Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Sensory and Motor Systems

Individualized Assays of Temporal Coding in the Ascending Human Auditory System

Agudemu Borjigin, Alexandra R. Hustedt-Mai and Hari M. Bharadwaj
eNeuro 22 February 2022, 9 (2) ENEURO.0378-21.2022; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0378-21.2022
Agudemu Borjigin
1Weldon School of Biomedical Engineering, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Agudemu Borjigin
Alexandra R. Hustedt-Mai
2Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN 47907
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hari M. Bharadwaj
1Weldon School of Biomedical Engineering, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN
2Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN 47907
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Hari M. Bharadwaj
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Stimulus paradigm and response from the EEG-TFS sensitivity measurement. A, The stimulus is a 1.5-s-long, 500-Hz pure tone that is amplitude modulated at 40.8 Hz. The red color represents the sound in the right ear, whereas the blue stands for the sound in the left ear. In the figure, the stimulus in the right ear leads in time till 0.98 s (indicated by the red segment of zoomed-out view of the stimulus), after which the ITD shifts in polarity, i.e., the stimulus in the left ear takes the lead. The ITD jump occurs when the stimulus amplitude is zero to minimize the involvement of monaural cues (pointed out by the dashed arrow). B, Averaged evoked response potential (ERP) from all trials across 42 subjects in “ITD = 540 μs” condition from Cz electrode. The red dashed line indicates where the ITD switched polarity, which resulted in N1 and P2 responses (denoted by red dots). C, ITC spectrogram of the EEG response, averaged across 42 subjects, with the colormap indicating the ITC. Robust ASSRs can be seen around the AM frequency of 40.8 Hz. There are also salient responses time locked to the stimulus onset, offset, and importantly, to the ITD jump. D, The average time course of the ITC for frequencies below 20 Hz is shown for each ITD jump condition. The response evoked by the shift in the ITD polarity increases monotonically with the size of the ITD jump, confirming that the response is parametrically modulated by TFS-based processing.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Measured versus predicted thresholds based on lapse rate. A, Measured versus predicted ITD detection thresholds. B, Measured versus predicted FM detection thresholds. The significant contribution of nonsensory factors is apparent, especially for the poorer performers.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Measured versus predicted AM thresholds based on lapse rate. The thresholds are the average detection thresholds of AM tones at 4 and 8 kHz. The significant contribution of nonsensory factors is apparent.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Individual EEG ITC (averaged under 20 Hz) values as a function of the jump size of the ITD. The ITC increases with the ITD for almost all subjects. Robust responses above noise floor are detected for most subjects for the “ITD = 180 μs” condition. Interestingly, individual differences present at 180 μs persist even at 540 μs despite the ITD jump being obviously perceptible and the response amplitude appearing to saturate.

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    FFR to the probe-forward-masker-probe stimulus sequence for an individual subject. The top row (green trace) represents the differential response across two stimulus polarities, whereas the bottom row (blue trace) represents the summed response across two stimulus polarities. The first boxed segments in both rows (red, dashed box, labeled d1 or s1) reflect the raw response to the probe tone, which is likely a mixture of neural and preneural responses (e.g., CM), whereas the second boxed segments in both rows (red, dashed box, labeled d2 or s2) is the adapted response after forward masking. For d2 and s2, the preneural (e.g., CM) component is expected to be intact, whereas the neural response is attenuated by forward masking (because of a very short 1-ms gap). The forward masker only partially suppresses the responses, suggesting a strong preneural contribution to d1 and s1. The weaker residuals obtained by subtraction, i.e., (d1 – d2) and (s1 – s2) are likely purely neural.

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    Frequency-domain representations of the d1 (A), d1–d2 (B), s1 (C), and s1–s2 (D) segments from Figure 5, but averaged across subjects. Forward masking partially attenuates both the 500-Hz component of d1 response, and the 1000-Hz component of the s1 response, suggesting that both responses reflect a mix of preneural and neural sources.

  • Figure 7.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 7.

    Model prediction of the ITD detection thresholds, based on the combination of lapse rate and slope (60- to 180-μs condition; A), or the combination of lapse rate and EEG latency (B). Please refer to Table 1 for the variance explained by each factor.

  • Figure 8.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 8.

    A sample of published reports of FM detection thresholds for comparison (Shower and Biddulph, 1931; Harris, 1952; Moore and Sek, 1996; He et al., 1998; Buss et al., 2004; Strelcyk and Dau, 2009; Ruggles et al., 2011; Grose and Mamo, 2012; Whiteford and Oxenham, 2015; Whiteford et al., 2017; Parthasarathy et al., 2020; Lelo de Larrea-Mancera et al., 2020). Error bar is 1 SD. The size of the dot represents the number of subjects (Whiteford and Oxenham (2015) has the most subjects; N = 100). Stimulus parameters such as stimulus level, carrier frequency, and modulation frequency in the cited studies are similar to those used in the current study, with slight differences (Strelcyk and Dau, 2009; Ruggles et al., 2011, used carrier at 750 Hz). Some threshold values are approximate from figures [e.g., mean and SD had to be estimated based on median and range in the box whisker plots in Whiteford and Oxenham (2015) and Whiteford et al. (2017)]. The mean and SD from the young and middle-aged group from Grose and Mamo (2012) were combined to generate a single data point. Some authors expressed the threshold in terms of ΔF/Fc, where ΔF is frequency deviation, and Fc is the carrier frequency. Moore and Sek (1996) used ΔF that was in two directions, i.e., peak-peak. Subjects from some studies were highly experienced in psychoacoustic tasks hence the thresholds were very low/good. Whiteford and Oxenham (2015) and Whiteford et al. (2017) obtained thresholds that fall in the lower end of the results of the current study from a very large number of subjects. This may be because their subjects were younger NH listeners and the stimuli were presented diotically and dichotically instead of monaurally.

  • Figure 9.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 9.

    A sample of published reports of ITD detection thresholds for comparison (Klumpp and Eady, 1956; Zwicker, 1956; Hershkowitz and Durlach, 1969; Henning, 1983; Dye, 1990; Bernstein and Trahiotis, 2002; Strelcyk and Dau, 2009; Hopkins and Moore, 2010; Grose and Mamo, 2010; Brughera et al., 2013). Error bar is 1 SD. The size of the dot represents the number of subjects (the current study has the most subjects; N = 36). Stimulus parameters such as level and carrier frequency in the cited studies are similar to those used in the current study, with slight differences [Strelcyk and Dau (2009) used carrier at 750 Hz]. Note that some threshold values were extracted approximately from figures rather than direct numerical reports. Some of the studies used stimuli with the leading ear switching from one side to the other (labeled “dynamic,” marked in green color), whereas others presented an ITD only in the target intervals, with the reference being the midline (labeled “static,” marked in blue color). Note that the values from Hershkowitz and Durlach (1969) and Brughera et al. (2013) were halved since the authors used ITD/2 in each interval. The mean and SD from young and middle-aged cohort from Grose and Mamo (2010) were combined to generate a single data point. Subjects from some studies were highly experienced in psychoacoustic tasks.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1

    Model prediction of the behavioral ITD detection thresholds, with factors including the nonsensory score, EEG latency, and EEG slope

    PredictorVariance explained
    Nonsensory score37.48%
    EEG latency10.03%
    EEG slope9.28%
    Explained56.79%
    Unexplained43.21%
    • The variations accounted for by the nonsensory score are more than three times as by either one of the two EEG metrics. Together, more than half the variance can be explained.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 9 (2)
eNeuro
Vol. 9, Issue 2
March/April 2022
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Individualized Assays of Temporal Coding in the Ascending Human Auditory System
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Individualized Assays of Temporal Coding in the Ascending Human Auditory System
Agudemu Borjigin, Alexandra R. Hustedt-Mai, Hari M. Bharadwaj
eNeuro 22 February 2022, 9 (2) ENEURO.0378-21.2022; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0378-21.2022

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Individualized Assays of Temporal Coding in the Ascending Human Auditory System
Agudemu Borjigin, Alexandra R. Hustedt-Mai, Hari M. Bharadwaj
eNeuro 22 February 2022, 9 (2) ENEURO.0378-21.2022; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0378-21.2022
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • electroencephalography
  • frequency modulation
  • interaural time difference
  • neural coding
  • nonsensory factors
  • temporal fine structure

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Parallel gene expression changes in ventral midbrain dopamine and GABA neurons during normal aging
  • Lactate receptor HCAR1 affects axonal development and contributes to lactate’s protection of axons and myelin in experimental neonatal hypoglycemia
  • Demyelination produces a shift in the population of cortical neurons that synapse with callosal oligodendrocyte progenitor cells
Show more Research Article: New Research

Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Task Modulation of Resting-State Functional Gradient Stability in Lifelong Premature Ejaculation: An fMRI Study
  • Synaptic Drive onto Inhibitory and Excitatory Principal Neurons of the Mouse Lateral Superior Olive
  • The Computational Bottleneck of Basal Ganglia Output (and What to Do About it)
Show more Sensory and Motor Systems

Subjects

  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.