Abstract
Proper somatosensory circuit assembly is critical for processing somatosensory stimuli and for responding accordingly. In comparison to other sensory circuits (e.g., olfactory and visual), somatosensory circuits have unique anatomy and function. However, understanding of somatosensory circuit development lags far behind that of other sensory systems. For example, there are few identified transcription factors required for integration of interneurons into functional somatosensory circuits. Here, as a model, we examine one type of somatosensory interneuron, Even-skipped (Eve) expressing laterally placed interneurons (ELs) of the Drosophila larval nerve cord. Eve is a highly conserved, homeodomain transcription factor known to play a role in cell fate specification and neuronal axon guidance. Because marker genes are often functionally important in the cell types they define, we deleted eve expression specifically from EL interneurons. On the cell biological level, using single neuron labeling, we find eve plays several previously undescribed roles in refinement of neuron morphogenesis. Eve suppresses aberrant neurite branching, promotes axon elongation, and regulates dorsal-ventral dendrite position. On the circuit level, using optogenetics, calcium imaging, and behavioral analysis, we find eve expression is required in EL interneurons for the normal encoding of somatosensory stimuli and for normal mapping of outputs to behavior. We conclude that the eve gene product coordinately regulates multiple aspects of EL interneuron morphogenesis and is critically required to properly integrate EL interneurons into somatosensory circuits. Our data shed light on the genetic regulation of somatosensory circuit assembly.
Significance Statement
In general, even-skipped (eve) genes are considered neural cell fate determinants. Here, we show that eve gene expression is required for refinement of axon and dendrite morphogenesis and for proper functional integration of neurons into somatosensory circuits. Thus, eve coordinately regulates multiple terminal neuronal features of a class of Eve-expressing interneurons, raising the possibility that, in other neuronal contexts, eve genes regulate a similar suite of features. Our study pushes the understanding of eve beyond the level of neuron morphology to the levels of circuit physiology and whole animal behavior. It thereby provides an updated understanding of eve in development. Further, our data identify eve as a genetic entry point for future study of sensorimotor circuit assembly in Drosophila.
Introduction
Proper assembly of somatosensory circuits is critical for perception and movement (Zeilig et al., 2012). During somatosensory circuit development, interneurons wire up in precise patterns both with sensory neurons and with other CNS neurons (Kohsaka et al., 2017; Clark et al., 2018; D’Elia and Dasen, 2018). This is a multistep process involving, first, cell fate specification; then, axon outgrowth and dendrite morphogenesis; and finally, functional integration of neuronal inputs and outputs into the circuit. However, in comparison to other sensory systems (e.g., visual and olfactory), the assembly of the somatosensory system is still poorly understood (Meng and Heckscher, 2021).
The Drosophila larval nerve cord is an excellent system to study somatosensory circuit development. The organization of Drosophila and vertebrate somatosensory circuits is similar. For example, in both, a diversity of sensory neurons project axons to distinct dorsal-ventral regions (or laminae) in the CNS (Rexed, 1952; Zlatic et al., 2009). Genetically-defined subtypes of somatosensory interneurons synapse with specific sensory neurons, and those interneurons contribute to either local reflex circuits or send information to the brain (Heckscher et al., 2015; Lai et al., 2016; Wreden et al., 2017). Therefore, principles uncovered in studies of Drosophila have the potential to be broadly relevant to vertebrate somatosensory circuit development.
In Drosophila, several aspects of somatosensory circuit development are well understood. In the peripheral nervous system (PNS), specific transcription factors that regulate sensory neuron dendrite morphogenesis have been identified (Zlatic et al., 2003; Parrish et al., 2006; Hattori et al., 2007). In the CNS, early cell fate specification and the transcriptional regulation of axon guidance have been characterized (Santiago and Bashaw, 2014; Stratmann et al., 2019). However, most studies of neurons in the Drosophila CNS have focused on experimentally accessible motor neurons. Motor neurons are fundamentally distinct from somatosensory interneurons. Axons of motor neurons exit the CNS, and in general, dendrites of motor neurons do not get direct synaptic input from sensory neurons (Couton et al., 2015). In Drosophila, there remain large gaps in our understanding of the genetic control of somatosensory interneuron morphogenesis, specifically in control of dendrite morphology and circuit integration. Furthermore, it is unclear to what extent multiple terminal features, such as axon and dendrite morphology, are coordinately controlled by single transcription factors (Kurmangaliyev et al., 2019).
In this study, as a model, we focus on Drosophila larval laterally placed interneurons (ELs). ELs are named for their expression of the transcription factor, Even-skipped (Eve) and their lateral cell body position. EL interneurons all process somatosensory stimuli and are necessary for normal behavior (Heckscher et al., 2015). We focus on ELs because in comparison to other Drosophila somatosensory interneurons, the developmental origins of ELs are known, and reagents to label ELs in embryos and larvae are available (Fujioka et al., 1999). To study the transcriptional control of EL development, we took a candidate gene approach focusing on the role of eve. eve encodes a conserved homeobox transcription factor. eve or its homologs are expressed in neurons in animals across phyla (Ferrier et al., 2001; Heckscher et al., 2015). We reasoned that eve is likely to play an important role in ELs because cell type-specific transcription factor genes often are important in the cell types they define. Further, in general, eve and its homologs play roles in cell fate specification and axon guidance (Doe et al., 1988; Landgraf et al., 1999; Moran-Rivard et al., 2001; Esmaeili et al., 2002; Fujioka et al., 2003; Pym et al., 2006; Zarin et al., 2014; Juárez-Morales et al., 2016). Here, we show eve gene expression is required for multiple aspects of terminal neuronal development and for proper functional integration of ELs into sensorimotor circuits. Specifically, eve regulates neurite branching, axon extension, dendrite positioning, formation of functional inputs, and mapping of functional outputs, as well as whole animal behavior. Thus, we uncover several, previously undescribed roles for eve in neuronal physiology, and we identify eve as a transcriptional regulator of Drosophila somatosensory circuit assembly.
Materials and Methods
Fly genetics
Standard methods were used to propagate fly stocks. Unless otherwise noted, larvae were raised at 25°C and fed yeast paste containing water and yeast (5:3 ratio by weight). For optogenetics experiments, yeast paste containing 100-μl all trans retinal (ATR) was used. For a list of stocks used in this study see Table 1.
Species sex
In these experiments, embryos and early-stage larvae were used. At these developmental stages, flies have no distinguishing sexual characteristics. Thus, all experiments were conducted in a manner that was blind to sex.
Immunostaining
We used standard methods (Meng et al., 2019, 2020). Larval brains were pulled at ambient temperature within a 7-min window. Brains were fixed in freshly prepared 1× phosphate buffered saline containing 4% formaldehyde for 7–10 min. Many primary antibodies were obtained from Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, created by the NICHD of the NIH and maintained at The University of Iowa, Department of Biology. See Table 1 for primary antibodies. Secondary antibodies were from Jackson ImmunoResearch and were used according to manufacturer’s instructions. Images were acquired on a Zeiss 800 confocal microscope with 40× objective. Embryos were staged for imaging based on standard morphologic criteria.
Four ways to label ELs interneurons
In this study, we use four ways to label ELs (Table 2).
Eve antibody staining
In wild-type, anti-Eve labels motor neurons and EL interneurons. In EL eve mutants, anti-Eve staining is lost from the ELs. Notably, in EL eve mutants in trans to a hypomorphic eve allele, eve(5), which generates a truncated Eve protein, anti-Eve can be used to track ELs.
EL-GAL4
In wild-type, EL-GAL4 expresses in ELs from the middle of embryogenesis until the middle of larval development. However, in EL eve mutants, EL-GAL4 drives only a pulse of gene expression. Thus, in ELs, Eve is dispensable for initiation of EL-GAL4 expression, but is required to maintain EL-GAL4 expression.
EL-GAL4 with a permanent labeling cassette
To positively mark ELs in an EL eve mutant background using EL-GAL4, we added a FLP-based permanent labeling cassette (UAS-FLP, actin-FRT-Stop-FRT-GAL4).
11F02-GAL4
In wild-type, 11F02-GAL4 drives expression in the late-born subset of ELs, as well as a few other uncharacterized neurons (Heckscher et al., 2014). 11F02-GAL4 expression is unchanged in EL eve mutants.
Larval behavior
L1 larvae were collected from 0 to 6 h after hatching. All larvae were rinsed in mesh chambers under dH20 until they were freed from food debris. Collected larvae were placed on 5-mm-thick 2% agarose gels set in 5.5-cm-wide Petri dishes at least 15 min before recording. All recordings were done at 22–25°C. Five to 30 larvae were allowed to freely crawl on a gel per recording. For pinching assay, pinches were delivered to the side of the larval body wall with a pair of forceps. A larva was considered to be rolling if the trachea disappeared under one side of the larva body and reappeared on the other side. Note that this criterion for a roll is not based on larval speed. For hunching assay, a vibration was delivered to the larvae using the speaker and sound described below. Hunches were considered to be a shortening of the distance between the head and body center associated with a pause in crawling, but not a head turn. For left-right asymmetry and crawling speed assays, larvae were recorded in our custom-built behavior rig (Wreden et al., 2017). Images were acquired at 10 frames per second. For optogenetic experiments, all larvae were recorded 48–54 h after hatching. A total of 10–100 larvae were placed on a rectangular 5-mm-thick agarose gel at least 15 min before recording. Recordings began with a 30-s period of no stimulus followed by a 30-s period of stimulating light and ending with a final 30-s period of no stimulus. For further setup details, please refer to Wreden et al. (2017).
Larvae were tracked using FIMTracker (Risse et al., 2017) software using default parameters including five spline points, this includes centroid, head, and tail points, plus two other points, one halfway between head and centroid and one halfway between the tail and centroid. Larvae were discarded from analysis if (1) the larva’s track was <300 frames; (2) the larva was improperly masked; or (3) if occasionally the larva collided with another larva and it was not rectified by thresholding. Tracks were then analyzed using custom scripts (Heckscher et al., 2015) on MATLAB (MathWorks). Speeds were calculated using the center spline point over a 10 frame (1-s) window. Statistical tests were performed using Prism 9 software (GraphPad).
Calcium imaging
For calcium imaging experiments, all larvae were within 6 h of age on the day of recording and collected 48–54 h after hatching. Larvae expressing GCaMP6m (UAS-FLP, act5C-FRT.stop-GAL4; ΔEL, Df(2R)eve/+; EL-GAL4/UASGCaMP6m or UAS-FLP, act5C-FRT.stop-GAL4; ΔEL, Df(2R)eve/ΔEL, Df(2R)eve; EL-GAL4/UAS-GCaMP6m) were rinsed with water and placed ventral side up on agarose pads with a 22 × 22 mm coverslip placed on top. Pads were made by pouring 3% agarose into a well. Recordings began with a 30-s period of no stimulus followed by a 30-s period of sound stimulus and ending with a final 30-s period of no stimulus. A Visaton FR12, 4 Ohm speaker (5 inches in diameter) and a PYLE PCA2 stereo power amplifier was used to project sound. For further details, refer to Wreden et al., 2017. Images were acquired on a Zeiss LSM 800 confocal microscope using 0.1–0.2% 488-nm laser power with the pinhole entirely open. Images were acquired at three frames per second using a 10× (0.3 NA) or 20× (0.8 NA) objective. The calcium signal was continuously collected before, during, and after the stimulus. Extracting changes in GCaMP6m fluorescence amplitude was done using Fiji as in Wreden et al. (2017). A region of interest (ROI) that included the larval nerve cord was manually drawn, and the mean fluorescence within the ROI was calculated for each time point.
Single neuron labeling
We labeled single neurons using MultiColor FLP Out (MCFO; Nern et al., 2015). MCFO stochastically labels the membranes with epitopes in cells within a GAL4 pattern. MCFO uses FLP recombinase, which we also use to label larval EL neurons in EL eve mutants (Table 2). So, we cannot use these reagents together. Here, for MCFO, we used 11F02-GAL4, which is expressed in late-born ELs and three poorly characterized non-EL interneurons directly adjacent to ELs (Heckscher et al., 2014; Wreden et al., 2017). For 11F02-GAL4 to be useful, we needed to distinguish between ELs and non-ELs in wild-type and EL eve mutant backgrounds. In both backgrounds, we performed single neuron MCFO labeling. In both backgrounds, we can identify three non-EL neurons based on morphology, which we call: 11F02d, 11F02m, and 11F02z. We conclude that loss of Eve from ELs does not have gross nonautonomous effects on non-EL neurons. We also conclude that we can distinguish between EL and non-EL neurons in EL eve mutants. To obtain single cell clones, adult flies were allowed to lay eggs for 24 h on apple juice caps. Caps were heat shocked in a water bath at 37–39°C for 15–30 min and incubated at 25°C for 4–5 h. First instar larvae were dissected. Their brains were stained for HA, Flag, and V5 epitopes to visualize single cell clones. Larvae were also stained for Eve protein to confirm the identity of each single cell clone as an EL, and to assign segmental identity to each clone. Clones were imaged on a Zeiss 880 or 800 confocal microscope. We generated >115 single-cell clones. Among these clones, we saw each neuronal morphology in a minimum of two independently derived clones (i.e., larvae). Each clone was analyzed in dorsal and posterior views.
Results
In EL eve mutants, EL interneurons lack eve expression
The objective of this study was to determine the role of the transcription factor Eve in EL somatosensory interneurons. eve is an essential gene in Drosophila. It is expressed in stripes in early embryos, in other developing tissues, as well as in a subset of motor neurons and EL interneurons (Frasch et al., 1987; Heckscher et al., 2014; Fig. 1A). In this study, we removed eve gene expression specifically from ELs using “EL eve mutants,” which were previously generated by Fujioka and colleagues (Fujioka et al., 2003). Briefly, a genomic construct containing all eve regulatory elements was generated (Fig. 1C). This construct rescues eve null mutants to viability, and eve is expressed at normal levels and in normal locations (Fujioka et al., 2003). Fujioka and colleagues deleted from the construct a regulatory region sufficient to drive eve expression in ELs, thereby making a “ΔEL” construct (Fig. 1C; Fujioka et al., 1999, 2003). When eve null alleles are rescued with ΔEL, eve is expressed at normal levels and locations everywhere except ELs interneurons, which completely lack Eve protein (Fig. 1B; Fujioka et al., 2003).
eve regulates multiple aspects of interneuron morphogenesis
eve or its homologs (e.g., evx genes) often regulate neuronal axonal pathfinding. For example, in mouse spinal cord, evx1 is required for V0v interneurons to send axons across the midline (Moran-Rivard et al., 2001). In Drosophila or Caenorhabditis elegans motor neurons, loss of eve genes results in dramatic changes in axon trajectories (Esmaeili et al., 2002; Fujioka et al., 2003). In contrast, in Zebrafish, evx1/2(–) V0v interneurons have normal axonal morphology (Juárez-Morales et al., 2016). These data demonstrate that eve plays a context dependent role in axons and raise the question to what extent eve regulates EL interneuron axon morphogenesis in Drosophila larvae.
In embryos, we characterized EL morphology by expressing a membrane localized GFP. In stage 15 embryos, in both control and EL eve mutants, EL axons cross the midline (Fig. 2A–C). In stage 16 embryos, we additionally stained with anti-Fas2 to visualize three fascicles in the neuropile—lateral, intermediate, and medial (Landgraf et al., 2003). In control, after midline crossing, most ELs grow laterally until reaching the intermediate fascicle and extend toward the anterior. In a subset of segments, ELs extend to the anterior along both intermediate and medial fascicles (Fig. 2D,F). In EL eve mutants, however, a larger proportion of ELs extend along both the intermediate and medial fascicles. Further, in some segments, ELs project along the intermediate and lateral fascicles, a phenotype never observed in controls (Fig. 2E,F). In stage 17 embryos (the final stage of embryogenesis), in control, ELs project so far to the anterior that they reach the next segment, making a ladder like pattern (Fig. 2G). In contrast, in EL eve mutants, only a small proportion of Eve(–) ELs reach the next segment (Fig. 2H,I).
At larval stages, ELs have mature morphologies and are incorporated into functioning somatosensory circuits. We labeled individual ELs, using MCFO, which allows us to determine which part of the EL, axon, dendrite, or both, is impacted by loss of eve expression (Nern et al., 2015). We restricted our analysis to local, late-born ELs, because they were most abundantly labeled in our dataset (Fig. 3A). First, we focused our analysis along medial-lateral and anterior-posterior axes because in Figure 2 we detected defects along these axes. We used Sholl analysis to count the number of intersections between an EL neurite and concentric circles with increasing radii (1-μm intervals) centered on the soma (Sholl, 1953). We plotted the median, minimum, and maximum number of intersections versus circle radius to generate a description of arborization. In control, local, late-born ELs have two sets of branching neurites off the main neurite. Ipsilaterally (same side) dendritic neurites are found ∼20 μm from the soma. Contralaterally (opposite side) axonal neurites branch ∼50 μm from the soma (Fig. 3B,D). In EL eve mutants, neurites of Eve(–) ELs branched excessively off the main neurite near the soma and in the midline, which is never seen in wild type (Fig. 3C, arrowheads). Furthermore, Eve(–) EL axons are less extended, branching at ∼40 μm from the soma (Fig. 3C, arrow). These changes are reflected in a statistically significant difference in the distribution of intersections (Fig. 3F).
Next, in the same set of larval clones as shown in Figure 3, we characterized ELs along the dorsal-ventral axis. For somatosensory interneuron dendrites, positioning along the dorsal-ventral axis is particularly important because different sensory neurons axons project to different dorsal-ventral domains within the CNS (Landgraf et al., 2003). Therefore, dorsal-ventral positioning of somatosensory interneuron dendrites is expected to dictate the types of sensory input received by a given interneuron. Currently, the molecular control of dorsal-ventral dendrite positioning of somatosensory interneurons is extremely poorly understood. Here, we counted the number of dorsally and ventrally projecting ipsilateral branches (i.e., dendrite). In control, nearly all Eve(+) ELs have dorsally, but not ventrally, projecting dendrites (Fig. 4A,C,D). In EL eve mutants, there is a significant reduction in dorsally projecting dendrites and a significant increase in ventrally projecting dendrites for Eve(–) ELs (Fig. 4B–D).
Finally, we asked whether loss of eve expression transforms the ELs into another interneuron type. To do so, we took both anatomic and genetic approaches. Anatomically, we mined the Drosophila larval connectome (Ohyama et al., 2015). We looked at each local neuron in the first abdominal segment, but found no neurons with morphology matching that of Eve(–) EL interneurons. Genetically, we looked for large-scale changes in gene expression, which often accompany cell fate changes. We surveyed 19 genes that were candidates to be regulated by eve in ELs. These candidates included genes regulated by eve in non-EL cell types (En, FasII, HB9, Islet; see Fig. 5A,B), genes expressed in ELs (Castor, Eagle, Zfh2, Knot, Kruppel, Nab, Pdm2, Seven-up; see Fig. 5C,D), and genes with putative Eve binding sites (Antp, AbdA/Ubx, AbdB, Cut, Dpn, Repo; Broihier and Skeath, 2002; Pym et al., 2006). There are no obvious changes in expression for any of these genes in Eve(–) ELs. Taken together, these data support the conclusion that, in ELs, Eve is not repressing alternative interneuron fate.
In summary, we find that, in ELs, eve expression is not required for the initial step of EL morphogenesis—axon midline crossing. However, eve is required for later stages of EL morphogenesis, refining morphology in all three axes, medial-lateral, anterior-posterior, and dorsal-ventral. Further, eve is required in both axons and dendrites. Thus, in EL interneurons, eve expression coordinately regulates multiple aspects of morphogenesis. Finally, we find no evidence to support the idea that, in ELs, Eve represses alternative neuronal fates because Eve(–) EL interneurons do not resemble any other wild-type interneuron.
Loss of eve expression disrupts EL somatosensory stimulus encoding
In wild-type animals, EL interneurons encode somatosensory stimuli. Late-born ELs get direct synaptic input from proprioceptive sensory neurons and indirect proprioceptive input via Jaam interneurons (Fig. 6A; Heckscher et al., 2015). Early-born ELs get direct synaptic input from vibration sensitive (chordotonal) sensory neurons and indirect chordotonal input via Basin interneurons (Fig. 6B; Wreden et al., 2017). The observation that Eve(–) EL dendrites are mispositioned raised the possibility that eve is required for ELs to properly encode somatosensory stimuli (Fig. 4). We test this idea here.
Mispositioned dendrites in Eve(–) EL interneurons likely disrupt the ability of ELs to form connections with their normal sensory neuron input partners (Fig. 5C,E). Alternatively, ELs could still form synapses with input partners via a compensatory mechanism (Fig. 6F). It is essential to consider this alternative because a recent study revealed the existence of compensatory mechanisms in Drosophila sensory neuron-to-somatosensory interneuron wiring (Valdes-Aleman et al., 2021). Specifically, genetically mispositioned sensory neurons form synapses with many of their normal interneuron partners, which grow abnormally to reach the mispositioned sensory neurons (Fig. 6D). In control and EL eve mutants, we characterized the position of sensory neurons by labeling vibration and proprioceptive sensory neurons with iav-GAL4 and 0165-GAL4 driving membrane GFP but find no differences in sensory neuron axon position (Fig. 6G,H).
Next, we tested the idea that eve expression is required for normal EL somatosensory stimulus encoding. Drosophila larvae are optically clear so we used calcium signals in intact larvae to monitor EL activity. Specifically, a larva expressing GCaMP6m in ELs was placed on a bed of agarose with a coverslip on top (Fig. 7A). In this preparation, larvae do not crawl, but they move, which stimulates proprioceptors, and sound can be used to stimulate vibration-sensitive chordotonal sensory neurons (Wreden et al., 2017). In a single recording, we imaged baseline EL fluorescence, EL activity while the sound is played, and return to baseline. In control, there are small amplitude changes in fluorescence intensity in ELs during periods of self-movement and large amplitude changes in response to vibration (Fig. 6B,D). In EL eve mutants, there is only bleaching of the calcium signal (Fig. 7C,E).
We conclude that in the absence of eve expression, EL interneurons no longer normally encode somatosensory stimuli.
Upon loss of eve expression, EL outputs are anatomically and functionally reconfigured
EL interneurons are necessary for normal Drosophila larval behavior (Heckscher et al., 2015). Late-born ELs contribute to a circuit that regulates left-right symmetrical crawling, and early-born ELs contribute to a circuit that triggers fast escape rolling (Wreden et al., 2017). The observation that Eve(–) EL axons are less extended raised the possibility that EL output circuits could be reconfigured (Fig. 3).
The specific output partners of EL interneurons are not well characterized, and so we determined the location of Eve(–) EL output synapses. In control and EL eve mutants, we visualized presynaptic nerve termini, using expression of a V5 epitope tagged Brunchpilot (BRP-V5) protein (Wagh et al., 2006; Chen et al., 2014). In control, BRP signal is concentrated in large puncta in the central intermediate and medial region of the neuropile (Fig. 8A’). However, in EL eve mutants, BRP signal is found diffusely throughout the neuropile (Fig. 8B’). To quantify this, we used Fas2 expression to subdivide the neuropile into medial (M), intermediate (I), and lateral (L) zones, and scored for BRP signal in each zone (Fig. 8C,D). We find a significant increase in lateral signal in EL eve mutants compared with control. These data suggest that Eve(–) EL interneurons output synapses are relocated.
We used functional approaches to determine the extent to which relocation of Eve(–) EL output synapses change circuit function. We recorded spontaneously occurring crawling in control and in EL eve mutant larvae (Fig. 9). We calculated crawling speed as centroid position over time and left-right body symmetry using the angles between centroid and head position and centroid and tail position. EL eve mutants crawl significantly slower than control (Fig. 9A–C) with left-right asymmetrical body posture (Fig. 9D,F–H). We had previously recorded the behavior of larvae that entirely lack EL interneurons, and here we noticed that, in comparison, EL eve mutant larvae have more severe crawling defects (Heckscher et al., 2015). This is quantified as significantly greater left-right asymmetry in larvae with Eve(–) ELs than in larvae without ELs (Fig. 9D–H). The discrepancy between severity of the phenotypes could be explained by the idea that mispositioned Eve(–) EL output synapses make new, functional connections, which could provide a dominant negative effect at the circuit level (see Discussion).
The crawling defects provide strong functional evidence that eve expression is required in EL interneurons for normal somatosensory circuit function (Fig. 9). However, we demonstrated that in Eve(–) ELs sensory encoding is disrupted (Fig. 7). Therefore, it is possible that dysfunction in sensory encoding could explain the observed behavioral deficits. And so, we assayed the behavioral response to optogenetic stimulation of Eve(–) ELs . Optogenetics uses light to activate neurons and bypasses the need for any input from upstream neurons. We reasoned that optogenetic stimulation of Eve(–) ELs might elicit a novel behavior, consistent with the idea that Eve(–) EL output synapses are remapped. In control and EL eve mutants, we expressed the optogenetic effector, CsChrimson, and fed larvae all trans retinal (ATR), a cofactor needed for CsChrimson light sensitivity. As expected, in response to light, any larva not fed ATR fails to respond (Fig. 10D,E). Also, as expected, control larvae fed ATR roll on light exposure, which is quantified as fast centroid movement over time (Fig. 10A,D,F). In EL eve mutants fed ATR, in response to light, larvae preform a novel behavior—a dorsal body bend. This is quantified as a small increase in centroid movement over time (Fig. 10B,E,G). The body bend is an extreme C shape where the head and tail nearly touch, and to our knowledge, does not occur naturally in Drosophila melanogaster. Dorsal bending is robust, displayed by nearly every EL eve mutant larva. This demonstrates that Eve(–) EL output synapses are functional and that their activation leads to a novel behavior.
Activation of Eve(–) EL interneurons could lead to novel behavior for two reasons. First, EL output synapses could be remapped to new, downstream targets and away from roll inducing circuits. Alternatively, circuits mediating rolling and other sensorimotor transformations could be generally dysfunctional. This possibility needed to be tested because homeodomain proteins, including Eve, can have nonautonomous effects (Lee et al., 2019). To probe the function of rolling circuits, we provided a body wall pinch, which served as a mechanical and noxious stimulus, to both control and EL eve mutants (Hwang et al., 2007). In both genotypes, larvae roll in response to pinch (Fig. 10H). Thus, in EL eve mutant larvae, roll inducing circuits are not disrupted. Further, we found that both EL eve mutant and control larvae respond to vibration with a hunch (Fig. 10I). Thus, in EL eve mutant larvae, somatosensory stimuli can be perceived and transformed into appropriate motor output, and somatosensory circuits are not generally dysfunctional. We conclude that, in Eve(–) ELs, output synapses are no longer mapped to roll inducing circuits but instead to novel targets.
Discussion
In this study, we removed the conserved, homeodomain transcription factor, Eve, from Drosophila Eve-expressing ELs (Fig. 1). We found that eve regulates multiple aspects of EL interneuron morphogenesis (Figs. 2-4), and that eve is required for the proper integration of ELs into somatosensory circuits at both the input (Figs. 6, 7) and output (Figs. 8-10) levels.
Previously undescribed roles for eve in neuronal morphogenesis
Here, we show that eve expression is required for positioning EL interneuron neurites in all three axes (i.e., medial-lateral, anterior-posterior, and dorsal-ventral; Figs. 2-4). In Drosophila, each axis is patterned by a separate ligand/receptor signaling system (Zlatic et al., 2009; Evans and Bashaw, 2010; Emerson et al., 2013). However, how individual interneurons read and interpret each signal is not well understood. Our data suggest eve is important for ELs to simultaneously read and/or interpret multiple ligand gradients simultaneously.
Generally, eve is considered a cell fate determinant. For example, in mouse V0v interneurons, evx1 represses expression of en1, a marker of V1 interneuron identities (Moran-Rivard et al., 2001). In V0v interneurons that lack evx1, en1 expression is derepressed and take on V1-like axonal projections. Similar fate changes are seen in Drosophila and C. elegans motor neurons when eve is disrupted (Landgraf et al., 1999; Esmaeili et al., 2002). Our data are more consistent with the idea that eve plays a role in the refinement of EL morphogenesis. In support for the morphogenetic refinement model is, first, in wild-type, there are no neurons with morphology that matches the morphology of Eve(–) ELs, as would be expected by a cell fate switching model. Second, there are no obvious large-scale changes in gene expression, which are typically associated with cell fate changes (Fig. 5). Third, eve expression in ELs is not playing a role in initial morphogenesis (see next paragraph for more).
Both Eve(–) and Eve(+) ELs cross the midline at embryonic stage 15 (Fig. 2A–C). Thus, eve expression is either dispensable for initial morphogenesis, or in EL eve mutants there is an undetectable pulse of early eve expression in ELs. But, we and others have not found Eve protein expression in ELs in EL eve mutants at any stage of development (Fig. 1A,B; Fujioka et al., 2003). In later stage embryos and larvae, we observe morphologic defects in Eve(–) ELs (Figs. 2D–H, 3, 4). This raises the possibility that, in general, eve genes may play a later role in morphogenesis. This is consistent with the observation that, in mouse V0v interneurons, there is early evx1 expression and later evx2 expression. However, the later role of evx2 is unknown (Moran-Rivard et al., 2001).
In general, eve genes are known to regulate axon morphogenesis (Doe et al., 1988; Landgraf et al., 1999; Moran-Rivard et al., 2001; Esmaeili et al., 2002; Fujioka et al., 2003). In this study, we show that late-born Eve(–) ELs have axonal defects (Figs. 3, 4). Notably, the role of eve in dendrite morphogenesis is extremely poorly characterized. The distinction between dendrite and axon is important because these two compartments carry out different functions. Further, in Drosophila, interneuron axons and dendrites are structurally different. Dendrites are often highly branched, and lack mitochondria and postsynaptic machinery. Whereas, axon terminals (boutons) are full of mitochondria, pools of synaptic vesicles, microtubules, and vesicle release sites. Each part of the arbor (axon or dendrite) can be independently controlled by different transcription factors (Kurmangaliyev et al., 2019). For example, in Drosophila sensory neurons, the transcription factors Knot and Cut specifically regulate dendrite morphogenesis, but not axonal morphology (Jinushi-Nakao et al., 2007). Thus, in Drosophila, axon and dendrite morphology can be controlled as independent modules. Here, we show that in addition to regulating axon morphology, eve regulates dorsal-ventral dendrite positioning (Fig. 4). eve expression is also required for dendrite morphogenesis in RP motor neurons (Fujioka et al., 2003). Taken together, our data show that eve coordinately regulates multiple aspects of neuronal morphogenesis, and that coordinate control may be a widely-occurring role for neuronal eve expression.
eve expression in ELs plays a role in somatosensory circuit assembly
Neuronal circuits are functional units of the nervous system. Sensorimotor circuits, specifically, transform somatosensory stimuli into motor output. Therefore, functional assays are required for the study of somatosensory circuit assembly. However, because the circuit context of individual interneurons is not well characterized, often researchers rely on anatomic assays to infer changes at the circuit level. One reason an anatomic approach can be flawed is the existence of compensatory mechanisms that allow for relatively normal circuit wiring despite changes in neuron morphology (Landgraf et al., 1999; Meng and Heckscher, 2021; Valdes-Aleman et al., 2021). In this study, we link defects in neuronal morphology to changes in circuit function, thereby explicitly demonstrating the role of eve expression in somatosensory circuit assembly.
The role of eve expression in formation of functional input synapses
We show that eve is required for somatosensory stimulus encoding by ELs (Fig. 7). Based on known connectivity of ELs with other neurons (Fig. 6A), we infer that in ELs, eve is required for the formation of at least four types of functional input synapses: those from vibration (chordotonal) sensory neurons to early-born ELs, from vibration-sensitive interneurons (Basins) to early-born ELs, from proprioceptive sensory neurons to late-born ELs, and from proprioceptive-sensitive interneurons (Jaams) to late-born ELs. The likely cell biological underpinning, at least for late-born ELs, is that axons from input sensory neurons (Fig. 6G,H) are not in close enough proximity to make synaptic contact with Eve(–) ELs (Fig. 4). Because of technical limitations, we could not visualize dendrite morphology of early-born ELs.
In the Drosophila nerve cord, there is unidirectional compensatory growth from interneurons to genetically misplaced sensory neurons (Valdes-Aleman et al., 2021; Fig. 6C,D). Thus, Drosophila sensory neuron-to-interneuron wiring can be robust to morphologic alterations to circuit components. The observation (Fig. 6G,H) that sensory neurons do not grow to reach mispositioned Eve(–) EL dendrites raises two possibilities: (1) in this system, compensatory growth is unidirectional (i.e., interneurons grow to misplaced sensory neurons, but not vice versa); and (2) alternatively, compensatory growth is bidirectional, however, eve expression is required for this process. Future experiments will be needed to distinguish between these models.
The role of eve in positioning output synapses
Our data show Eve(–) EL output synapses are functional, but remapped. Spontaneously-occurring crawling behavior is disrupted in EL eve mutants (Fig. 7), and that this disruption is significantly worse than in larvae which lack EL neurons altogether. This could be explained by requirement for ELs during early circuit development (e.g., acting as a scaffold for normal axonal pathfinding for other neurons). Alternatively, mature Eve(–) ELs could exert a dominant negative effect at the level of circuit function. We favor the latter idea because it is consistent with the optogenetic experiments presented in Figure 9, and the anatomic data presented in Figure 7. Figure 7 shows that in controls, EL output synapses are excluded from many zones of the neuropile including the dorsal lateral zone, which houses the dendrites of dorsally-projecting motor neurons. However, Eve(–) ELs are likely to form output synapses in this region. This specific re-distribution of output synapses is notable because it raises the possibility that Eve(–) ELs output synapses (ELs are excitatory) could be directly re-mapped to dorsal motor neurons. Such a re-mapping could explain the novel behavioral phenotype, dorsal body bending phenotype seen on optogenetic activation of Eve(–) ELs in Figure 9. Regardless of the exact anatomic changes, the data in Figure 9 show that output synapses of Eve(–) ELs are functional, but are functionally re-mapped to new output circuits.
In conclusion, we have provided an updated understanding of the role of eve expression in neurons. Our data provide understanding of the role of neuronal eve at the levels of circuit physiology and animal behavior. Further they provide insight into the genetic logic of somatosensory circuit assembly, demonstrating that multiple terminal neuronal features can be coordinately regulated by the activity of a single postmitotic transcription factor. Finally, our data raises new questions about the role of eve expression in other neuron types and enable future experimental inquiry into somatosensory circuit assembly in Drosophila.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements: We thank Chip Ferguson and Paschalis Kratsios (University of Chicago), Chris Doe (University of Oregon), and members of the Heckscher lab for comments that improved this manuscript. We also thank Richard Baines (Manchester) for sharing Eve binding site data.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health Grant R01-NS105748 and the University of Chicago, Department of Molecular Genetics and Cell Biology Start-up funds (to E.S.H.).
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.