Abstract
Operant chambers are widely used in animal research to study cognition, motivation, and learning processes. Paired with the rapidly developing technologies for brain imaging and manipulations of brain activity, operant conditioning chambers are a powerful tool for neuroscience research. The behavioral testing and imaging setups that are commercially available are often quite costly. Here, we present a custom-built operant chamber that can be constructed in a few days by an unexperienced user with relatively inexpensive, widely available materials. The advantages of our operant setup compared with other open-source and closed-source solutions are its relatively low cost, its support of complex behavioral tasks, its user-friendly setup, and its validated functionality with video imaging of behavior and calcium imaging using the UCLA Miniscope. Using this setup, we replicate our previously published findings showing that mice exposed to social defeat stress in adolescence have inhibitory control impairments in the Go/No-Go task when they reach adulthood. We also present calcium imaging data of medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) neuronal activity acquired during Go/No-Go testing in freely moving mice and show that neuronal population activity increases from day 1 to day 14 of the task. We propose that our operant chamber is a cheaper alternative to its commercially available counterparts and offers a better balance between versatility and user-friendly setup than other open-source alternatives.
Significance Statement
Operant conditioning chambers are widely used in neuroscience research, but commercially available operant setups are often costly. Here, we describe the construction of an open-source, low-cost operant conditioning setup which can be constructed in a few days using widely available materials. Using this setup, we replicate our previous findings showing that exposure to social defeat stress in adolescence impairs inhibitory control in adulthood. We also pair our setup with in vivo calcium imaging, and we show that we can record calcium activity from individual neurons in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) of freely moving mice while they carry out a cognitive task. Our custom-built operant conditioning setup is a useful tool to study the neurobiology of both adaptive and pathologic behavior.
Introduction
Operant conditioning chambers (“Skinner boxes”) are widely used in animal research to study learning and other cognitive processes in rodents, some birds, and nonhuman primates (Panlilio and Goldberg, 2007; Paglieri et al., 2014). An operant chamber allows for precisely controlled presentation of stimuli (auditory, visual, etc.) and for the measurement of responses to these stimuli (e.g., lever presses or nose pokes). By controlling the presentation of stimuli and their relationship to the responses of test subjects, experimenters can design behavioral tasks that assess motivation, learning (operant and classical conditioning) or other cognitive processes (e.g., attention, inhibitory control, etc.). Optogenetic tools and fluorescent biosensors for calcium imaging have made it possible to record and control brain activity during such behavioral tasks to identify neural circuits involved in overt behavior (Bernstein and Boyden, 2011; Knopfel, 2012). This combined approach is the basis of many preclinical studies investigating behavioral traits of model organisms that resemble symptoms observed in psychiatric disease in humans and has provided important insight into the neurobiological basis, potential treatment, and prevention strategies for psychiatric disorders (Tye and Deisseroth, 2012; Siciliano and Tye, 2019).
As the technologies employed in behavioral and brain-imaging studies have improved, their associated costs have increased significantly, the commercially available conditioning and imaging setups have become much more sophisticated but also much more expensive. Researchers often need to invest a lot in equipment which comes with additional expenses, proprietary software, high maintenance costs, costly updates, and additional features. Therefore, the interest in open-source platforms and tools is gaining increasing popularity (Freeman, 2015; White et al., 2019). Many scientific laboratories now produce their own behavioral platforms, brain imaging systems and data analytic tools to address specific research questions (Pineno, 2014; Freeman, 2015; Devarakonda et al., 2016; Kokras et al., 2017; O’Leary et al., 2018; Ribeiro et al., 2018; Erskine et al., 2019; Gurley, 2019; Buscher et al., 2020; Lee et al., 2020; Mazziotti et al., 2020; Tran et al., 2020; Vassilev et al., 2020; Matikainen-Ankney et al., 2021).
Here, we present the procedure for building an open-source Arduino-based operant chamber paired with an in vivo calcium imaging platform, the UCLA Miniscope (Cai et al., 2016; Aharoni and Hoogland, 2019; Aharoni et al., 2019), to assess medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) neuronal activity of freely moving mice while they perform the Go/No-Go task. Our operant setup is relatively low-cost, which makes it preferable to commercially available alternatives. Compared with open-source alternatives, it has a wider range of programmable stimuli (two auditory and two visual), it offers the code and setup of a complex behavioral task with two measures of impulsivity, as well as attention, and it has a relatively user-friendly setup with fewer electronic components and less programming. Finally, it has validated functionality with video imaging of behavior and calcium imaging.
We demonstrate the efficacy of our setup by reproducing our previous findings that exposure to accelerated social defeat stress (AcSD) in adolescence leads to impaired Go/No-Go performance in adulthood (Vassilev et al., 2021). The Go/No-Go task measures the ability to withhold a previously learned response which is no longer appropriate (i.e., inhibitory control or motor impulsivity), and neurons of the mPFC have been shown to encode information in this task (Pinto and Dan, 2015; Kamigaki and Dan, 2017; Li et al., 2020). Using our setup, we provide calcium imaging data showing changes in population activity of mPFC neurons while male mice are performing early versus late stages of the task. The Go/No-Go task has an equivalent in both rodents and humans, and impairments in the task, as well as abnormal mPFC activity, are characteristic traits of several psychiatric disorders with adolescent onset (Weisbrod et al., 2000; Kaiser et al., 2003; Reynolds et al., 2018). We propose that our setup is an affordable and versatile tool for preclinical and translational research into the neural substrates of both adaptive and pathologic behavior.
Materials and Methods
Animals
Experimental procedures were performed in accordance with the guidelines of the Canadian Council of Animal Care and approved by the McGill University and Douglas Hospital Animal Care Committee. All mice were housed in a temperature-controlled and humidity-controlled (21–22°C; 60%) colony room of the Neurophenotyping center of the Douglas Mental Health University Institute, on a 12/12 h light/dark cycle (light on at 8 A.M.). The mice had ad libitum access to food and water throughout the experiments (except during food restriction for Go/No-Go experiments). Mice were assigned randomly to each experimental condition.
Male C57BL/6J wild-type mice (n = 27) supplied by The Jackson Laboratory, arrived at the housing facilities on postnatal day (PND)24. These mice were housed in groups of three to four animals per cage before exposure to AcSD and single-housed after AcSD. Male CD-1 retired breeder mice (more than three months of age) obtained from Charles River Canada were used as aggressors in the AcSD paradigm.
The present study is aimed primarily at validating the custom-built behavioral and imaging setup, so only male mice were used as subjects. Although sex differences in the effects of adolescent social stress are very likely and of considerable interest, they are outside the scope of the present article.
AcSD paradigm
The AcSD procedure was conducted as described previously (Vassilev et al., 2021). The AcSD apparatus consisted of a transparent rat cage with two mouse housing compartments separated by a transparent and perforated central divider that allowed sensory but not physical contact between mice. Before the AcSD sessions, aggressor CD-1 mice were screened for aggression toward adolescent C57BL/6J mice. Then, they were housed on one side of the divider, where defeat sessions took place. Adolescent C57BL/6J mice were exposed to the aggressive CD-1 mice for two sessions per day for a total of 4 d (between PND25 and PND28; “defeat” group). Following defeat sessions, experimental mice were housed on the empty side of the cage divider until the next session. To ensure aggressive behavior toward adolescent mice, the CD-1 mouse aggressors were primed for aggression by a brief (30 s) exposure to an adult (PND65) C57BL/6 mouse before each AcSD session. Twenty-four hours after the last session of AcSD, C57BL/6J adolescent mice were assessed in the social interaction test (SIT) to measure their approach and/or avoidance behavior toward an unfamiliar CD-1 mouse. We calculated an interaction ratio (IR) based on the ratio between the time spent in the interaction zone (IZ) in the presence and the absence of an unfamiliar CD-1 mouse. Mice with ratios >/= 1 were classified as “resilient” and < 1 – as “susceptible.”
Calcium imaging
GRIN implantation and viral injection surgery
GRIN lens implantation and virus injection were done in the same surgical session. Mice were anaesthetized with isoflurane (5% induction and 1–2% maintenance) and placed on a stereotaxic frame. Eye ointment was applied to keep the eyes from drying. Intraperitoneal injections of saline and carprofen were administered to maintain hydration and to reduce inflammation. The skull was exposed by making a circular excision of the skin and connective tissue from between the eyes up to between the ears of the mouse. Two screws were secured to the back of the skull, one in each parietal bone, with the help of a microdrill and a screwdriver. A third whole (1.1 mm in diameter) was drilled in the skull just above the mPFC [centered at anterior-posterior (AP) +1.7 mm and medial-lateral (ML) +0.5 mm from bregma]. A 5-μl microsyringe was used to inject 0.3 μl of AAV9-Syn-GCaMP6f-WPRE virus (AddGene), over 6 min, in the mPFC [AP +1.7 mm, ML +0.5 mm, dorsal-ventral (DV) −1.8 mm]. After 10 min, the needle was extracted from the brain and a 1 × 4 mm GRIN lens (GoPhoton) held by a vacuum holder was inserted through the same hole in the skull that was used for the viral injection. The bottom of the GRIN lens was positioned at the same coordinates as the viral injection. We did not aspirate brain tissue because, in our experience, the hole made by the viral injection in the brain is sufficient to make way for the GRIN implantation. The GRIN lens was secured to the skull using glue and dental cement. The top of the lens was protected by placing a small protective cap made from the bottom of a PCR tube, secured by silicone (KwikSil, World Precision Instruments).
Three weeks later, mice were anaesthetized again, and an aluminum baseplate was secured to the head using dental cement to allow for the attachment of the miniature epifluorescent endoscope (UCLA Miniscope v3).
Recording and analysis of calcium transients
For the sake of consistency across sessions, the mice carried the miniscope during every session of the Go/No-Go task, from day 1 of training until the last day of testing. However, recording was done only during the testing phase (last 14 d). The recording of calcium transients was done using the open-source software of the UCLA Miniscope (Miniscope DAQ software 1.0, GitHub version August 30, 2020), at 20 frames per second. We recorded for the length of the whole 30 min Go/No-Go sessions. Cell detection and calcium trace extraction was achieved using the Suite2p package (Pachitariu et al., 2017). Signal drift across the session because of bleaching was corrected using a sliding window method (Pinto and Dan, 2015). To visualize neuronal activity in response to the presentation of “Go” and “No-Go” cues, we calculated the z-scored change in fluorescence in all detected neurons (regardless of significant or non-significant changes in activity) as well as cue-modulated neurons only and plotted the median fluorescence across trials. The custom MATLAB scripts are available on reasonable request.
Go/No-Go task
To test cognitive function, we used the Go/No-Go task as previously described (Reynolds et al., 2018; Cuesta et al., 2020; Vassilev et al., 2021). The only difference from the previously described procedure was adding an initial training stage (for 3 d) where the active nose-poke hole was constantly illuminated until the mouse nose-poked. In this stage, every response led to reward delivery and a 10-s time out where the nose-poke light went off and responses were not rewarded. This stage was used for fine-tuning of the miniscope recording parameters. First, mice underwent AcSD and SIT in adolescence; ∼30 d later, in adulthood, mice were food restricted to 85% of free-feeding weight and started training for the behavioral task. Chocolate-flavored dustless precision pellets (BioServ) were used as a reinforcer. The task required mice to nose poke in response to an illuminated Go cue within a limited amount of time or inhibit their response to this cue when presented together with an auditory No-Go cue. Nose poke responses to the Go cue (“hits”) resulted in the delivery of a food pellet, while the same response to the No-Go cue was considered a “commission error” and resulted in reward omission. Responses during a 3- to 9-s window before cue presentation were considered premature and led to reward omission and trial restart. We also calculated a correct response rate representing the proportion of all available rewards that were acquired by mice on both Go and No-Go trials combined.
Custom-built operant chambers and calcium imaging setup
All mice were trained and tested on the Go/No-Go task in custom operant chambers built on-site. The general layout of the setup is shown in Figure 1. A desktop PC connects to an Arduino UNO microcontroller which in turn controls the inputs and outputs of the operant chamber. The desktop PC also connects to the Data Acquisition Box (DAQ) of the UCLA Miniscope and to a USB camera for observation of mouse movement. Finally, the Arduino connects to the DAQ to initiate the recording of calcium activity through the miniscope.
Polycarbonate components of operant chambers
The base and walls of the operant chambers were constructed using polycarbonate sheets (Fig. 2). We used plastics cement to attach individual polycarbonate pieces together to form the walls of the chamber. To allow removal and maintenance, we used white tac (UHU) or waterproof tape (e.g., Gorilla) to attach electronic components. The base of the chamber consists of a floor and a front wall with three identical openings, one for the food pellet receptacle and two for the nose-poke holes (Fig. 2A–C). Importantly, we designed the size of the nose-poke holes to accommodate for the size of the mouse head carrying a miniscope. The back of the wall carries the food dispenser, the Arduino, and all electronic components (Fig. 2B,M; described in detail below). We used the services of a local plastics store to cut the polycarbonate sheets which comprise the main body of the operant chambers (Fig. 2L). The dimensions of the individual polycarbonate pieces are provided in Table 1. A V-shaped polycarbonate barrier with overhangs forms the back walls of the testing chamber and prevents mice from jumping out during behavioral testing (Fig. 2D–F). Finally, the outside walls were constructed as a separate part of the chamber which can be removed to allow for cleaning (Fig. 2G–I). These outer walls have two thin polycarbonate pieces on top that carry a commutator (DragonFly Inc.) attached to a pipette box allowing free movement of the mouse carrying the miniscope (Fig. 2J).
Extended Data Figure 2-1
Miniscope DAQ modification for external triggering of recording. A, B, A coaxial cable with an SMA connector is soldered to the input port (red square) right next to the miniscope input. C, A second coaxial cable is connected via SMA and a coaxial-to-SMA PCB. The free end is adapted to fit a breadboard by soldering two pins to the PCB. D, A top view of the operant chamber showing the miniscope DAQ (red square) connected to the miniscope via the coaxial cables (white) and the commutator. The placement of the USB camera is also visible. Download Figure 2-1, TIF file.
Extended Data Figure 2-2
Arduino script for Go/No-Go training stage 0, left nose-poke hole active. Download Figure 2-2, TXT file.
Extended Data Figure 2-3
Arduino script for Go/No-Go training stage 0, right nose-poke hole active. Download Figure 2-3, TXT file.
Extended Data Figure 2-4
Arduino script for Go/No-Go training stage 1, left nose-poke hole active. Download Figure 2-4, TXT file.
Extended Data Figure 2-5
Arduino script for Go/No-Go training stage 1, right nose-poke hole active. Download Figure 2-5, TXT file.
Extended Data Figure 2-6
Arduino script for Go/No-Go training stage 2, left nose-poke hole active. Download Figure 2-6, TXT file.
Extended Data Figure 2-7
Arduino script for Go/No-Go training stage 2, right nose-poke hole active. Download Figure 2-7, TXT file.
Extended Data Figure 2-8
Arduino script for Go/No-Go training stage 3, left nose-poke hole active. Download Figure 2-8, TXT file.
Extended Data Figure 2-9
Arduino script for Go/No-Go training stage 3, right nose-poke hole active. Download Figure 2-9, TXT file.
Extended Data Figure 2-10
Arduino script for Go/No-Go training stage 4, left nose-poke hole active. Download Figure 2-10, TXT file.
Extended Data Figure 2-11
Arduino script for Go/No-Go training stage 4, right nose-poke hole active. Download Figure 2-11, TXT file.
Electronic components, Arduino circuit, and programming
The connectivity of the electronic circuit is presented in Figure 2M. Infrared LEDs and sensors detect nose-poke hole entries. There is a green LED above each nose-poke hole, and a house light for the presentation of visual stimuli. We installed two speakers for the presentation of auditory stimuli. We modified a Med Associates rat food pellet dispenser to administer food rewards (dustless chocolate-flavored pellets) by 3D-prinitng the internal component to accommodate mouse-sized food pellets. This choice of pellet dispenser was based on our available materials. A mouse pellet dispenser can be used directly without modifications and open-source alternatives are also available (see Discussion). The pellet dispenser was attached to the front wall of the chamber using screws. The Arduino microcontroller integrated and controlled all inputs and outputs of the circuit, including the start of the calcium imaging recording in the beginning of each behavioral session. We used the in-built port of the DAQ to send a triggering signal from the Arduino at the start of the behavioral testing session. This required a modification of the DAQ circuit board to include a SMA cable connection (Digikey, ID: WM9477-ND; see Table 2) to the Arduino (Extended Data Fig. 2-1).
The Arduino scripts for each stage of the task are available as extended data (Extended Data Figures 2-2, 2-3, 2-4, 2-5, 2-6, 2-7, 2-8, 2-9, 2-10, 2-11). The scripts are named stage0 to stage4, the latter being the actual Go/No-Go task and all preceding stages being the training stages. All components necessary for the construction of one operant chamber are listed in Table 2.
Results
AcSD in adolescence leads to resilient and susceptible phenotypes in the SIT
Following exposure to AcSD in adolescence, susceptible mice had lower IRs (Fig. 3A) and spent less time in the IZ of the SIT (Fig. 3B) relative to controls. One-way ANOVAs revealed that resilient mice approached the social target similarly to controls (n = 23; IR: F(2,20) = 6.88, p = 0.0053, Holm–Sidak post hoc tests: control vs susceptible, t(20) = 3.04, p = 0.0128, control vs resilient, t(20) = 1.21, p = 0.2389; time spent in IZ: F(2,20) = 7.68, p = 0.0034, Holm–Sidak post hoc: control vs susceptible, t(20) = 2.85, p = 0.0197, control vs resilient, t(20) = 1.81, p = 0.0852). Accordingly, there was a significant difference between resilient, susceptible and control mice in the time spent in corner zones of the SIT arena (H(2) = 6.51, p = 0.0386; Fig. 3C). As we have reported previously (Vassilev et al., 2021), there were more resilient than susceptible mice following AcSD in adolescence, 60% and 40%, respectively (Fig. 3D). This is a characteristic difference between the adolescent AcSD paradigm and the adult 10-d CSDS paradigm which leads to a majority of mice showing a susceptible phenotype (Krishnan et al., 2007; Golden et al., 2011).
Exposure to AcSD in adolescence leads to impaired inhibitory control in adulthood as measured by the Go/No-Go task
In adulthood, approximately one month after the SIT (Fig. 3E), control and socially defeated mice were trained and tested on the Go/No-Go task (Fig. 3F). The Go/No-Go data were not divided according to the resilient and susceptible phenotypes observed in adolescence to increase the power of the statistical analysis and because we have previously shown that both groups perform similarly on the task (Vassilev et al., 2021). A 2 × 2 ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of time on the correct response rate (Fig. 3G) suggesting that, overall, mice improved their performance over time (n = 17, F(13,195) = 14.87, p < 0.0001). There was also a significant main effect of time on the proportion of commission errors (F(13,195) = 13.85, p < 0.0001; Fig. 3H), indicating that, overall, mice became better at correctly withholding their response on No-Go trials. Finally, there was a significant TIME × STRESS group interaction effect on the proportion of commission errors (F(13,195) = 1.92, p = 0.0303), suggesting that defeated mice showed inhibitory control impairment relative to controls, as they were less capable of withholding responding on No-Go trials. The main effect of time and the TIME × STRESS group interaction were non-significant for the proportion of hits (Fig. 3I), suggesting similar task engagement over time and between defeated mice and controls (TIME, F(4.40,66.00) = 1.18, p = 0.3269; interaction, F(13,195) = 1.67, p = 0.0689). These findings are in accordance with our previous findings showing cognitive impairments in adulthood following exposure to social defeat stress in adolescence in male mice (Vassilev et al., 2021).
Miniscope calcium imaging reveals increased mPFC neuronal activity in response to Go and No-Go cues
To test whether our setup could be used for calcium imaging in freely moving mice, we implemented the UCLA Miniscope v3 to record calcium activity of mPFC neurons expressing GCaMP6f while mice (n =4) were performing the Go/No-Go task. The behavioral performance of the mice on the task is shown in Extended Data Figure 4-1. Histologic data revealed that the viral injection and the placement of the GRIN lens were targeted toward the anterior cingulate (Cg1) and prelimbic (PrL) areas of the mPFC (Fig. 4A). We calculated the median population activity of neuronal cells across trials on days 1 and 14 of the Go/No-Go task, within 3 s before and 10 s after the presentation of the Go and No-Go cues (Fig. 4C–H). This time window was chosen to include the minimum pretrial period and the minimum time following cue presentation until the next trial begins (Fig. 4B). Data are presented as median z-scored change in fluorescence signal (ΔF/F). We found an increase in overall activity from day 1 to day 14 following cue presentation (Fig. 4C). The same increase in activity from day 1 to day 14 was observed when we calculated population activity separately for Go and No-Go cues (Fig. 4D), suggesting that the increase in overall activity applied to both Go and No-Go trials. Finally, we observed the same increase in population activity when calculated separately depending on correct and incorrect responses across trials (Go and No-Go trials combined; Fig. 4E), suggesting that the increase in overall activity was not associated with task performance. The increase in overall activity was preserved if we selected only neurons that were significantly modulated by cue presentation (based on a Wilcoxon rank-sum tests, with a cutoff significance value at p < 0.0001; Fig. 4F). The heat-maps of individual cue-modulated neurons show that, on day 1 (Fig. 4G), cue presentation is followed by a diverse neuronal response with variable strength and timing between neurons, while on day 14 (Fig. 4H), cue presentation is followed by a more pronounced and synchronized neuronal activity.
Extended Data Figure 4-1
Go-No/Go performance of the 4 mice during calcium imaging. A, Overall correct response rate. B, Percent hits. C, Percent commission errors. D, Percent premature responses. Download Figure 4-1, TIF file.
Discussion
Here, we present a low-cost, custom-built operant conditioning setup for the assessment of cognitive behavior in mice, paired with calcium imaging. Using our setup, we show that mice readily learn a Go/No-Go task, and that performance on this task is impaired in mice exposed to social defeat stress in adolescence, corroborating our previous findings (Vassilev et al., 2021). We demonstrate that our setup can be paired with a head-mounted miniscope for recording of calcium activity in freely moving mice by showing that neuronal activity in the mPFC increases from day 1 to day 14 of the task. We propose that our setup is a useful tool for the study of the neuronal activity correlates of both adaptive and pathologic behavior.
There are several advantages of our setup over other commercially available and open-source equivalents. It can be constructed with basic tools and materials within 2–3 d, and we include the Arduino code for the training and testing stages of the Go/No-Go task. Although our operant chamber uses only Arduino, which makes it easy to setup, it also offers the possibility to carry out complex tasks such as the Go/No-Go. This will allow users to recreate the behavioral testing setup quickly and easily, while also offering high flexibility and versatility. The chamber was constructed with an easily removable outer walls to facilitate cleaning, and we have found that the polycarbonate sheets are resistant to frequent cleaning with alcohol and other detergents. We are currently conducting parallel sessions with two operant chambers, as every miniscope requires its own desktop PC for optimal functionality. However, given enough available space, or other recording tools, our setup is easily scalable as it may be run even without direct connection to a PC. The Arduino can run on its own power supply and sessions can be restarted using the reset button. Finally, our operant setup allows for the reinforcement of nose-poking behavior, a widely used response in operant studies, while the mouse is carrying a head-mounted miniscope. Using our setup allows for adequate comparisons with other studies employing this type of reinforced response in mice, while adding the benefit of calcium imaging in freely moving animals. This is important considering documented differences between nose-poke and lever-pressing behavior in mice and rats (Schindler et al., 1993; Caine et al., 1999; Goeders et al., 2009). The fact that we replicated previous findings from our lab showing impaired performance on the Go/No-Go task in adulthood following social defeat stress in adolescence (Vassilev et al., 2021) demonstrates the reliability of our findings and the efficacy of our operant conditioning setup.
One aspect of our operant chamber that users might find difficult to reproduce is the adaptation of a Med Associates pellet dispenser for the delivery of food pellets. As an alternative to our approach, we suggest that the reader considers other custom-built pellet dispenser alternatives (Oh et al., 2017; O’Leary et al., 2018; Gurley, 2019). There are also several alternatives to our use of the Suite2p package for the analysis of calcium imaging data (Giovannucci et al., 2019; Pnevmatikakis, 2019; Cantu et al., 2020; Dong et al., 2021), and open-source alternatives to the UCLA Miniscope (Barbera et al., 2019; de Groot et al., 2020) and the DragonFly Inc. commutator (e.g., see miniscope.org; Barbera et al., 2020). One possible improvement on our setup would be the fabrication of a custom Arduino shield to improve the wiring stability of the electronic circuit. We did not include one with the current design to reduce the need for custom fabrication. Also, we used semi-transparent polycarbonate sheets for construction because of availability, and because we keep our boxes in sound-insulated cubicles but using opaque sheets for construction may reduce distractions for the mice during testing. Finally, we would like to point out that the design of our setup makes it possible to use it in conjunction with other techniques for imaging and manipulation of brain activity such as optogenetics and fiber photometry.
Creating new tools for the assessment of neural substrates of behavior is an indispensable part of neuroscience research. As accessible, open-source tools become more and more diverse, so do the possibilities to address specific, theory-driven questions. Our custom-built operant conditioning setup is a useful addition to the growing field of open-source science.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements: Figure illustrations were created using templates from BioRender.com. We thank Dr. Florence Allain for assisting with the box illustrations for the figures and Jose-Maria Restrepo for assisting with the miniscope analysis. We also thank David Munro for assisting with the 3D print of the pellet dispenser component.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse Grant R01DA037911 (to C.F.), Canadian Institutes for Health Research Grants MOP-74709 and MOP-431097 (to C.F.), and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Grant 04703 (to C.F.). A.H.P.-U. is supported by The National Council for Science and Technology/The Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnologia (CONACYT) from México and FRQNT-Merit scholarship program for foreign students (PBEEE).
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.