mortality for nontransgenic, APP KI, and APP KI/14-3-3θ mice. C, Quantification of distance traveled in the open field task at eight to nine months of age. n = 10–16 mice per group. Error bars represent SEM. D, Quantification of time spent in the open arm on the EPM at eight to nine months of age. n = 10–15 mice per group; *p ≤ 0.05, ***p ≤ 0.001 (Tukey’s multiple comparison test). Error bars represent SEM. E, Quantification of latency in crossing to the dark side on day 2 of the passive avoidance task. n = 8–14 mice per group. Error bars represent SEM.

14-3-3θ does not affect Aβ plaque deposition in vivo

Immediately on the conclusion of the last behavioral time point, both cohorts of mice were killed, and their brains were examined for Aβ plaque load using immunofluorescent staining. As expected, the J20 mice had Aβ plaques in the hippocampus, whereas Aβ plaques were not observed in the nTg controls (Fig. 4A). There was no difference in hippocampal Aβ plaque load between the GFP-injected and 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice (unpaired, two-tailed t test: t(9) = 0.09254, p = 0.9238; Fig. 4A). We did not analyze plaque load in the cortex of J20 mice as the AAV injection was directed to the hippocampi. Similarly, the APP KI mice had a large number of plaques in both the hippocampus and cortex, but there was no difference between the APP KI and APP KI/14-3-3θ in either the hippocampus (unpaired, two-tailed t test: t(24) = 1.038, p = 0.3084; Fig. 4B) or cortex (unpaired, two-tailed t test: t(23) = 1.475, p = 0.1538; Fig. 4C). Based on these findings, 14-3-3θ overexpression did not reduce Aβ plaque burden in either the J20 or APP KI mouse models.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

14-3-3θ overexpression does not reduce Aβ plaque burden in AD mice. A, Representative hippocampal images and quantification of total plaque area of J20 mice injected with either AAV-GFP or AAV-14-3-3θ at 6 MPI. n = 5–6 per group. Error bars represent SEM. Scale bar: 500 μm. B, Representative hippocampal images and quantification of total plaque area of APP KI and APP KI/14-3-3θ mice at eight to nine months of age. n = 10–16 per group. Error bars represent SEM. Scale bar: 500 μm. C, Representative cortical images and quantification of total plaque area of APP KI and APP KI/14-3-3θ mice at eight to nine months of age. n = 10–15 per group. Error bars represent SEM. Scale bar: 500 μm.

14-3-3θ does not affect synaptic density in APP models in vivo

We also examined mouse brains for potential loss of synaptic density by staining for the presynaptic marker synapsin in both cohorts of mice. In the J20 mice, there was no difference in the area that stained positive for synapsin, regardless of genotype or injection type (two-way ANOVA: genotype F(1,23) = 0.1737, p = 0.6807; virus F(1,23) = 0.7492, p = 0.3957; interaction F(1,23) = 0.3085, p = 0.5839; Fig. 5A). Similarly, the APP KI mice showed no changes in synapsin, regardless of genotype (one-way ANOVA: F(2,31) = 0.4169, p = 0.6627; Fig. 5B). Immunostaining with the presynaptic marker vesicular glutamate transporter vGLUT1 and postsynaptic marker NR2A confirmed synaptic integrity in the brains of both mouse cohorts (Fig. 5). Based on these results, we concluded that 14-3-3θ overexpression did not alter synaptic density in either cohort of mice.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

14-3-3θ overexpression does not impact synaptic density in AD mice. A, Representative hippocampal images of synapsin, vGLUT1, and NR2A immunoreactivity and quantification of synapsin immunoreactivity in nontransgenic and J20 mice injected with either AAV-GFP or AAV-14-3-3θ at 6 MPI. n = 5–9 per group. Error bars represent SEM. Scale bars: 50 μm for synapsin images and 10 μm for vGLUT1/NR2A images. B, Representative hippocampal images of synapsin, vGLUT1, and NR2A immunoreactivity and quantification of synapsin immunoreactivity in nontransgenic, APP KI, and APP KI/14-3-3θ mice at eight to nine months of age. n = 9–16 per group. Error bars represent SEM. Scale bars: 50 μm for synapsin images and 10 μm for vGLUT1/NR2A images.

APP KI mice show altered subcellular 14-3-3θ distribution and solubility

Given the lack of protective effect for 14-3-3θ overexpression in the APP models, we examined the subcellular 14-3-3θ distribution in J20 and APP KI mice. In GFP-injected J20 mice and in APP KI mice, 14-3-3θ immunoreactivity was distributed in a very intense rim within the cytoplasm of neurons in deep cortical neurons and hippocampal neurons compared with that seen in nTg mice (Figs. 2C, 3A, 6). A similar pattern was observed in the presence of 14-3-3θ overexpression in 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice and in the APP KI/14-3-3θ mice (Figs. 2D, 3A, 6). This pattern of staining in some neurons suggested possible insolubilization of 14-3-3θ in J20 and APP KI mice, as has been observed in human AD brains and in AD mouse models (G. Xu et al., 2013; McFerrin et al., 2017). To test for insoluble 14-3-3θ, brain sections were treated with proteinase K. While proteinase K digestion eliminated 14-3-3θ immunoreactivity in nTg mice, 14-3-3θ immunoreactivity, though reduced, was still observed in APP KI and APP KI/14-3-3θ mice with proteinase K digestion (Fig. 6).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

14-3-3θ immunoreactivity is partially proteinase K-resistant in APP KI mice. Representative images of 14-3-3θ immunostaining in cortical slices with and without proteinase K (PK) treatment from nontransgenic, APP KI, and APP KI/14-3-3θ mice. Scale bar: 50 μm.

Discussion

In this study, we sought to study the effect of 14-3-3θ in AD, using both primary cultured neurons and in vivo AD mouse models. In primary culture, we found that 14-3-3θ overexpression ameliorated, while pan 14-3-3 inhibition exacerbated, Aβ toxicity in cortical cultures. No clear effect was observed in hippocampal cultures, pointing to a potential regional effect of 14-3-3 modulation. Based on these data, we hypothesized that overexpressing 14-3-3θ in AD mice would lead to an improvement in both the cognitive deficits and pathology. However, 14-3-3θ overexpression failed to reduce cognitive decline or Aβ pathology or affect synaptic density in either the J20 or APP KI models. In the case of the J20 mice, we injected AAV-14-3-3θ into the hippocampus because of the difficulty of obtaining widespread cortical AAV expression. Based on our in vitro data, targeting the hippocampus would not necessarily be sufficient to rescue behavior or pathology. However, boosting 14-3-3θ expression in both the cortex and the hippocampus, as was done in our APP KI × 14-3-3θ transgenic cross, was also not sufficient to rescue behavioral or pathologic changes in the cortex or hippocampus.

While it was unexpected to see 14-3-3θ protection in culture but not in vivo, this discrepancy likely reflects the vast differences in the environments of in vitro and in vivo experiments. In the in vivo system, other brain cell types, such as glia and astrocytes, interact with the neuronal population and could affect the findings. Alternatively, 14-3-3θ may be protective only in the short term, such that the protective effects were observed only in culture but not in vivo at the time points examined. Additionally, our in vitro studies showed that the protective effect was limited to cortical cultures and not to hippocampal cultures. This finding suggests that any protective effect of 14-3-3θ could be restricted to certain neuronal populations such that any global protection may not be seen in vivo. Further, we propose that 14-3-3θ overexpression failed to protect against Aβ toxicity in vivo because of potential insolubilization of 14-3-3θ. We found altered patterns of 14-3-3θ immunoreactivity in both the APP KI mice and the APP KI/14-3-3θ mice, in which neurons showed intense, “rim-like” cytoplasmic staining in neurons in the cortex and hippocampus, compared with less intense, more diffuse immunoreactivity in nTg control mice. This immunoreactivity in both APP KI mice and the APP KI/14-3-3θ mice was partially resistant to proteinase K digestion, suggesting a reduction in the solubility of 14-3-3θ, even in the presence of 14-3-3θ overexpression. We hypothesize that the presence of the APP mutations since conception in vivo may drive the insolubilization and consequent dysfunction of 14-3-3θ and thus block its protective effect, even in the presence of overexpression. In contrast, in vitro Aβ oligomers are added to neurons cultured from 14-3-3θ mice that do not contain the APP mutations so that 14-3-3θ is soluble at the time of Aβo treatment, as demonstrated by Western blotting for 14-3-3θ using Triton X-100 soluble fractions from primary neurons (Fig. 1C). Because the neurons are only exposed to the Aβ oligomers for a relatively short time, it is likely not long enough to lead to the insolubility of 14-3-3θ; and therefore, 14-3-3θ is able to maintain its protective functions.

In the J20 mice, we examined cognitive function using the MWM. At both 2 and 6 MPI, the J20 mice showed a reduced ability to locate the platform, but there was no significance difference between J20 mice who received the AAV-GFP injection compared with the AAV-14-3-3θ/GFP. At the earlier time point of 2 MPI, the 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice actually showed a trend of doing worse than the GFP-injected J20 mice. While no difference in water maze performance was noted at the 6 MPI time point between GFP-injected and 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice, increased mortality of the 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice by 6 MPI could have limited our ability to detect a small difference. However, we did not see an increase in mortality in the APP KI/14-3-3θ, which were similarly impaired compared with APP KI mice, so it is unlikely that our lack of improvement was due solely to the poor health or increased mortality of the 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice. 14-3-3θ showed no protection against cognitive decline and potentially worsened cognitive function and mortality in the J20 model.

In the APP KI mice, we assessed cognitive function by using the passive avoidance task. Surprisingly, we failed to see the expected differences in latency to cross to the dark side in APP KI mice compared with nTg controls. Previous studies have shown that APP KI mice perform poorly in this task compared with nTg control mice (Masuda et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2020). One possible explanation for the lack of an effect of the APP KI mice in this behavioral test could be a result of early life exposure to intermittent stress. Unfortunately, because of circumstances out of our control, these mice were exposed to intermittent stress in the form of a randomly malfunctioning fire alarm in the animal facility where the mice were housed. This early exposure to intermittent stress may have led to a desensitization to stress and thus a failure to respond in the passive avoidance task. Therefore, interpretation of this behavioral test is limited. The APP KI mice did demonstrate decreased anxiety as demonstrated by the EPM, as previously shown (Sakakibara et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2020; Degawa et al., 2021), which was not changed by 14-3-3θ overexpression.

One surprising result of our study was a possible trend toward higher mortality of the 14-3-3θ-injected J20 mice compared with the GFP-injected mice. A total of 45% (5/11) of the 14-3-3θ-injected mice died before our planned kill date, whereas only 20% (2/10) of the GFP-injected mice failed to survive. Of note, we had previously noted a high mortality rate of 14-3-3θ/J20 double transgenic mice when we tried to cross the 14-3-3θ transgenic mice with the J20 mice as an initial experimental approach. Because of the early mortality of double transgenic mice from this cross, we decided to switch to the AAV approach. One potential cause for this increased death within the 14-3-3θ-overexpressing J20 mice could be seizures. The J20 mouse line, like most lines expressing high levels of Aβ, has hippocampal hyperexcitability that leads to nonconvulsive seizures and higher rates of premature mortality (Palop et al., 2007). It is possible that the overexpression of 14-3-3θ increased seizure activity. Several studies have implicated 14-3-3s in seizure activity, although most of the data has pointed to a protective effect of other 14-3-3 isoforms in seizures (Schindler et al., 2006; Murphy et al., 2008; Brennan et al., 2013). Reducing tau in J20 mice reduces the severity of seizure activity in these mice (Palop et al., 2007), and 14-3-3ζ has also been shown to interact with tau and regulate tau aggregation in vivo (Sadik et al., 2009a,b). Therefore, it is possible that 14-3-3θ may interact with tau to affect seizure activity in the J20 mice. Finally, 14-3-3 proteins affect both neuronal morphology and glutamatergic receptor trafficking (Gohla and Bokoch, 2002; Gehler et al., 2004; Simsek-Duran et al., 2004; Chen and Roche, 2009; Ramser et al., 2010; Yoon et al., 2012; Qiao et al., 2014; Chung et al., 2015; Foote et al., 2015; Lavalley et al., 2016; Kaplan et al., 2017), such that any 14-3-3θ-induced alterations in receptor trafficking or neuronal morphology could impact seizure activity and thus mortality.

Beyond the lack of behavioral rescue by 14-3-3θ overexpression, we also failed to see any protection against pathologic changes. 14-3-3θ overexpression failed to reduce Aβ plaque deposition. We also measured synaptic density by staining for synapsin, but failed to see any significant effect of 14-3-3θ overexpression on synapsin immunoreactivity. While others have described a loss of synaptophysin immunoreactivity in the J20 mice (Chin et al., 2005), we did not detect a reduction for synapsin staining in J20 or APP KI mice compared with nTg mice by our approach. Our methodology was not optimal for measuring synaptic loss, as our brain sections were much thinner compared with those brain sections used in studies demonstrating synaptophysin changes in J20 mice. Golgi staining or super-resolution structured illumination microscopy would have been a more sensitive approach for measuring synapse loss (Hong et al., 2016). To our knowledge, quantification of synaptic loss has not been demonstrated in the APP KI mouse, although regional synaptophysin decrease around Aβ plaques has been seen (Saito et al., 2014).

14-3-3s are key interactors of several proteins implicated in other neurodegenerative diseases (Ostrerova et al., 1999; Hashiguchi et al., 2000; J. Xu et al., 2002; Sumioka et al., 2005; Kaltenbach et al., 2007; Nichols et al., 2010). A key role for 14-3-3θ dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease (PD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), both disorders marked by α-synuclein (αsyn) aggregation, has previously been established. A reduction of the 14-3-3θ isoform in cellular and mouse αsyn models and in human DLB brains has been shown (Yacoubian et al., 2010; Ding et al., 2013; McFerrin et al., 2017). 14-3-3θ overexpression reduces toxicity in multiple PD models, including neurotoxin, αsyn, and LRRK2 models (Yacoubian et al., 2010; Slone et al., 2011; Ding et al., 2015; Lavalley et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2018). In particular, 14-3-3θ overexpression reduces αsyn aggregation, cell-to-cell transmission, behavioral deficits, and neuronal loss in both in vitro and in vivo αsyn models (Wang et al., 2018; Underwood et al., 2021). This protective effect of 14-3-3θ in αsyn models is a result of its chaperone activity to prevent αsyn misfolding (Wang et al., 2018; Underwood et al., 2021). Our study here testing the impact of 14-3-3θ in Aβ models does not support the hypothesis that the protective effect of 14-3-3θ extends to Aβ mouse models. Indeed, the reduction of 14-3-3s in both human patients with AD and in AD mouse models (McFerrin et al., 2017; G. Xu et al., 2013) may instead point to this reduction in 14-3-3θ in AD human brains as a compensatory effect.

In conclusion, we examined the effect of 14-3-3θ on Aβ-induced toxicity in several AD models. While protective against Aβo-induced toxicity in cortical cultures, 14-3-3θ overexpression failed to rescue cognitive decline and pathologic changes in two APP mouse models. These findings do not support the idea of manipulating 14-3-3θ as a therapeutic approach for AD.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Summary of statistical tests used

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgments: We thank Dr. Yi Zhou at Florida State University who provided the difopein mouse line that was used in the Aβ culture studies.

Footnotes

  • T.A.Y. has a United States Patent #7,919,262 on the use of 14-3-3s in neurodegeneration. All other authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants R01 NS088533 and R01 NS112203 (to T.A.Y.), the Alzheimer’s of Central Alabama (T.A.Y.), and the University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine AMC21 Multi-R01 Pilot Grant (to T.A.Y.).

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Alzheimer’s Association (2014) Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement 10:e47–92.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Boros BD, Greathouse KM, Gentry EG, Curtis KA, Birchall EL, Gearing M, Herskowitz JH (2017) Dendritic spines provide cognitive resilience against Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 82:602–614. doi:10.1002/ana.25049 pmid:28921611
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Brennan GP, Jimenez-Mateos EM, McKiernan RC, Engel T, Tzivion G, Henshall DC (2013) Transgenic overexpression of 14-3-3 zeta protects hippocampus against endoplasmic reticulum stress and status epilepticus in vivo. PLoS One 8:e54491. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0054491 pmid:23359526
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Chen BS, Roche KW (2009) Growth factor-dependent trafficking of cerebellar NMDA receptors via protein kinase B/Akt phosphorylation of NR2C. Neuron 62:471–478. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.015 pmid:19477150
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Cheng IH, Scearce-Levie K, Legleiter J, Palop JJ, Gerstein H, Bien-Ly N, Puoliväli J, Lesné S, Ashe KH, Muchowski PJ, Mucke L (2007) Accelerating amyloid-beta fibrillization reduces oligomer levels and functional deficits in Alzheimer disease mouse models. J Biol Chem 282:23818–23828. doi:10.1074/jbc.M701078200 pmid:17548355
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Chin J, Palop JJ, Yu GQ, Kojima N, Masliah E, Mucke L (2004) Fyn kinase modulates synaptotoxicity, but not aberrant sprouting, in human amyloid precursor protein transgenic mice. J Neurosci 24:4692–4697. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0277-04.2004 pmid:15140940
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Chin J, Palop JJ, Puoliväli J, Massaro C, Bien-Ly N, Gerstein H, Scearce-Levie K, Masliah E, Mucke L (2005) Fyn kinase induces synaptic and cognitive impairments in a transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 25:9694–9703. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2980-05.2005 pmid:16237174
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    Chung C, Wu WH, Chen BS (2015) Identification of novel 14-3-3 residues that are critical for isoform-specific interaction with GluN2C to regulate N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor trafficking. J Biol Chem 290:23188–23200. doi:10.1074/jbc.M115.648436 pmid:26229101
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    Degawa T, Kawahata I, Izumi H, Shinoda Y, Fukunaga K (2021) T-type Ca(2+) channel enhancer SAK3 administration improves the BPSD-like behaviors in App(NL-G-F/NL-G-F) knock-in mice. J Pharmacol Sci 146:1–9. doi:10.1016/j.jphs.2021.02.006 pmid:33858649
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    DeKosky ST, Scheff SW (1990) Synapse loss in frontal cortex biopsies in Alzheimer’s disease: correlation with cognitive severity. Ann Neurol 27:457–464. doi:10.1002/ana.410270502 pmid:2360787
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Ding H, Fineberg NS, Gray M, Yacoubian TA (2013) α-Synuclein overexpression represses 14-3-3θ transcription. J Mol Neurosci 51:1000–1009. doi:10.1007/s12031-013-0086-5 pmid:23912650
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Ding H, Underwood R, Lavalley N, Yacoubian TA (2015) 14-3-3 inhibition promotes dopaminergic neuron loss and 14-3-3θ overexpression promotes recovery in the MPTP mouse model of Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience 307:73–82. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.08.042 pmid:26314634
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Dougherty MK, Morrison DK (2004) Unlocking the code of 14-3-3. J Cell Sci 117:1875–1884. doi:10.1242/jcs.01171 pmid:15090593
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    Foote M, Qiao H, Graham K, Wu Y, Zhou Y (2015) Inhibition of 14-3-3 proteins leads to schizophrenia-related behavioral phenotypes and synaptic defects in mice. Biol Psychiatry 78:386–395. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.015 pmid:25863357
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Gehler S, Shaw AE, Sarmiere PD, Bamburg JR, Letourneau PC (2004) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor regulation of retinal growth cone filopodial dynamics is mediated through actin depolymerizing factor/cofilin. J Neurosci 24:10741–10749. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2836-04.2004 pmid:15564592
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    Gohla A, Bokoch GM (2002) 14-3-3 regulates actin dynamics by stabilizing phosphorylated cofilin. Curr Biol 12:1704–1710. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01184-3 pmid:12361576
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Hardy J, Selkoe DJ (2002) The amyloid hypothesis of Alzheimer’s disease: progress and problems on the road to therapeutics. Science 297:353–356. doi:10.1126/science.1072994 pmid:12130773
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    Harris JA, Devidze N, Halabisky B, Lo I, Thwin MT, Yu GQ, Bredesen DE, Masliah E, Mucke L (2010) Many neuronal and behavioral impairments in transgenic mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease are independent of caspase cleavage of the amyloid precursor protein. J Neurosci 30:372–381. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5341-09.2010 pmid:20053918
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    Hashiguchi M, Sobue K, Paudel HK (2000) 14-3-3zeta is an effector of tau protein phosphorylation. J Biol Chem 275:25247–25254. doi:10.1074/jbc.M003738200 pmid:10840038
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    Hong S, Beja-Glasser VF, Nfonoyim BM, Frouin A, Li S, Ramakrishnan S, Merry KM, Shi Q, Rosenthal A, Barres BA, Lemere CA, Selkoe DJ, Stevens B (2016) Complement and microglia mediate early synapse loss in Alzheimer mouse models. Science 352:712–716. doi:10.1126/science.aad8373 pmid:27033548
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    Kaltenbach LS, et al. (2007) Huntingtin interacting proteins are genetic modifiers of neurodegeneration. PLoS Genet 3:e82. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.0030082 pmid:17500595
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Kaplan A, Morquette B, Kroner A, Leong S, Madwar C, Sanz R, Banerjee SL, Antel J, Bisson N, David S, Fournier AE (2017) Small-molecule stabilization of 14-3-3 protein-protein interactions stimulates axon regeneration. Neuron 93:1082–1093.e5. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.018 pmid:28279353
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Kobayashi DT, Chen KS (2005) Behavioral phenotypes of amyloid-based genetically modified mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease. Genes Brain Behav 4:173–196. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00124.x pmid:15810905
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Lavalley NJ, Slone SR, Ding H, West AB, Yacoubian TA (2016) 14-3-3 proteins regulate mutant LRRK2 kinase activity and neurite shortening. Hum Mol Genet 25:109–122. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddv453 pmid:26546614
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Lee CW, Han J, Bamburg JR, Han L, Lynn R, Zheng JQ (2009) Regulation of acetylcholine receptor clustering by ADF/cofilin-directed vesicular trafficking. Nat Neurosci 12:848–856. doi:10.1038/nn.2322 pmid:19483689
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Liao L, Cheng D, Wang J, Duong DM, Losik TG, Gearing M, Rees HD, Lah JJ, Levey AI, Peng J (2004) Proteomic characterization of postmortem amyloid plaques isolated by laser capture microdissection. J Biol Chem 279:37061–37068. doi:10.1074/jbc.M403672200 pmid:15220353
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    Mackintosh C (2004) Dynamic interactions between 14-3-3 proteins and phosphoproteins regulate diverse cellular processes. Biochem J 381:329–342. doi:10.1042/BJ20031332 pmid:15167810
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    Masters SC, Yang H, Datta SR, Greenberg ME, Fu H (2001) 14-3-3 inhibits Bad-induced cell death through interaction with serine-136. Mol Pharmacol 60:1325–1331. doi:10.1124/mol.60.6.1325 pmid:11723239
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    Masuda A, Kobayashi Y, Kogo N, Saito T, Saido TC, Itohara S (2016) Cognitive deficits in single App knock-in mouse models. Neurobiol Learn Mem 135:73–82. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2016.07.001 pmid:27377630
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Mateo I, Llorca J, Infante J, Rodríguez-Rodríguez E, Berciano J, Combarros O (2008a) Gene-gene interaction between 14-3-3 zeta and butyrylcholinesterase modulates Alzheimer’s disease risk. Eur J Neurol 15:219–222. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2008.02059.x pmid:18290843
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Mateo I, Sánchez-Juan P, Rodríguez-Rodríguez E, Infante J, Fernández-Viadero C, Peña N, Berciano J, Combarros  O (2008b) 14-3-3 zeta and tau genes interactively decrease Alzheimer’s disease risk. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 25:317–320. doi:10.1159/000119123 pmid:18319590
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    McFerrin MB, Chi X, Cutter G, Yacoubian TA (2017) Dysregulation of 14-3-3 proteins in neurodegenerative diseases with Lewy body or Alzheimer pathology. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 4:466–477. doi:10.1002/acn3.421 pmid:28695147
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Meilandt WJ, Cisse M, Ho K, Wu T, Esposito LA, Scearce-Levie K, Cheng IH, Yu GQ, Mucke L (2009) Neprilysin overexpression inhibits plaque formation but fails to reduce pathogenic Abeta oligomers and associated cognitive deficits in human amyloid precursor protein transgenic mice. J Neurosci 29:1977–1986. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2984-08.2009 pmid:19228952
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    Mucke L, Masliah E, Yu GQ, Mallory M, Rockenstein EM, Tatsuno G, Hu K, Kholodenko D, Johnson-Wood K, McConlogue L (2000) High-level neuronal expression of abeta 1-42 in wild-type human amyloid protein precursor transgenic mice: synaptotoxicity without plaque formation. J Neurosci 20:4050–4058. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-11-04050.2000 pmid:10818140
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    Murphy N, Bonner HP, Ward MW, Murphy BM, Prehn JH, Henshall DC (2008) Depletion of 14-3-3 zeta elicits endoplasmic reticulum stress and cell death, and increases vulnerability to kainate-induced injury in mouse hippocampal cultures. J Neurochem 106:978–988. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05447.x pmid:18466333
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Nichols RJ, Dzamko N, Morrice NA, Campbell DG, Deak M, Ordureau A, Macartney T, Tong Y, Shen J, Prescott AR, Alessi DR (2010) 14-3-3 binding to LRRK2 is disrupted by multiple Parkinson’s disease-associated mutations and regulates cytoplasmic localization. Biochem J 430:393–404. doi:10.1042/BJ20100483 pmid:20642453
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Ognibene E, Middei S, Daniele S, Adriani W, Ghirardi O, Caprioli A, Laviola G (2005) Aspects of spatial memory and behavioral disinhibition in Tg2576 transgenic mice as a model of Alzheimer’s disease. Behav Brain Res 156:225–232. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2004.05.028 pmid:15582108
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Ostrerova N, Petrucelli L, Farrer M, Mehta N, Choi P, Hardy J, Wolozin B (1999) alpha-Synuclein shares physical and functional homology with 14-3-3 proteins. J Neurosci 19:5782–5791. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-14-05782.1999
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    Palop JJ, Chin J, Roberson ED, Wang J, Thwin MT, Bien-Ly N, Yoo J, Ho KO, Yu GQ, Kreitzer A, Finkbeiner S, Noebels JL, Mucke L (2007) Aberrant excitatory neuronal activity and compensatory remodeling of inhibitory hippocampal circuits in mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuron 55:697–711. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.025 pmid:17785178
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Pennington KL, Chan TY, Torres MP, Andersen JL (2018) The dynamic and stress-adaptive signaling hub of 14-3-3: emerging mechanisms of regulation and context-dependent protein-protein interactions. Oncogene 37:5587–5604. doi:10.1038/s41388-018-0348-3 pmid:29915393
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    Pozueta J, Lefort R, Shelanski ML (2013) Synaptic changes in Alzheimer’s disease and its models. Neuroscience 251:51–65. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.05.050 pmid:22687952
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Qiao H, Foote M, Graham K, Wu Y, Zhou Y (2014) 14-3-3 proteins are required for hippocampal long-term potentiation and associative learning and memory. J Neurosci 34:4801–4808. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4393-13.2014 pmid:24695700
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    Ramser EM, Buck F, Schachner M, Tilling T (2010) Binding of alphaII spectrin to 14-3-3beta is involved in NCAM-dependent neurite outgrowth. Mol Cell Neurosci 45:66–74. doi:10.1016/j.mcn.2010.05.013 pmid:20598904
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Roberson ED, Scearce-Levie K, Palop JJ, Yan F, Cheng IH, Wu T, Gerstein H, Yu GQ, Mucke L (2007) Reducing endogenous tau ameliorates amyloid beta-induced deficits in an Alzheimer’s disease mouse model. Science 316:750–754. doi:10.1126/science.1141736 pmid:17478722
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    Sadik G, Tanaka T, Kato K, Yamamori H, Nessa BN, Morihara T, Takeda M (2009a) Phosphorylation of tau at Ser214 mediates its interaction with 14-3-3 protein: implications for the mechanism of tau aggregation. J Neurochem 108:33–43. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05716.x pmid:19014373
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Sadik G, Tanaka T, Kato K, Yanagi K, Kudo T, Takeda M (2009b) Differential interaction and aggregation of 3-repeat and 4-repeat tau isoforms with 14-3-3zeta protein. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 383:37–41. doi:10.1016/j.bbrc.2009.03.107 pmid:19324008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    Saito T, Matsuba Y, Mihira N, Takano J, Nilsson P, Itohara S, Iwata N, Saido TC (2014) Single App knock-in mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Neurosci 17:661–663. doi:10.1038/nn.3697 pmid:24728269
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Sakakibara Y, Sekiya M, Saito T, Saido TC, Iijima KM (2018) Cognitive and emotional alterations in App knock-in mouse models of Aβ amyloidosis. BMC Neurosci 19:46–46. doi:10.1186/s12868-018-0446-8 pmid:30055565
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    Schindler CK, Heverin M, Henshall DC (2006) Isoform- and subcellular fraction-specific differences in hippocampal 14-3-3 levels following experimentally evoked seizures and in human temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurochem 99:561–569. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.04153.x pmid:16981892
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Selkoe DJ (2011) Resolving controversies on the path to Alzheimer’s therapeutics. Nat Med 17:1060–1065. doi:10.1038/nm.2460 pmid:21900936
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Simsek-Duran F, Linden DJ, Lonart G (2004) Adapter protein 14-3-3 is required for a presynaptic form of LTP in the cerebellum. Nat Neurosci 7:1296–1298. doi:10.1038/nn1348 pmid:15543142
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Slone SR, Lesort M, Yacoubian TA (2011) 14-3-3theta protects against neurotoxicity in a cellular Parkinson’s disease model through inhibition of the apoptotic factor Bax. PLoS One 6:e21720. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0021720 pmid:21799745
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    St Martin JL, Klucken J, Outeiro TF, Nguyen P, Keller-McGandy C, Cantuti-Castelvetri I, Grammatopoulos TN, Standaert DG, Hyman BT, McLean PJ (2007) Dopaminergic neuron loss and up-regulation of chaperone protein mRNA induced by targeted over-expression of alpha-synuclein in mouse substantia nigra. J Neurochem 100:1449–1457. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.04310.x pmid:17241127
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Sumioka A, Nagaishi S, Yoshida T, Lin A, Miura M, Suzuki T (2005) Role of 14-3-3gamma in FE65-dependent gene transactivation mediated by the amyloid beta-protein precursor cytoplasmic fragment. J Biol Chem 280:42364–42374. doi:10.1074/jbc.M504278200 pmid:16223726
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    Terry RD, Masliah E, Salmon DP, Butters N, DeTeresa R, Hill R, Hansen LA, Katzman R (1991) Physical basis of cognitive alterations in Alzheimer’s disease: synapse loss is the major correlate of cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol 30:572–580. doi:10.1002/ana.410300410 pmid:1789684
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Tu S, Okamoto S, Lipton SA, Xu H (2014) Oligomeric Aβ-induced synaptic dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease. Mol Neurodegener 9:48. doi:10.1186/1750-1326-9-48 pmid:25394486
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Umahara T, Uchihara T, Tsuchiya K, Nakamura A, Iwamoto T, Ikeda K, Takasaki M (2004) 14-3-3 proteins and zeta isoform containing neurofibrillary tangles in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathol 108:279–286. doi:10.1007/s00401-004-0885-4 pmid:15235803
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    Underwood R, Gannon M, Pathak A, Kapa N, Chandra S, Klop A, Yacoubian TA (2021) 14-3-3 mitigates alpha-synuclein aggregation and toxicity in the in vivo preformed fibril model. Acta Neuropathol Commun 9:13. doi:10.1186/s40478-020-01110-5 pmid:33413679
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    VanGuilder HD, Yan H, Farley JA, Sonntag WE, Freeman WM (2010) Aging alters the expression of neurotransmission-regulating proteins in the hippocampal synaptoproteome. J Neurochem 113:1577–1588. pmid:20374424
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    VanGuilder HD, Farley JA, Yan H, Van Kirk CA, Mitschelen M, Sonntag WE, Freeman WM (2011) Hippocampal dysregulation of synaptic plasticity-associated proteins with age-related cognitive decline. Neurobiol Dis 43:201–212. doi:10.1016/j.nbd.2011.03.012 pmid:21440628
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    Wang B, Underwood R, Kamath A, Britain C, McFerrin MB, McLean PJ, Volpicelli-Daley LA, Whitaker RH, Placzek WJ, Becker K, Ma J, Yacoubian TA (2018) 14-3-3 proteins reduce cell-to-cell transfer and propagation of pathogenic α-synuclein. J Neurosci 38:8211–8232. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1134-18.2018 pmid:30093536
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    Workman ER, Haddick PC, Bush K, Dilly GA, Niere F, Zemelman BV, Raab-Graham KF (2015) Rapid antidepressants stimulate the decoupling of GABA(B) receptors from GIRK/Kir3 channels through increased protein stability of 14-3-3η. Mol Psychiatry 20:298–310. doi:10.1038/mp.2014.165 pmid:25560757
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    Xu G, Stevens SM Jr., Moore BD, McClung S, Borchelt DR (2013) Cytosolic proteins lose solubility as amyloid deposits in a transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer-type amyloidosis. Hum Mol Genet 22:2765–2774. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddt121 pmid:23512986
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Xu J, Kao SY, Lee FJ, Song W, Jin LW, Yankner BA (2002) Dopamine-dependent neurotoxicity of alpha-synuclein: a mechanism for selective neurodegeneration in Parkinson disease. Nat Med 8:600–606. doi:10.1038/nm0602-600 pmid:12042811
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    Yacoubian TA, Slone SR, Harrington AJ, Hamamichi S, Schieltz JM, Caldwell KA, Caldwell GA, Standaert DG (2010) Differential neuroprotective effects of 14-3-3 proteins in models of Parkinson’s disease. Cell Death Dis 1:e2. doi:10.1038/cddis.2009.4 pmid:21152247
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    Yoon BC, Zivraj KH, Strochlic L, Holt CE (2012) 14-3-3 proteins regulate retinal axon growth by modulating ADF/cofilin activity. Dev Neurobiol 72:600–614. doi:10.1002/dneu.20955 pmid:21780304
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    Zhang F, Gannon M, Chen Y, Yan S, Zhang S, Feng W, Tao J, Sha B, Liu Z, Saito T, Saido T, Keene CD, Jiao K, Roberson ED, Xu H, Wang Q (2020) β-Amyloid redirects norepinephrine signaling to activate the pathogenic GSK3β/tau cascade. Sci Transl Med 12:eaay6931.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Karl Herrup, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Zoltan Simandi, Jie Zhang.

The revised manuscript is significantly improved from the original submission. It remains disappointing that the in vivo studies were negative, but the explanation offered for the discordance is plausible. Only a few minor changes are needed before a final acceptance

1. The authors report that 14-3-3 AAV injection in hippocampus significantly shortened the lifespan of the J20 mice. It would help the reader interpret the findings if they were to provide the mortality data for the APP-KI and APP-KI/14-3-3 mice.

2. Based on the amyloid staining in Figure 4B, the amyloid plaque burden is obviously increased in hippocampus of APP KI/14-3-3 mice compared with APP KI mice. While, the variation in statistics analysis is very big, the authors should add the sample numbers or selected more representative images.

3. To better document the effects of the experimental manipulation on synaptic integrity, immunostaing for synapses should be provided. Any number of agents could be used, for example synapsin or synaptophysin/PSD95 double staining. This should be done in both J20 and APP KI mice brain.

Author Response

Dear Dr. Herrup,

Enclosed is a revised manuscript entitled “14-3-3θ does not protect against behavioral or pathological deficits in Alzheimer’s disease mouse models” that we would like to resubmit for publication in eNeuro. All authors have read and approved this manuscript.

We appreciate the comments by the reviewers of our revised manuscript. Overall, the reviewers found the work significantly improved from the initial submission and require a few minor changes before acceptance. A point-by-point response detailing changes to the manuscript is below:

1. To help clarify that the 14A3 mice did not have the same premature mortality that we saw with the J20 mice, we have included a survival curve for the 14A3 mice (Figure 3B). This should make it easier for the readers to interpret the data.

2. As noted by the reviewer, we do not see a difference in the beta amyloid burden between the APP-KI and APP-KI/14-3-3 mice, but the representative images that we had chosen for the hippocampus of these mice showed increased plaque load in the APP-KI/14-3-3θ image. We have selected better representative images to reflect our findings more accurately.

3. The reviewer asks us to provide better documentation of the effects of the experimental manipulation on synaptic integrity. We had previously quantified and provided representative images of synaptic density based on synapsin staining (Fig. 5), but to validate synaptic integrity, we have added a co-stain using a presynaptic marker VGLUT1 and a postsynaptic marker NMDAR 2A (Fig. 5).

We feel that our manuscript is stronger by addressing the reviewers’ concerns. We hope that our revisions have adequately addressed the reviewers’ concerns and meet the criteria for publication in eNeuro.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 9 (3)
eNeuro
Vol. 9, Issue 3
May/June 16
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
14-3-3θ Does Not Protect against Behavioral or Pathological Deficits in Alzheimer’s Disease Mouse Models
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
14-3-3θ Does Not Protect against Behavioral or Pathological Deficits in Alzheimer’s Disease Mouse Models
Mary Gannon, Bing Wang, Sara Anne Stringfellow, Stephan Quintin, Itzel Mendoza, Thanushri Srikantha, A. Claire Roberts, Takashi Saito, Takaomi C. Saido, Erik D. Roberson, Talene A. Yacoubian
eNeuro 13 June 2022, 9 (3) ENEURO.0368-21.2022; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0368-21.2022

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
14-3-3θ Does Not Protect against Behavioral or Pathological Deficits in Alzheimer’s Disease Mouse Models
Mary Gannon, Bing Wang, Sara Anne Stringfellow, Stephan Quintin, Itzel Mendoza, Thanushri Srikantha, A. Claire Roberts, Takashi Saito, Takaomi C. Saido, Erik D. Roberson, Talene A. Yacoubian
eNeuro 13 June 2022, 9 (3) ENEURO.0368-21.2022; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0368-21.2022
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
    • Author Response
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • β-amyloid
  • 14-3-3
  • Alzheimer’s disease
  • cortex
  • hippocampus
  • mouse model

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: Negative Results

  • In Vivo Injection of Anti-LGI1 Antibodies into the Rodent M1 Cortex and Hippocampus Is Ineffective in Inducing Seizures
  • Investigating the Role of Rhodopsin F45L Mutation in Mouse Rod Photoreceptor Signaling and Survival
  • Effect of the Matrix Metalloproteinase Inhibitor Doxycycline on Human Trace Fear Memory
Show more Research Article: Negative Results

Disorders of the Nervous System

  • Characterization of the Tau Interactome in Human Brain Reveals Isoform-Dependent Interaction with 14-3-3 Family Proteins
  • Impaired AMPARs translocation into dendritic spines with motor skill learning in the Fragile X mouse model
  • Glycolytic System in Axons Supplement Decreased ATP Levels after Axotomy of the Peripheral Nerve
Show more Disorders of the Nervous System

Subjects

  • Disorders of the Nervous System

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.