Abstract
The activity of primary auditory cortex (A1) neurons is modulated not only by sensory inputs but also by other task-related variables in associative learning. However, it is unclear how A1 neural activity changes dynamically in response to these variables during the learning process of associative memory tasks. Therefore, we developed an associative memory task using auditory stimuli in rats. In this task, rats were required to associate tone frequencies (high and low) with a choice of ports (right or left) to obtain a reward. The activity of A1 neurons in the rats during the learning process of the task was recorded. A1 neurons increased their firing rates either when the rats were presented with a high or low tone (frequency-selective cells) before they chose either the left or right port (choice-direction cells), or when they received a reward after choosing either the left or right port (reward-direction cells). Furthermore, the proportion of frequency-selective cells and reward-direction cells increased with task acquisition and reached the maximum level in the last stage of learning. These results suggest that A1 neurons have task- and learning-dependent selectivity toward sensory input and reward when auditory tones and behavioral responses are gradually associated during task training. This selective activity of A1 neurons may facilitate the formation of associations, leading to the consolidation of associative memory.
Significance Statement
The activity of the primary auditory cortex (A1) neurons is modulated not only by sensory inputs but also by other task-related variables in associative learning. However, it is unclear how A1 neural activity changes dynamically in response to these variables during the learning processes of associative memory tasks. This study showed that neurons in A1 have the task-related and learning-dependent selectivity toward sensory inputs and rewards when auditory tones and behavioral responses are gradually associated during the training of an associative memory task. We conclude that this selective activity of A1 neurons might facilitate the formation of associations, leading to the consolidation of associative memory.
Introduction
Associative learning between stimuli and behavioral responses induces modulation of neural activity in several brain regions. For successful associative learning, neural circuits integrate stimulus information with correct choices and rewards. This means that sensory regions, in addition to the hippocampal and motor regions, modulate their neural activity during learning processes (Weinberger, 2004). During learning with auditory stimuli, the primary auditory cortex (A1) neurons, each of which intrinsically responds to a specific tone frequency (tonotopic representation), become tuned to the frequency of the conditioned tone, and the proportion of these neurons increases with training (Bakin and Weinberger, 1990; Weinberger, 2004). Thus, A1 neurons have high plasticity, and their firing preference is modulated by auditory associative learning (Irvine, 2018).
The activity of A1 neurons is modulated not only by inputs, but also by other task-related variables (Caras and Sanes, 2017; Lin et al., 2019). Guo et al. (2019) reported that A1 neurons show choice-direction selectivity, whereas Bigelow et al. (2019) reported that motor movements modulate A1 neuronal activity. Furthermore, reward-related activity has been detected in A1 neurons (Brosch et al., 2011). Therefore, it is likely that A1 neurons represent important variables related to associative memories that form relations among auditory stimuli, behavioral choices, and rewards. However, it is unclear how A1 neural activity changes dynamically in response to these variables during the learning processes related to associative memory tasks. Hence, we developed an auditory associative memory task for rats (Takamiya et al., 2021), in which associative memory is gradually acquired over several days. In this task, the rats were required to associate tone frequencies (high and low) with the choice of ports (right and left) to obtain a reward. We recorded the firing activity of A1 neurons in the rats during the learning process of the task. We hypothesized that the number of task-related cells in A1 gradually increased; that is, their firing was gradually modulated during task acquisition.
Materials and Methods
Animals
Six male Wistar albino rats (Shimizu Laboratory Supplies) were individually housed and maintained on a laboratory light/dark cycle (lights on at 8:00 A.M. and off at 9:00 P.M.). The rats were placed under food restriction with ad libitum access to water. The animals were maintained at ∼80% of their baseline weight throughout the experiments. All experiments were conducted in accordance with the guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals provided by the Animal Research Committee of Doshisha University.
Apparatus
Behavioral training was performed in an operant chamber (23 × 11 × 35 cm; Ohara-Ika) with two ports in the front wall and a port in the back wall for a snout-poke response (Fig. 1A). The distance between the right and left ports was 70 mm. Each port was equipped with an LED light and an infrared sensor, which detected the nose-poke responses in the animal. A loudspeaker (15 cm in diameter) was placed 15 cm above the top of the chamber for the sound stimuli. A food dispenser delivered a 45 mg food pellet to a magazine located 1.5 cm above the floor and on the middle of the front wall. The chamber was enclosed in a soundproof box (Brain Science Idea). All the events were controlled using a personal computer (NEC).
Behavioral task
Associative memory task
Six rats were trained in an associative memory task with tones, in which they were required to associate tone frequencies (high and low) and port locations (right and left; Fig. 1A). All animals learned the task with the same rule; that is, they learned a high tone-right reward association and a low tone-left reward association (Fig. 1B). At the start of each trial, the light in port in the back wall was turned on, and a high or low tone was randomly presented when the rat poked its snout into the port. Subsequently, the light of the port in the back wall was turned off, and the lights in the right and left ports in the front wall were turned on. When the tone was high, it was correct for the rat to poke its snout into the right port. When the tone was low, the correct response was to choose the port on the left. Immediately after the choice response, the light from the ports in the front wall were turned off. A food pellet was delivered into the pellet magazine along with a buzzer noise when the rat made the correct choice (Fig. 1A), and the light in port in the back wall was turned on again to start the next trial. When the rat chose incorrect ports, a timeout was imposed, and the lighting on the back wall port to start the next trial was delayed for 5 s. When the rat did not choose either the right or left port for 10 s, the trial was canceled. The selection of high versus low tones was pseudorandom, long runs of more than four successions of one type of tone were not permitted, and there was an equivalent number of high-tone and low-tone trials in each session.
Rats were trained with 3 and 1 kHz tone stimuli until their accuracy reached >80%. After completion of the training, the rats underwent surgery for electrode implantation. A week after surgery, the rats were trained in the same task, but the tone stimuli were 10 and 6 kHz. They were trained until the choice accuracy was >80%, and we recorded all neural activity during the training process. Each training session consisted of 150–250 trials/d.
Passive tone condition
Three of the six rats were used for the passive tone condition after training the associative memory task. The rats were randomly exposed to 3 s of 10 and 6 kHz tones presented 50 times each at 20 s intervals in the operant chamber.
Surgery
The surgical procedures were almost identical to those reported in previous studies (Ohnuki et al., 2020; Osako et al., 2021; Takamiya et al., 2021). Rats were anesthetized with 2.5% isoflurane before surgery, which was maintained throughout the surgical procedure. We monitored the body temperature and depth of anesthesia of the rats as needed. An eye ointment was used to keep the eyes of the animals moistened throughout surgery. A craniotomy was performed over the right auditory cortex (distance relative to bregma: AP, −5.5 to −5.4 mm; ML, 6.7–7.2 mm; 1.0 mm below the brain surface), and custom-designed tetrodes attached to a microdrive were vertically implanted using a stereotactic manipulator. A stainless steel screw was placed over the cerebellum and served as the ground during the recording.
Recording
For each rat, eight tetrodes composed of four tungsten wires (12.5 μm; California Fine Wire) were used for extracellular recordings. Each tetrode was covered by a polyimide tube (A-M Systems), and tetrodes were placed 100 μm apart. The tip impedance was 200–1000 kΩ at a frequency of 1 kHz. The signals were recorded using a head stage (Intan Technologies) and a multichannel electrophysiology acquisition board (Open Ephys) at a sampling rate of 30 kHz and bandpass filtered between 0.3 and 6 kHz. The mean activity of all the tetrodes was used as a reference. During a week of postsurgical recovery, the tetrodes were advanced by 20 μm/d until the firing of some cells was observed. We did not move the tetrodes during the training of the rats to record the same cell population throughout the training process.
Histology
After the experiment, each rat was anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital and perfused with PBS and 4% paraformaldehyde. The brains were removed and postfixed in 4% paraformaldehyde, and 50 μm coronal sections of the brains were prepared to confirm the recording sites.
Data analysis
Spike-sorting analysis was performed using MATLAB (MathWorks). To detect single-neuron activity, spikes were manually clustered with MClust (A.D. Redish; https://redishlab.umn.edu/mclust) in MATLAB. Only those neurons that met the following criteria were included for further analyses: (1) spikes with sufficient isolation quality (isolation distance ≥ 15); and (2) spikes with reliable refractory periods of 2 ms (violations were <1% for all spikes).
Detecting task-related neurons
To evaluate the task-related neurons, we computed peristimulus time histograms using a 20 ms bin width and smoothed them by convolving spike trains with a 40-ms-wide Gaussian filter in the following four trial outcome conditions: high-right-correct (HRc), low-right-erroneous (LRe), low-left-correct (LLc), and high-left-erroneous (HLe; Fig. 1B). To examine the relationship between firing rate modulation and the development of behavioral epochs in the behavioral task, we used event-aligned spike histograms (EASHs). As the choice epoch durations varied for each trial, the median duration from the time when the stimulus was presented to the time when the rats chose either port was calculated, resulting in a median time of 2.38 s. The spike timing during choice epoch of each trial was linearly transformed into a median duration (Fig. 2A). For each neuron, we performed an area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (auROC) analysis (Green and Swets, 1966; Shiotani et al., 2020; Tanisumi et al., 2021). Starting from the baseline period of each condition (1 s of fixation before the start of the trial), the ROC value was calculated for each 100 ms bin. This bin was then stepped forward in 20 ms increments until 5 s after the start of the trial. We also performed auROC analysis to evaluate the selectivity of each neuron by comparing the firing rate of each trial condition (HRc vs LRe, HRc vs HLe, HRc vs LLc, LLc vs HLe, LLc vs LRe, and HLe vs LRe) in the same manner as described previously. We used permutation tests (1000 iterations) to determine the statistical significance (p < 0.01). auROC values were defined as 2 × (auROC curve − 0.5), ranging from −1 to 1. The sample size for the test was no fewer than eight trials. This is sufficient because the minimal sample size used in a previous study was four trials (Felsen and Mainen, 2008).
We defined several task-related neurons under the following conditions: frequency-selective cells modulated their firing rates in response to tone differences (high or low). These cells consist of high-frequency-selective and low-frequency-selective cells. High-frequency-selective cells were defined as follows: (1) auROC values of HRc versus LLc that were significant for five bins in a row from the start of the trial to the points of choice; (2) the auROC values of HRc versus LRe that were significant for the same period as in 1; and (3) the auROC values of HRc versus HLe that were not significant for the same period as in 1 (Fig. 2A).
We defined low-frequency-selective cells as follows: (1) auROC values of LLc versus HRc that were significant for five bins in a row from the start of the trial to the points of choice; (2) auROC values of LLc versus HLe that were significant for the same period as in 1; and 3 auROC values of LLc versus LRe that were not significant for the same period as in 1.
Sound-evoked cells
The auROC values of all conditions were calculated by comparing the firing rates of each period to the baseline of each condition that were significant for five bins in a row from the start of the trial up to 1 s after the response (Fig. 3A).
Choice-direction cells modulated their firing rates in response to the choice direction (right or left). These cells consisted of right choice-selective and left choice-selective cells. We defined right choice-selective cells as follows: (1) auROC values of HRc versus LLc that were significant for five bins in a row from the start of the trial to the points of choice; (2) auROC values of HRc versus HLe that were significant for the same period as in 1; and (3) auROC values of HRc versus LRe that were not significant for the same periods as in 1 (Fig. 4A).
We defined left choice-selective cells as follows: (1) auROC values of LLc versus HRc that were significant for five bins in a row from the start of the trial to the points of choice; (2) auROC values of LLc versus LRe that were significant for the same period as in 1; and (3) auROC values of LLc versus HLe that were not significant for the same period as in 1.
Reward direction-selective cells modulated their firing rates after the rats chose either of the correct ports (left or right). These cells consist of right-choice reward-selective cells and left-choice reward-selective cells. We defined right-choice reward-selective cells as follows: (1) auROC values of HRc versus LLc that were aligned by choice response time and were significant for five bins in a row from the time of choice to 3 s after the response; (2) auROC values of HRc versus LRe that were aligned by choice response time and were significant for the same periods as in 1; and (3) auROC values of HRc versus HLe that were aligned by choice response time and were significant for the same periods as in 1 (Fig. 5A).
We defined left-choice reward-selective cells as follows: (1) auROC values of LLc versus HRc that were aligned by choice response time and were significant for five bins in a row from the time of choice to 3 s after the response; (2) auROC values of LLc versus LRe that were aligned by the choice response time and were significant for the same periods as in 1; and (3) auROC values of LLc versus HLe that were aligned by choice response time and were significant for the same periods as in 1.
We detected a few neurons that responded to two or three events (frequency, choice direction, and reward direction), and each conjunctive cell was counted twice or thrice for each of the three response types (see Table 3).
Quantifying the degree of selectivity
We computed a selectivity index to evaluate the degree of selectivity, which was calculated as (H – L)/(H + L).” Here, H and L are the average firing rates of the HRc and LLc trials in the 100 ms around the selectivity peak, respectively.
Statistical analysis
Behavioral performance
One-way ANOVA and multiple-comparison tests were used to compare differences in task performance.
Detecting task-related cells
We used auROC analysis and permutation tests because it is usually difficult to predict the type (normal or non-normal) of the distribution of neuronal firing rates and populations of task-related neurons (Sakurai, 1994, 1996). Therefore, our past and recent studies (Sakurai, 1994, 1996; Shiotani et al., 2020; Tanisumi et al., 2021) used distribution-free nonparametric statistics (Siegel, 1956) or ROC analyses (Green and Swets, 1966) rather than parametric analyses (e.g., ANOVA) to analyze neuronal firing rates statistically and to define task-related activity. auROC analysis has an advantage in that we can compare differences between conditions quantitatively using auROC values, whereas we cannot compare them quantitatively with p-values in ANOVA.
Proportions of task-related cells
The χ2 test was used for group effects, and Fisher’s exact test was used to detect a significant difference between the two conditions. The tallies from Fisher’s exact test were corrected using the Holm correction.
Strength of selectivity
Selectivity index comparisons between two conditions and among three conditions were analyzed using Wilcoxon rank-sum and Kruskal–Wallis tests, respectively.
Results
Learning of the associative memory task and recording of neural activity from A1
Six rats were trained in the associative memory task with frequencies of 3 and 1 kHz as pretraining. The electrodes were implanted when the rats completed the pretraining. A week after the surgery for electrode implantation, the rats began to learn the task at frequencies of 10 and 6 kHz (learning session). The rats performed the task in three to five sessions (i.e., 3–5 d). Table 1 shows the percentage of correct responses from each animal during each session. Figure 1C shows examples of the task performance of the following three rats: one was a fast learner (rat 1) and the others were slow learners (rats 3 and 6) that showed different learning curves. We divided the learning sessions into three stages (Fig. 1C). The first session was defined as the “early stage” (mean behavioral accuracy = 59.80%, n = 6), the last session as the “late stage” (mean behavioral accuracy = 84.41%, n = 6), and the sessions before the last session as the “middle stage” (mean behavioral accuracy = 74.49%, n = 6). The middle stage could be the second, third, or fourth session, and the last stage could be the third, fourth, or fifth session according to the performance of each animal. The behavioral accuracies of these three stages differed significantly (Fig. 1D; one-way ANOVA: F(2,15) = 32.18, p = 3.75 × 10−6; Tukey’s HSD: early < middle, p = 6.98 × 10−4; middle < late, p = 0.02; and early < late, p = 2.56 × 10−6). The performance of the rats gradually increased during the first, second, and last stages (Fig. 1C). This gradual increase became apparent when the total number of learning sessions was divided into three stages according to the task performance of each animal.
We also compared the accuracies of the first (early stage), second (mean behavioral accuracy = 69.54%, n = 6), and third sessions (mean behavioral accuracy = 72.90%, n = 6), but the differences were not significant (Fig. 1E; one-way ANOVA: F(2,15) = 2.83, p = 0.09). We compared the performances of these sessions to reveal that the differences in neuronal activity depend on increases in task performance (learning) and session numbers (trials). Although task performance seemed to gradually increase during the first, second, and third sessions (Fig. 1E), the increase was not statistically significant because of the high variability in each session. However, as described above, the increase in task performance during the early, middle, and late stages was highly significant (Fig. 1D).
We recorded the spiking activity of A1 cells of the rats when they were learning the task (Fig. 1F, Table 2). We recorded 66, 52, and 44 neurons in the early, middle, and late stages, respectively, and 66, 63, and 49 neurons in the first (early), second, and third sessions, respectively. The tetrodes were not moved, and the constant-spike waveforms of the individual neurons were confirmed within a single session. The spike waveforms were also stable in repeated recordings on successive days (Fig. 1G). The stability was quantitatively judged to be reliable when the means of the spike energies for each spike cluster were within ±2 SDs on different days. The waveform consistency also indicates consistency among the neuron types (principal neurons vs interneurons) throughout the learning stages, as principal neurons and interneurons have different spike waveforms. These results suggested that the recordings were most likely from the same cell population. However, occasionally, some neurons disappeared or new neurons appeared between recording sessions (Li et al., 2017), resulting in a variation in the number of recorded neurons among the three stages of learning.
Frequency-selective activity of A1 neurons
We observed frequency-selective cells that increased or decreased their firing rates in response to tone differences (high or low), regardless of whether the choice was left or right, and correct or incorrect (Fig. 2A). These frequency-selective cells were observed in each session, but their proportions differed significantly among learning stages (Fig. 2B; χ2 test, χ2 (2) = 16.38, p < 0.01). We observed that 21.2% (14 of 66) of the neurons had frequency selectivity in the early stage, and the proportion of the neurons significantly increased to 59.1% (26 of 44 neurons) in the late stage (Fisher’s exact test with Holm correction; early < late, p = 9.43 × 10−5). However, frequency selectivity was observed only in 22% (14 of 63) and 34% (17 of 49) of the neurons in the second and third sessions, respectively, without significant differences among the first, second, and third sessions (Fig. 2C; χ2 test, χ2 (2) = 3.18, n.s.). We did not perform pairwise comparisons of the firing rates between sessions because of the small difference in the number of recorded cells in the sessions.
To assess the difference in the strength of selectivity among the learning stages, we calculated the degree of selectivity of each of the frequency-selective cells based on their firing rates in the high-tone and low-tone trials. There were no significant differences among the learning stages (Fig. 2D; Kruskal–Wallis test, p = 0.17) or among the first, second, and third sessions (Fig. 2E; Kruskal–Wallis test, p = 0.21).
Frequency-selective activity engagement in the task
To investigate whether the frequency selectivity of A1 cells was engaged in the task, we compared the firing activity between the associative memory task (27 cells) and the passive tone condition (32 cells) in three of six rats. The tetrodes were not moved, and the recorded cells were from the same cell populations as those used in the task training. In the passive tone condition, 25% (8 of 32) of the neurons showed sound-evoked firing that was modulated by tone presentation, regardless of the tone frequency (Fig. 3A). This type of frequency-insensitive firing was not observed in the associative memory task (Fig. 3B). Moreover, 44% (12 of 27) and 19% (6 of 32) of the neurons were frequency-selective cells (e.g., Fig. 2) in the task and passive tone conditions, respectively. The proportion of frequency-selective cells was significantly higher in the task than in the passive tone condition (Fig. 3C; Fisher’s exact test, p = 4.75 × 10−2). Furthermore, the frequency selectivity of the frequency-selective cells was higher in the task than in the passive tone condition (Fig. 3D; Wilcoxon rank-sum test, p = 7.54 × 10−4).
Choice direction-selective activity of A1 neurons
We observed choice direction-selective cells during the associative memory task. These cells modulated their firing rates in response to the choice direction (left or right), regardless of whether the tone was high or low and whether the choice was correct or incorrect (Fig. 4A). The proportions of these cells were 9% (6 of 66), 19% (10 of 52), and 18% (8 of 44) in the early, middle, and late stages, respectively, without a significant difference among the learning stages (Fig. 4B; χ2 test, χ2 (2) = 2.91, n.s.). The proportions of the choice direction-selective cells in the second and third sessions were 12.7% (8 of 63) and 10.2% (5 of 49), respectively, but there were no significant differences among them (Fig. 4C; χ2 test, χ2 (2) = 0.46, n.s.). We then compared the strength of the choice-direction selectivity, but there were no significant differences among the learning stages (Fig. 4D; Kruskal–Wallis test, p = 0.42) or the three sessions (Fig. 4E; Kruskal–Wallis test, p = 0.54).
We also observed several choice-direction cells that exhibited frequency selectivity (Table 3). The proportions of these conjunctive cells were 1.5% (1 of 66), 3.8% (2 of 52), and 9.1% (4 of 44) in the early, middle, and late stages, respectively.
Reward direction-selective activity of A1 neurons
Some neurons in A1 showed reward direction selectivity. Reward direction-selective cells modulated their firing rates after the rats chose either of the correct ports (left or right; Fig. 5A). Therefore, the reward-direction cells consist of both right- and left-choice reward cells.
As shown in Figure 5A, there were distinct effects in the immediate phasic response following reward delivery after choosing the left port (LLc). However, there was no significant difference in the phasic response between LLc and HRc, as shown by the auROC values at the bottom of Figure 5A. Therefore, the phasic response of this neuron did not exhibit directional selectivity. The auROC values also showed that the prolonged responses that followed the reward delivery after choosing the right port (HRc) contributed to the directional selectivity of this neuron.
Concerning the dichotomy between the early (phasic) and late (prolonged) responses, we analyzed the activity of each reward-direction cell in the phasic and prolonged periods separately. We observed that 10% of the reward-direction cells (2 of 20) showed both phasic and sensory responses after the reward buzzer and prolonged responses corresponding to reward and choice direction. The other reward-direction cells showed only a prolonged response corresponding to the reward and choice directions.
The proportion of these cells differed significantly among the learning stages (Fig. 5B; χ2 test, χ2 (2) = 8.46, p < 0.025). The proportions of these cells were 4.5% (3 of 66) and 3.8% (2 of 52) in the early and middle stages, respectively, but they showed a significant increase to 18% (8 of 44) in the late stage (Fisher’s exact test with Holm correction; middle < late, p = 0.04). However, in the second and third sessions, the proportions of these cells were 3.2% (2 of 63) and 10.2% (5 of 49), respectively, without significant differences among the three sessions (Fig. 5C; χ2 test, χ2 (2) = 2.80, n.s.).
We observed a few reward-direction cells that exhibited frequency selectivity (Table 3). The proportions of these conjunctive cells were 1.5% (1 of 66), 1.9% (1 of 52), and 9.1% (4 of 44) in the early, middle, and late stages, respectively. We also observed a few reward-direction cells that exhibited choice-direction selectivity. The proportions of these conjunctive cells were 1.5% (1 of 66), 1.9% (1 of 52), and 6.8% (3 of 44) in the early, middle, and late stages, respectively.
Discussion
In this study, we examined neuronal activity in A1 during the entire process of learning an auditory associative memory task. We found that A1 neurons showed selectivity for tone frequencies, choice directions, and reward directions. The proportion of frequency-selective and reward-direction cells increased during task acquisition. Therefore, A1 neurons showed learning-dependent modulation of activity when associations between auditory tones and behavioral responses were gradually induced by learning.
The proportion of frequency-selective cells was learning dependent and increased significantly in the late stages of the learning process (Fig. 2B). This increase in the proportion was not caused by a simple increase in sessions, but by a significant increase in correct performance because of learning. This conclusion was derived because the proportion of cells did not differ significantly among the first, second, and third sessions, wherein the correct performance on the task did not increase (Fig. 2C). Furthermore, the selectivity did not appear to be because of the difference between the correct and incorrect trials. This is because the auROC values of the HRc versus HLe and LLc versus LRe conditions were not significant in high-frequency-selective cells and low-frequency-selective cells, respectively. These comparisons indicated that the activity of the frequency-selective cells was modulated only by frequency and not by correct or erroneous trials. The strength of selectivity in these cells, however, did not differ significantly between the learning stages and sessions (Fig. 2D,E). The strength of the selectivity had to increase for each cell to become frequency selective. However, learning dependency was related to the increase in the number of frequency-selective cells and not to the increase in the strength of each selective cell. These results suggest that tone frequency selectivity in A1 increases during the learning process by increasing the number of frequency-selective cells but not by increasing the strength of the frequency selectivity of each cell. This modulation of proportions might be related to plastic changes in the tonotopic map related to the learning task, as the tetrodes were not moved throughout the successive days of training and the recording sites were not changed. The proportion of frequency-selective cells was also significantly different between the task and passive tone conditions (Fig. 3C), and the strength of the selectivity was significantly higher in the former than in the latter (Fig. 3D). Furthermore, some frequency-selective cells showed phasic modulation in brief periods following tone presentation, whereas others showed tonic modulation during tone presentation. These results suggest that the frequency selectivity of A1 neurons emerges strongly when the tone is engaged in a task with choices and rewards.
Some previous studies reported that the representation area of tone frequency in A1 increased with learning (Recanzone et al., 1993; Polley et al., 2006), and that enlargement occurred very quickly in several trials (Edeline and Weinberger, 1993; Fritz et al., 2003, 2005). However, in our results, the number of frequency-selective cells increased over a longer time span of several days. This might be because of the differences in the demands and difficulties of the tasks between previous studies and this study. In other words, the conditions may have been simpler in the former than in the latter, which could have been acquired slowly. This notion is supported by previous studies reporting that the sound-evoked activity of A1 neurons is modulated by task engagement (Atiani et al., 2009; De Franceschi and Barkat, 2021) and task structures (David et al., 2012).
A1 neurons also showed choice-direction selectivity, and the proportion of these cells did not differ significantly among learning stages (Fig. 4B). These cells might not be affected by the different acoustic properties of the different parts of the experimental chamber, as the distance between the right and left ports was only 70 mm and the distance from the loudspeaker was 550 mm for both ports. Furthermore, the selectivity of the choice-direction cells was not affected by the difference between the correct and erroneous trials. This is because the auROC values of the HRc versus LRe and LLc versus HLe conditions were not significant for right choice-selective and left choice-selective cells, respectively. This indicates that A1 neurons represent tone signal and behavioral choice information, and these representations are stable throughout the learning process.
The activity of A1 neurons in response to behavioral movements has been reported in previous studies (Nelson et al., 2013; Rummell et al., 2016). In addition, Guo et al. (2019) reported that A1 and posterior striatum neurons show choice-direction selectivity based on reward expectations. Choice-related activity has also been found in other primary sensory cortices, such as the visual cortex (Stringer et al., 2021) and olfactory cortex (Miura et al., 2012; Poo et al., 2022). In our previous study, choice direction-selective cells were also found in the hippocampal CA1 region in the same associative memory task, and the proportion of these cells did not differ significantly among the learning stages (Takamiya et al., 2021). These responses in previous studies were not necessarily associated with the formation of associations that led to the consolidation of associative memory. Therefore, it could be suggested that choice directions in associative learning are represented in several brain regions, including in the sensory cortices and hippocampus, and such representations have stable functions throughout the learning process.
We found that some A1 neurons showed reward-direction selectivity, and the proportion of reward-direction selectivity in the late stage of the learning process was significantly higher than that in the early stage (Fig. 5B). This suggests that A1 neurons represent reward directions as well as tone frequencies and choice directions, and the proportion of these cells increases when the rats learned the associative memory task. The increases in the reward-direction cells did not depend on the number of sessions, but on the accuracy of the task. This conclusion was drawn because the proportion of these cells did not significantly differ among the first, second, and third sessions, wherein there was no improvement in accuracy (Fig. 5C). The activity of the reward-direction cells was not affected by the buzzer sounds related to reward delivery or by sounds from the food delivery system or motor aspects of reward consumption, as these sounds were present in both the right-choice and left-choice reward trials. If these sounds from rewarded trials affected neuronal activity, the neurons should have shown the same firing rate modulations in both the right-choice and left-choice reward trials. The activity of these cells was also unaffected by an increase in the number of rewarded trials.
A few reward-directed cells showed both phasic and prolonged responses after reward delivery (Fig. 5A). The prolonged responses could correspond to reward intake, as they appeared from 0.5 s after the choice and reward, and continued for a short time. However, it should be noted that this prolonged activity was dependent on the choice direction and did not simply reflect the reward intake itself. In contrast, the phasic response corresponded to the sensory input of the reward buzzer and was not dependent on the reward direction. The phasic response could correspond to input from primary sensory pathways, whereas activity during the remaining prolonged time could correspond to signals from other cortical and/or other areas.
Our previous study (Takamiya et al., 2021) suggests that the activity of reward-direction cells in the hippocampal CA1 reflects positive feedback of the correct port choice to form an association between auditory stimuli and port directions. In that study, the proportion of reward-directed cells in the hippocampal CA1 increased in the middle stage and decreased in the late stage of the learning process. According to the two-stage model (Buzsáki, 1996, 2015), the hippocampus could be called the “fast learner,” as it rapidly encodes information via changes in synaptic strength during behavioral acquisition. The information is repeatedly replayed during slow-wave sleep and is gradually transferred to the neocortex, resulting in a hippocampal–neocortical interaction that could be referred to as a “slow-learning” interaction. Thus, the hippocampus and auditory cortex interact with each other, and the interaction between them plays an important role in memory formation and leads to consolidation (Rothschild et al., 2017). The activity of the reward-direction cells in the A1 region may reflect positive feedback, thereby forming and stabilizing auditory associative memories. The actual interaction between hippocampal and A1 neurons during the formation of auditory associative memory should be examined in the near future.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgment: We thank members of Laboratory of Neural Information at Doshisha University for helpful discussion.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This research was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science KAKENHI Grants 20J20997 (to S.T.), 20H00109, and 20H05020 (to Y.S.).
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.