Abstract
Postnatal CNS development is fine-tuned to drive the functional needs of succeeding life stages; accordingly, the emergence of sensory and motor functions, behavioral patterns and cognitive abilities relies on a complex interplay of signaling pathways. Strictly regulated Ca2+ signaling mediated by L-type channels (LTCCs) is crucial in neural circuit development and aberrant increases in neuronal LTCC activity are linked to neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders. In the amygdala, a brain region that integrates signals associated with aversive and rewarding stimuli, LTCCs contribute to NMDA-independent long-term potentiation (LTP) and are required for the consolidation and extinction of fear memory. In vitro studies have elucidated distinct electrophysiological and synaptic properties characterizing the transition from immature to functionally mature basolateral subdivision of the amygdala (BLA) principal neurons. Further, acute increase of LTCC activity selectively regulates excitability and spontaneous synaptic activity in immature BLA neurons, suggesting an age-dependent regulation of BLA circuitry by LTCCs. This study aimed to elucidate whether early life alterations in LTCC activity subsequently affect synaptic strength and amygdala-dependent behaviors in early adulthood. In vivo intra-amygdala injection of an LTCC agonist at a critical period of postnatal neurodevelopment in male rat pups was used to examine synaptic plasticity of BLA excitatory inputs, expression of immediate early genes (IEGs) and glutamate receptors, as well as anxiety and social affiliation behaviors at a juvenile age. Results indicate that enhanced LTCC activity in immature BLA principal neurons trigger persistent changes in the developmental trajectory to modify membrane properties and synaptic LTP at later stages, concomitant with alterations in amygdala-related behavioral patterns.
Significance Statement
Early-life alterations to normal developmental processes are known to change the morphology and functioning of the amygdala in the adult brain and are correlated to emotional and behavioral disorders. However, the precise cellular and molecular determinants of key neurodevelopmental stages predisposing the amygdala circuitry to pathologic states remain to be fully defined. Our study provides evidence that Ca2+ signaling through L-type calcium channels (LTCCs) during a vulnerable period of postnatal development is linked to long-lasting synaptic remodeling associated to altered behavioral outcomes in early adulthood. The results expand our knowledge concerning the possible pathophysiologies underlying psychiatric disorders of neurodevelopmental origin.
Introduction
Early brain development involves the integration of numerous cellular and molecular mechanisms that must occur with temporal and spatial precision (Dehorter et al., 2012). Calcium signaling through neuronal L-type calcium channels (LTCCs) Cav1.2 and Cav1.3 critically influences neurite outgrowth, myelination, synaptogenesis and fine-tuning of neuronal networks (Cheli et al., 2016; Oh et al., 2016; Kamijo et al., 2018). At the molecular level, Ca2+ entry through LTCCs initiates signal transduction cascades that activate immediate early gene (IEG) transcription and regulate transcription factors during development (West and Greenberg, 2011; Wild et al., 2019; Perfitt et al., 2020).
The crucial roles of LTCCs in regulating neural circuit maturation is further evident from cognitive and behavioral disorders linked to LTCC gene dysfunction. For example, Timothy syndrome (TS), is a severe disorder associated with core symptoms of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and results from gain-of-function mutations in the Cav1.2 and Cav1.3 LTCC genes (Splawski et al., 2005). Further, polymorphisms and loss-of-function LTCC variants are linked with autism and schizophrenia (Dedic et al., 2018; Kisko et al., 2018; Moon et al., 2018).
Neuroimaging studies in humans and experimental evidence in rodents have revealed a key involvement of the amygdala in neurodevelopmental disorders (Benarroch, 2015; Herrington et al., 2017). In animal models of ASD, functional alterations of the basolateral subdivision of the amygdala (BLA) result in the impairment of social interactions (Truitt et al., 2007; Sosa-Díaz et al., 2014). Further, experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in the amygdala underlies fear processing, anxiety and social activities (Rainnie et al., 2004; Truitt et al., 2007).
Seminal studies of the postnatal maturation of rat BLA neurons show substantial changes in resting membrane potential, input resistance, firing patterns and synaptic activity occurring between postnatal day P7 and P21, concomitant with the expansion of dendritic arbor (Ehrlich et al., 2009, 2012, 2013; Ehrlich and Rainnie, 2015; Ryan et al., 2016). These studies have led to the notion that the first three weeks of rat postnatal life represent a critical vulnerable period and that pathologic alterations can cause permanent changes in the BLA circuitry, in line with the hypotheses of developmental etiology of ASD and schizophrenia (Meredith, 2015).
Prenatal stress can induce age-specific changes in socioemotional behavior in rats through alterations in BLA excitability, suggesting that developmental manipulation of membrane electrical properties can produce long-lasting changes in the amygdala network (Ehrlich and Rainnie, 2015). Our recent study employing rat brain slices showed that acute application of the LTCC agonist (S)-BayK8644 (BayK) evoked a robust increase in action potential firing and an overall increase in BLA excitability with significantly higher impact on P7 immature neurons compared with P21 neurons (Zhang et al., 2020). However, it remains to be elucidated whether LTCC-mediated BLA hyperexcitability at P7 could subsequently translate into physiologically relevant changes in amygdala-dependent behaviors in early adulthood.
To examine longer-term effects of increased LTCC activity, stereotaxic bilateral injections of BayK into the BLA were performed in rat pups at P7, P14, or P21. Electrophysiological, synaptic and behavioral characteristics were examined at P28, a juvenile stage when the BLA circuit has reached maturity. BayK injection was found associated with noted changes in neuronal membrane properties, long-term potentiation (LTP) of excitatory inputs, ionotropic glutamate receptor subunit expression, impairment of social interaction ability, increased anxiety and the presence of repetitive behaviors. Together, the results support the hypothesis that the BLA network is prone to functional manipulation of LTCC activity, and provide evidence of a key role of LTCCs in regulating the developmental organization of mature networks sustaining complex behaviors.
Materials and Methods
Animals
All experimental protocols were approved by the Animal Care Committee (ACC protocol A16-0127) of the University of British Columbia (UBC). Pregnant Sprague Dawley rats from Charles River Laboratories (17 d of pregnancy on the day of arrival) were housed at room temperature (24–26°C) in a 12/12 h light/dark cycle at UBC Animal Resource Unit. To minimize any potential effects of environmental changes we followed the UBC SOP on handing of rat and mouse pups that undergo procedures (September 27, 2016) for habituation. Briefly, rat pups were handled initially in the presence of the mother and then separated from the mother (30 min/d) from day 0 to the day of surgery. Rat pups were weaned at P21. Pups were randomly assigned to three experimental groups with each of the groups receiving a one-time BayK or DMSO bilateral injection at a specific age (P7, or P14, or P21). Male rats were used in all experiments. While the present study was limited to sampling males, we expect that future studies involving females may identify and consider any important sex differences relevant to BLA postnatal development and LTCC involvement (Guadagno et al., 2020).
Stereotaxic surgery
The stereotaxic injection of vehicle (DMSO) or BayK was performed on rat male pups from different litters at P7, P14, or P21 using a stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments, Model 900) with a resolution of 100 μm. Initially, rat pups were placed in an acrylic chamber and anesthetized with isoflurane using a VetEquip vaporizer; anesthesia was maintained throughout the surgery using a nose cone/face mask. Once the surgical plane of anesthesia was reached, the skin over the cerebrum was incised by a 1 cm-long median-sagittal cut. Two symmetrical holes were made using a dental drill on each side of the skull over the cerebrum (P7: bregma −2.4 mm anterior-posterior, ±3.4 mm medial-lateral, 6.8 mm dorsal-ventral; P14: bregma −2.6 mm anterior-posterior, ±3.8 mm medial-lateral, 7 mm dorsal-ventral; P21: bregma −3.8 mm anterior-posterior, ±4.0 mm medial lateral, 7.2 mm dorsal-ventral). Bregma coordinates were established for each age considering the rapid change in the position of brain structures during the periods examined (Khazipov et al., 2015). A Hamilton Neuros micro-syringe was inserted on each side to deliver 0.5 μl of a 1 mm BayK solution (corresponding to a calculated dose of 8 μg/kg) or the same volume of DMSO vehicle into the BLA regions. The injection duration was 5 min at a rate of 0.1 μl/min. After injection, the microsyringe needle was left stationary for a further 3 min before retrieval. After completion of the procedures, the skin was sutured with subcuticular sutures (6–0 coated Vicryl). To attenuate surgical pain in pups, bupivacaine (diluted in saline solution to 0.25%) was dripped onto the skin when closing the incision at the end of surgery.
In order to verify the location of bilateral injection within the BLA, a subset of naive rats received 0.2 μl methylene blue (20%) stereotaxic injection at either P7, P14, or P21. Animals were euthanized, brains dissected and 300 μm slices collected with a vibratome (VT 1200, Leica) and visually examined under the microscope. The area of BLA containing methylene blue confirmed the accuracy and consistency of the bilateral injection. In addition, to verify the diffusion area of BayK injection in BLA, animals (n = 4) were euthanized, brains dissected 1 h after BayK injection, slices prepared and observed under the microscope. The BLA area showed a rounded area of ∼1 mm in diameter which was visible because of the yellow color of the BayK solution.
Acute brain slice preparation
At age P28, animals were anesthetized using isoflurane [5% in oxygen (v/v)] and decapitated using a rodent guillotine. The brain was quickly removed and transferred to ice-cold sucrose cutting solution: 214 mm sucrose, 26 mm NaHCO3, 1.25 mm NaH2PO4, 11 mm glucose, 2.5 mm KCl, 0.5 mm CaCl2, and 6 mm MgCl2, bubbled constantly with 95% O2/5% CO2. Trimmed brain tissue was glued to the cutting chamber of a vibratome. Coronal brain slices containing amygdala were cut to 300 μm thickness then incubated at 32°C for 1 h in artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF): 130 mm NaCl, 30 mm NaHCO3, 3.5 mm KCl, 1.1 mm KH2PO4, 1.3 mm MgCl2, 2.5 mm CaCl2, 10 mm glucose, 0.4 mm sodium ascorbate, 0.8 mm thiourea, and 2 mm sodium pyruvate, saturated with 95% O2/5% CO2.
Electrophysiological recordings
Acute brain slices were transferred to a recording chamber (234 μl total volume; Warner RC-26G) mounted on the stage of an upright microscope (Zeiss Axioskop 2) and maintained in ACSF perfusion at a flow rate of 2 ml/min. An Ag/AgCl pellet was used for grounding the recording chamber.
BLA pyramidal neurons were visually identified using infrared differential interference contrast (IR-DIC) in combination with a 40× water immersion objective. To confirm the localization of the BLA area, a subset of BLA neurons were labeled with biocytin (0.05% in internal pipette solution). All electrophysiological recordings were performed using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier with pClamp software version 11 and digitized with a DigiData 1550B (Molecular Devices) acquisition system.
Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were performed in standard ACSF solution. An AP-1000 micropipette puller (Sutter Instrument) was used to pull patch pipettes from thick wall borosilicate capillary glass with final resistances of 3–5 MΩ. For neuronal action potential recordings in current clamp, pipettes were filled with the following internal solution: 130 mm K gluconate, 2 mm KCl, 10 mm HEPES, 3 mm MgCl2, 2 mm K-ATP, 0.2 mm Na-GTP, and 5 mm phosphocreatine Tris, the pH was adjusted to 7.3 with KOH, and osmolality was adjusted to 290 mOsmol/L with D-Mannitol. Cells with a resting membrane potential less than −55 mV, or access resistance >30 MΩ, or action potentials exhibiting no overshoot were excluded. The access resistance and bridge balance values were checked throughout the recording and cells with >15% changes were not included in data analyses. Current-clamp values are reported without subtracting the liquid junction potential, calculated as 11.4 mV. For current–voltage (I-V) relationship analyses, steady-state voltage changes were measured and averaged over a 400-ms period at the end of a 1 s square current pulse, and the input resistance (Rin) calculated from the linear part of the I-V curve near the resting membrane potential. The steady-state frequency of action potentials was obtained from the last 400 ms of the depolarizing pulses, and plotted as the function of normalized current injection for frequency-current (f-I) relationship. Gain was measured as the slope of the initial linear part of f-I curve. The recordings were obtained and low-pass filtered at 10 kHz and digitized at 50 kHz.
The following internal solution was used to record evoked EPSCs: 134.63 mm Cs-methanesulfonate, 5 mm CsCl, 5 mm TEA-Cl, 0.4 mm EGTA, 10 mm HEPES, 2.5 mm Mg-ATP, 2.5 mm Na-GTP, and 5 mm phosphocreatine Tris; the pH was adjusted to 7.3 with CsOH and osmolality was adjusted to 290 mOsm/L with D-Mannitol. To examine evoked EPSCs, BLA cells were held at −60 mV and a concentric bipolar stimulating electrode (CBAPC100 from FHC Inc.) was placed on the lateral amygdala (LA). Single pulses were generated with an S48 Stimulator via a Stimulus Isolation Unit SIU5 (Grass Instruments) at 0.05 Hz. In order to induce LTP, high-frequency stimulation (HFS) was applied at 100 Hz for 1 s, repeated five times with an interval of 10 s, while the cells were held at a membrane potential of +30 mV. EPSCs were recorded in ACSF containing GABAA receptor antagonist picrotoxin (PTX 100 μm) and GABAB receptor antagonist CGP52432 (1 μm). The amplitude of EPSCs was calculated as the peak current in the first 50 ms following the stimulation artifact. Data acquisition was sampled at 2 kHz and filtered at 50 kHz.
Two different approaches were used to examine the effect of BayK on synaptic plasticity: in one group of animals the induction of LTP was tested subsequent to the behavioral tests; in a second group, LTP recordings were performed without animals having been exposed to any prior behavioral experience.
Behavioral tests
All tests were performed during the dark period of the day/night cycle in a quiet symmetrically designed test room under dark conditions. Animal behavior was recorded with an overhead infrared camera (Amcrest ProHD 1080P) placed 1.5 m away above the test chamber. At P28 the juvenile animals were examined in the following ASD-related behavioral tests:
Three chamber test
Rodents have a preference to spend time with conspecifics and this test is used to assess cognition in the form of general sociability and interest in social novelty in rodent models of CNS disorders (Malkova et al., 2012). Experimental animals were tested in a 0.89 m L × 0.48 m W × 0.4 m H Plexiglas box divided into three chambers; rats could freely move between chambers through a small opening (10 × 8 cm). At the beginning of the session, rats were placed in the central chamber for 10 min to explore the empty box to evaluate bias for either of the side chambers. Once we confirmed that the animal had no evident bias, a stranger rat of the same sex and similar body weight was placed in a wire cage (12 cm in height, 9 cm in diameter) on one lateral chamber chosen randomly; a second, identical empty wire cage was placed on the opposite side chamber as a nonsocial object. The time spent in each of the three chambers was measured and social preference calculated as [timesocial chamber/(timesocial chamber + timenonsocial chamber)] × 100 – 50. The test chambers were cleaned with 70% ethanol at the end of each session.
Social interaction test
Social interaction behaviors are employed in juvenile and adult rats for evaluating social motivation and nonplay social behaviors (Vanderschuren et al., 2016; Pellis et al., 2018). Experimental animals were placed individually in the test box (0.89 m L × 0.48 m W × 0.4 m H Plexiglas box with an open-top) 10 min before the test for habituation. Then, an age-matched, sex-matched, and weight-matched stranger rat, which had not been housed together before the test, was placed in the test box. Social interaction behaviors of the two juvenile rats were recorded for 10 min. The frequency and duration of the following behavioral patterns from experimental animals were measured with a chronometer by a blind-to-treatment observer using the video recording: (1) chasing/sniffing, the experimental rat follows the stranger rat and sniffs its back and genital area; this parameter evaluates social exploration; (2) climbing, a dominant posture in which the experimental rat stands over the back or the exposed ventral area of the stranger rat and with its front paws presses the shoulder of the opponent against the floor; this parameter evaluates the ability to engage in active social interaction with conspecifics.
Self-grooming test
Self-grooming can provide an index of repetitive/stereotypic behavior in rodents (Silverman et al., 2010). Individual rats were placed into an inverted glass beaker of 10-cm diameter × 15-cm height, covered with a filter top, for 10 min as it is known that a restricted environment induces repetitive behavior (Lewis et al., 2007). Self-grooming behavior was then evaluated by recording the number and duration of self-grooming for a further 10 min. All beakers were thoroughly cleaned with 70% ethanol before and after each test.
Open field test
This test was originally developed to assess for emotionality or locomotor activity in rodents; more recently it has been widely used for testing the level of anxiety. The animal was placed into the open field (0.4 m L × 0.32 m W × 0.3 m H Plexiglas box) and behaviors recorded over? 10 min. Rodents typically prefer not to be in the center, and tend to walk close to the walls since the chamber is a novel, presumably stressful, environment. A decreased amount of time spent in the center area (0.18 m L × 0.14 m W) of the open field may indicate a higher anxiety level.
Quantitative real-time PCR (qPCR)
BLA tissue samples were microdissected from 300 μm coronal brain sections using a stainless-steel brain punch tissue set (Leica Biosystems) to examine mRNA expression levels of IEGs and ionotropic glutamatergic receptors using qPCR (Palkovits, 1985). For IEGs c-fos, arc, and Homer1a, tissue samples were collected at 1, 3, or 12 h after stereotaxic injection of BayK or DMSO. For ionotropic glutamatergic receptors GluA and GluN genes, tissue samples were collected at P28 (Table 1). BLA samples were either stored at −80°C or processed immediately. Tissue was homogenized and total RNA was isolated using Dynabeads Magnetic Beads (MicroMod) and MagMax 96 for Microarrays Total RNA Isolation kit (Ambion), then dissolved in TE buffer (IDT). RNA was reverse transcribed into first-strand cDNA with the High-Capacity DNA Reverse Transcription kit (Thermo Fisher Scientific) under the following conditions in a Bio-Rad Thermal cycler: 25°C for 10 min, 37°C for 120 min, and 85°C for 5 s. All qPCR probes were obtained from Integrated DNA Technologies (IDT); primer sequences are given in Table 1. cDNA samples and probes were prepared with KAPA Probe Fast 2× Mix (Sigma) and loaded as triplicates into a 384-well processing plate. Real-time qPCR was performed in a Bio-Rad CFX384 Touch Real-Time PCR system with an initial step of 98°C for 2 min, then the cycling conditions at 98°C for 5 s, 57.5°C for 15 s, with 40 cycles total, followed by finishing steps at 56.6°C for 5 s and 98°C for 50 s. All samples were processed in triplicate and expression levels of each product were normalized to the expression of GAPDH. Ct values were measured from the regression function of the amplification curve. Data are reported as mean ± SEM. Statistical comparison was performed with one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni test.
Real-time qPCR probe sequences
Data analysis
Electrophysiological data analyses were performed using Clampfit version 11 (Molecular Devices) and Origin version 2019 (OriginLab). Data are reported as mean ± SEM. Statistical comparison was performed using hypothesis testing (unpaired Student’s t test; Welch correction where appropriate). Student’s t test was used to analyze the significance between BayK and DMSO treatment groups in behavioral tests. A p value < 0.05 was considered to indicate statistical significance. Estimation statistics, which provide quantitative answers and information about the precision of each measurement by using effect size and confidence intervals (CIs; Gardner and Altman, 1986; Ho et al., 2019; Bernard, 2019; Calin-Jageman and Cumming, 2019), was also used to analyze the data where appropriate using GraphPad Prism 9 (GraphPad Software). In the estimation plots in Figures 2, 3, the left axis is used for the scatter graph where individual data points are plotted, the right axis shows the difference between means with a 95% CI (in blue); dotted lines are aligned to each respective mean. In Figures 4, 6, the line in the box plot represents the median, the open square represents the mean value, and whiskers are set to cover the minimum and maximum of the data values (Krzywinski and Altman, 2014). All n values reported as sample size represents number of neurons recorded in electrophysiology experiments, or number of animals in behavioral tests and real-time qPCR experiments.
Drugs
(S)-Bay K8644, nifedipine, D-AP5, CNQX, CGP52432, and QX-314 were purchased from Tocris Bioscience. PTX was purchased from Sigma-Aldrich. (S)-Bay K8644, nifedipine, CNQX were dissolved in DMSO and stored at −20°C before use. Other drugs were dissolved in nanopure H2O. All drugs were applied by perfusion in the bath solution.
Results
Altering LTCC activity via bilateral BLA injection of BayK at P7, P14, or P21 differentially modifies neuronal electrophysiological properties recorded at P28
Our previous study using brain slices showed acute BayK application to profoundly yet differentially change the intrinsic excitability of BLA neurons at distinct early postnatal ages (Zhang et al., 2020). Here, we examined whether BayK injection at P7, P14, or P21 resulted in subsequent changes to basal electrophysiological properties of BLA principal neurons at P28 (Fig. 1). In animals that received BayK injection at P14 the resting membrane potential was significantly depolarized at P28 compared with vehicle control (DMSO: −59.7 ± 0.4 mV, n = 5, BayK: −55.1 ± 1.3 mV, n = 11, p = 0.038, mean difference 4.659 [95% CI 0.301–9.017]; Fig. 2A), making it closer to the action potential threshold. In contrast, animals that received BayK treatment at either P7 or P21 did not show significant differences in resting potential at P28 compared with the DMSO group (P7, DMSO: −59.9 ± 1.9 mV, n = 9, BayK: −57.9 ± 2.1 mV, n = 12, p = 0.516, mean difference 1.947 [95% CI −4.205–8.099]; P21, DMSO: −55.9 ± 0.8 mV, n = 7, BayK: −55.5 ± 0.9 mV, n = 6, p = 0.685, mean difference 0.505 [95% CI −2.164–3.173]; Fig. 2A). As determined by I-V relations measured at P28, also notable with BayK bilateral BLA injections at P14, but not P7 or P21, was a reduction in the slow depolarizing sag observed during membrane hyperpolarizations (Fig. 1B, arrowhead) and a significant increase in steady-state voltage responses toward hyperpolarizing currents (Fig. 1B). Thus, we compared the magnitude of the sag index (Jarsky et al., 2008) and rectification ratio (Farries et al., 2005) from the I-V recordings at P14. The sag index was calculated as the difference between the initial maximum and the steady-state voltage deflection in response to the largest hyperpolarizing current step from the I-V curve, divided by the magnitude of the steady-state at the end of the current step. Neurons from DMSO treatment showed higher sag index (0.251 ± 0.041, n = 10) than those from BayK treatment (0.123 ± 0.016, n = 18; p = 0.002; mean difference −0.129 [95% CI −0.205 to −0.051]; estimation analysis shown in Fig. 2B).
Bilateral intra-amygdala injection of BayK at early postnatal periods produces differential changes in BLA neurons intrinsic excitability at P28. I-V relationship plots corresponding to each experimental group are shown in the left panels (A, P7; B, P14; C, P21). Representative traces in the middle panels show voltage responses to hyperpolarizing current pulses, and a train of action potentials elicited by a suprathreshold depolarizing pulse. The arrows denote a slow depolarizing sag in the electrotonic potentials evoked by membrane hyperpolarization. A significant sag reduction (B, arrowhead middle traces) is observed in neurons from animals treated at P14 compared with DMSO control group. The f-I relationship was plotted with values of the steady-state firing frequency as a function of current injection (right panels). Animals treated with BayK at P7 showed a small increase in steady-state frequency across all current injection range, compared with the DMSO control group (A, right). No obvious changes were found in the I-V or f-I relationships in groups treated with BayK at P21 (C). Data are presented as mean ± SEM.
P14 BayK-injected animals also exhibited an apparent but nonsignificant increase in input resistance at P28 (DMSO 77.1 ± 11.3 MΩ, n = 5, BayK 128.9 ± 20.6 MΩ, n = 11, p = 0.128 mean difference 51.79 [95% CI −16.87–120.4]; Fig. 2C). To quantitatively determine whether the apparent nonlinearity of the IV-curve in the BayK treated neurons was associated to an increase in inward rectification, we calculated the rectification ratio as the quotient of membrane resistance at negative membrane potentials divided by the resistance around resting membrane potential. We found no significant differences between the mean value of the rectification ratios from control (0.71 ± 0.06, n = 5) and BayK group (0.67 ± 0.05, n = 9; p =0.655). Input resistances were not significantly different in P7 (DMSO: 111.3 ± 14.6 MΩ, n = 9, BayK: 133.6 ± 29.5 MΩ, n = 12, p = 0.550, mean difference 22.26 [95% CI −54.20–98.72]) or P21 injection groups (DMSO: 83.67 ± 8.8 MΩ, n = 6, BayK: 84.8 ± 8.1 MΩ, n = 6, p = 0.924, mean difference 1.167 [95% CI −25.41–27.74]; Fig. 2C).
Estimation plots of BLA neuron passive membrane properties. Estimation analysis was used to compare resting membrane potential (A), sag index (B), and input resistance (C) in P28 rats injected with BayK (red) or DMSO (black) at P7 (left panels), P14 (middle panels), or P21 (right panels). 95% CIs are shown in blue. Resting membrane potential was increased in rats injected at P14 (mean difference between DMSO and BayK is 4.659 [95% CI 0.3014, 9.017], two tailed unpaired t test t = 2.293, df = 14, p = 0.0378) and the sag index was reduced (mean difference between DMSO and BayK is −0.1285 [95% CI −0.2054, −0.05154], two tailed unpaired t test t = 3.432, df = 26, p = 0.0020). No other properties were impacted by BayK injection.
To further examine the effects of postnatal BayK injection on BLA neuronal excitability, f-I relations (steady-state firing frequency vs current injection) were examined at P28. Results show that the rheobase was not significantly changed at P28 for BayK injection at any of the postnatal ages (P7, DMSO: 65.0 ± 6.9 pA, n = 10, BayK: 66.7 ± 15.63 pA, n = 9, p = 0.921 mean difference 1.667 [95% CI −33.09–36.42]; P14, DMSO: 82.5 ± 12.2 pA, n = 8; BayK: 73.3 ± 19.1, n = 6, p = 0.679 mean difference −9.167 [95% CI −56.34–38.00]; P21, DMSO:75.0 ± 22.2 pA, n = 4; BayK: 80.0 ± 14.1 pA, n = 4, p = 0.856 mean difference 5.000 [95% CI −59.35–69.35]; Fig. 3A). Although the p value did not reach significance level, BLA neurons from animals treated with BayK at P7 exhibited slightly higher steady-state firing frequency across the entire range of current injection tested (Fig. 1A, right; maximal frequency values, DMSO: 22.8 ± 1.6 Hz, n = 6, BayK: 25.3 ± 1.2 Hz, n = 13, p = 0.267, mean difference 2.440 [95% CI −2.050–6.929]). No changes were found in the animals treated at P14 (Fig. 1B, right; DMSO: 20.9 ± 0.63 Hz, n = 4, BayK: 22.7 ± 1.2 Hz, n = 7, p = 0.322, mean difference 1.729 [95% CI −2.003–5.461]) or P21 (Fig. 1C, right; DMSO: 22.5 ± 2.2 Hz, n = 6, BayK: 22.5 ± 4.3 Hz, n = 6, p = 0.992, mean difference 0.051 [95% CI −10.74–10.84]; Fig. 3B). Measuring the initial slope of the f-I curve indicated that the gain was not significantly different between animals treated with BayK or DMSO at P7 (DMSO: 0.17 ± 0.02 Hz/pA, n = 10; BayK: 0.19 ± 0.03; n = 12, p = 0.450 mean difference 0.0293 [95% CI −0.050–0.109]), P14 (DMSO: 0.19 ± 0.02 Hz/pA, n = 7; BayK: 0.18 ± 0.02 Hz/pA, n = 9, p = 0.697 mean difference −0.011 [95% CI −0.071–0.049]), or P21 (DMSO: 0.16 ± 0.03, n = 5, BayK: 0.17 ± 0.04; n = 4, p = 0.798 mean difference 0.013 [95% CI −0.102–0.128]; Fig. 3C), indicating that overall sensitivity in response to electrical stimuli is not affected by BayK injection. Overall, these results point to qualitative differences between the effects of acute L-type-mediated regulation of BLA intrinsic excitability and membrane properties (Zhang et al., 2020), and the effects of a single in vivo injection of an L-type channel agonist which raises the possibility that compensatory mechanisms operating during the timeframe between BayK treatment and electrophysiological recordings could oppose sustained hyperexcitability. Although the exact mechanism remains to be determined, this observation is consistent with a previous study showing that conditional CNS deletion of Cav1.2 channel was less effective than acute intraventricular administration of isradipine in altering the acquisition of conditioned fear and LTP induction in thalamocortical inputs to the LA (Langwieser et al., 2010).
Estimation plots of BLA neuron firing properties. Estimation analysis was used to compare rheobase (A), maximal firing frequency (B), and gain (C) in P28 rats injected with BayK (red) or DMSO (black) at P7 (left panels), P14 (middle panels), or P21 (right panels). 95% CIs are shown in blue. No parameters were significantly different between BayK and DMSO injected groups.
Bilateral BLA BayK injection at early postnatal ages produces long-lasting alterations to juvenile behavioral patterns
It was of interest and relevance to determine whether postnatal BayK injection into the BLA affected subsequent behaviors in young adult rats. Several behavioral tests are commonly used for animal models of ASD, including the three-chamber test, social interaction and self-grooming test. The three-chamber paradigm, also known as Crawley’s sociability and preference for social novelty, has been successfully employed to study social affiliation in rodents (Kaidanovich-Beilin et al., 2011). We first measured the ratio of the total time a subject rat spent in the chamber of a stranger rat over the time in chamber of the novel object. At P28, animals that had been treated with BayK at P14 showed a decreased ratio compared with DMSO treatment (Fig. 4A; DMSO: 5.27 ± 0.94, n = 9; BayK: 2.48 ± 0.55, n = 11; p = 0.022), whereas no significant differences were observed between BayK and DMSO treatment for either the P7 group (DMSO: 3.49 ± 0.36, n = 9; BayK: 3.59 ± 0.52, n = 10, p = 0.76) or the P21group (DMSO: 3.37 ± 1.12, n = 8; BayK: 3.71 ± 0.61, n = 8, p = 0.73). Measuring the time a subject rat spent in the chamber of stranger rat, there was found to be a significant decrease induced by BayK treatment at P14 compared with DMSO treatment (DMSO: 425.85 ± 20.7, n = 9, BayK: 338.27 ± 27.7, n = 11; p = 0.047), no significant difference between BayK and DMSO treatment in the experimental group of P7 (DMSO: 400.2 ± 19.1, n = 9, BayK: 356.33 ± 44.4, n = 10; p = 0.19) or P21(DMSO: 374 ± 37.4, n = 8, BayK: 393.46 ± 36.9, n = 8; p = 0.72). Similarly, for animals that received BayK treatment at P14, a corresponding increase was observed in the time spent in the chamber of the novel object (DMSO: 102.9 ± 15.3, n = 9, BayK: 178.2 ± 27.4, n = 11; p = 0.037). Again, no significant differences were found for the P7 (DMSO: 126.3 ± 9.9, n = 9, BayK: 131.8 ± 15.2, n = 10; p = 0.76) and P21 groups (DMSO: 152 ± 31.7, n = 8, BayK: 133.7 ± 24.5, n = 8; p = 0.66). The results indicate that at P14, but not P7 or P21, increased BLA LTCC activity mediated via BayK injection results in impaired social motivation toward stranger rats and an increased interest in novel objects at P28. The altered behaviors at P14 are those suggested to resemble certain phenotypes associated with ASD.
BayK injection at early developmental stages produced long-term alterations in amygdala-related behavioral phenotypes. Four different behavioral tests were performed in P28 juvenile rats previously treated at different ages with BayK or DMSO as vehicle control. Simplified schematic diagrams in the upper part of each panel indicate the experimental paradigms, where the white rat indicates the experimental subject and the dark rat indicates the naive animal used as stranger: A, three-chamber social affiliation test; B, social interaction behavior; C, repetitive behavior; D, anxiety test. Corresponding parameters are shown in black symbols for control (DMSO) and red symbols for experimental (BayK) groups. A, The social affiliation was measured by the ratio of time the experimental rat spent around the stranger, over the time spent around the novel object (empty cage used as nonsocial object) and values are shown in box plots. Animals that received BayK injection at P7 (DMSO n = 9, BayK n = 10) or P21 (DMSO n = 8, BayK n = 8) showed no significant differences in this parameter compared with control, whereas BayK treatment at P14 have significant lower stranger/object ratio (DMSO n = 9, BayK n = 11), indicating a reduced social affiliation. B, Social interactions were monitored for 10 min and the duration of periods of chasing/sniffing exploratory behaviors was recorded. Box plots show that BayK treatment did not induce significant differences in any age group on this behavioral parameter. C, Repetitive behavior was measured as the duration of self-grooming in a restricted space. Box plots show that animals receiving BayK injection at P7 exhibited an increase in the duration of self-grooming when tested at P28 (C, left bars), whereas no differences were found in the P14 and P21 experimental groups. D, Levels of anxiety were evaluated using an open field test and measured as the time spent in the central area. Animals with BayK treatment at P7 spent less time in the center, indicating an increased anxiety level compared with the DMSO group. No differences were found in the P14 and P21 experimental groups. Data are presented as mean ± SEM; *p < 0.05.
We next examined whether BayK injection during early development affected P28 rats with regard to social interaction. The total duration a subject rat spent on chasing/sniffing a stranger rat, a typical pattern of social exploration, was examined although no significant differences were found between BayK and DMSO treatments in any of the experimental groups (Fig. 4B; P7 DMSO: 31.5 ± 5.36 s, n = 8, BayK: 36.1 ± 2.2 s, n = 12, p = 0.38; P14 DMSO: 30.9 ± 4.9 s, n = 9, BayK: 29.7 ± 3.7 s, n = 10, p = 0.84; P21 DMSO: 26.6 ± 3.1, n = 8, BayK: 28.4 ± 3.4, n = 8, p = 0.71). Also measured was the total duration of climbing up on the back of a stranger rat as an index of active social interaction (data not shown), no significant differences were found in the three experimental groups.
The occurrence of repetitive behavior was also examined (Fig. 4C). Animals treated with BayK at P7 showed a significant increase in self-grooming duration at P28 compared with DMSO treated animals (DMSO: 107.1 ± 35.6 ms, n = 9, BayK: 182.5 ± 25.7 ms, n = 11, p = 0.038), indicating a higher level of repetitive behavior. Contrastingly, no differences were found for the P14 or P21 experimental groups (P14, DMSO: 129.6 ± 30.1 ms, n = 9, BayK: 147.2 ± 31.3 ms, n = 10, p = 0.69; P21, DMSO: 193.9 ± 36.3 ms, n = 7, BayK: 207.1 ± 25.3 ms, n = 6, p = 0.76).
Human and animal studies indicate important roles for the amygdala in mediating fear and anxiety, and in the manifestation of anxiety disorders (Forster et al., 2012), thus we also examined the effects of BLA BayK injection on long-term anxiety in juvenile rats (Fig. 4D). The results show that animals treated with BayK at P7 (DMSO: 62.5 ± 12.3 ms, n = 9, BayK: 35.9 ± 5.1 ms, n = 15; p = 0.03) exhibit a significant decrease in time spent in the center of the area, indicating a higher level of anxiety at P28 compared with DMSO treatment. No significant differences were found for animals that received treatments at either P14 (DMSO: 61.9 ± 19.5 ms, n = 8, BayK: 44.8 ± 8.5 ms, n = 14; p = 0.36) or P21 (DMSO: 42.1 ± 14.6 ms, n = 8, BayK: 33.9 ± 6.9 ms, n = 8, p = 0.62).
Taken together, behavioral studies at P28 following bilateral injection of BayK to rat pups showed distinct long-term behavioral alterations; BayK at P7 produced higher anxiety levels and repetitive behaviors, while BayK at P14 induced an impaired ability for social affiliation, together suggesting the crucial but differential involvement of LTCC activity in BLA circuits underlying the development of behavioral phenotypes. Interestingly, BayK injection into the BLA at P21 had no significant behavioral effects at P28 on any of the behaviors examined. The changes observed for the P7 and P14 experimental groups suggest the existence of a vulnerable period during early development, while the lack of observable effects at P21 may reflect that neural circuits underlying these behaviors have already reached a stage where they are less sensitive to changes in LTCC activity.
Altering LTCC activity at distinct postnatal stages differentially affects LTP in BLA neurons
The LTP of synaptic activity is the most widely studied form of synaptic plasticity and plays important roles in learning, memory formation, fear, stress and social activities (Jung et al., 2013; Leuner and Shors, 2013; Nicoll, 2017). Here, we examined whether BayK injection into the BLA at P7, P14, and P21 mediated changes in synaptic plasticity at P28 regarding excitatory inputs from the LA into the BLA.
In the presence of GABA receptor blockers PTX and CGP52432, single-pulse stimulation at low frequency applied in the LA elicited EPSC responses in BLA neurons (recorded as basal synaptic activity during a 5-min period; Fig. 5, left representative traces). To induce LTP we applied HFS (1-s train of repetitive pulses at 100 Hz, repeated 5 times at 0.1 Hz) paired with membrane potential depolarization to +30 mV. Under these conditions LTP was not induced in neurons from vehicle-treated animals at any of the P7, P14, or P21 ages. In contrast, HFS induced an increase in EPSC amplitude in BLA neurons from animals treated with BayK at both P7 and P14 (Fig. 5A,B) but not P21 (Fig. 5C). LTP in BLA neurons following BayK treatment at P7 showed a significant increase in EPSC amplitude over the 30 min recording period (Fig. 5A; normalized EPSC amplitude at 25 min: DMSO 0.97 ± 0.08, n = 3, BayK 1.84 ± 0.14, n = 5, p = 0.005; EPSC amplitude at baseline: 126.18 ± 11.9 pA for DMSO and 131.5 ± 17.1 pA for BayK, EPSC amplitude at 25 min of recording: 122.9 ± 19.6 pA for DMSO, and 241.7 ± 36 pA for BayK). Similarly, LTP in neurons from animals treated with BayK at P14 was significantly enhanced (Fig. 5B; normalized EPSC amplitude at 25 min: DMSO 0.99 ± 0.09, n = 5, BayK 1.86 ± 0.09, n = 5, p = 0.004; EPSC amplitude at the baseline: 137.9 ± 19.3 pA for DMSO and 123.3 ± 21.3 pA for BayK, EPSC amplitude at 25 min of recording: 136.9 ± 19.7 pA for DMSO, and 226.3 ± 30.1 pA for BayK). LTP could not be induced in neurons from either the P21 vehicle or BayK groups (Fig. 5C; normalized EPSC amplitude at 25 min: DMSO 0.96 ± 0.12, n = 4, BayK 1.02 ± 0.11, n = 4, p = 0.71; EPSC amplitude at baseline: 102 ± 17.5 pA for DMSO and 121.6 ± 20.1 pA for BayK, EPSC amplitude at 25 min of recording: 98.8 ± 26 pA for DMSO and 124.2 ± 12.7 pA for BayK). Importantly, BayK application on BLA slices from naive animals did not result in the induction of LTP using the HFS protocol (Fig. 6E), suggesting that activation of acute LTCC activation alone is not enough for LTP induction.
LTP was induced in P28 BLA neurons from rats that received bilateral BayK injection at P7 and P14, but not at P21. EPSC responses in BLA neurons were elicited by stimulating LA at 0.05 Hz. A HFS at 100 Hz (1 s duration, repeat 5 times at 0.1 Hz, membrane potential was holding at +30 mV) was used to induce LTP. The representative traces in the left panels show single EPSCs recorded before (left) and 25 min after (right) HFS. Black traces represent recordings in control groups while red traces represent BayK groups. EPSC amplitudes are presented as normalized values relative to the baseline amplitude before HFS. LTP was induced in the animals that received BayK treatment at P7 (A) or P14 (B) compared with their DMSO counterparts. No LTP was observed from animals treated with BayK at P21 (C). Data are presented as mean ± SEM.
LTCC-associated LTP induction is enhanced but not determined by prior behavioral experience
Environmental novelty and/or prior exposure to behavioral testing can impact the subsequent induction of LTP (Roman et al., 1999; Moncada and Viola, 2007; Eckert and Abraham, 2010). In a valproate rat model of ASD the induction of LTP for thalamic inputs into the LA occurred following behavioral experiments measuring fear conditioning and extinction with an identical LTP observed in the absence of preceding behavioral tests (Lin et al., 2013). Here, we asked whether the LTP observed in BayK-treated animals could have resulted as a consequence of the behavioral experience itself during assessment of behavioral traits, and also whether BayK treatment per se could cause permissive functional changes for LTP to occur in the intra-amygdala circuit.
First, we performed slice patch-clamp recordings on P28 rats that received BayK or vehicle injections at P7 without any behavioral tests or environmental disturbances before the recordings. No significant differences in voltage-current relationship, input resistance and resting membrane potentials between BayK and DMSO experimental groups were detected (data not shown). LTP induction experiments (Fig. 6A) showed similar results when compared to recordings performed after behavioral tests (Fig. 6C; EPSC relative amplitude at 25 min: DMSO 1.05 ± 0.07, n = 6, BayK 2.02 ± 0.26, n = 5, p = 0.003). These data indicate that BayK treatment alone is sufficient to produce functional changes in the observed electrophysiological properties.
Changes in synaptic strength induced after BayK treatment at P7 or P14 occur without preceding behavioral tests. Panels A, B show data from P28 animals treated with BayK or DMSO at P7 or P14 with no previous behavioral tests performed. EPSC amplitudes are presented as normalized values relative to the baseline amplitude before HFS. HFS paired with postsynaptic depolarization induced LTP that was significantly different between BayK and DMSO control in both P7 (A) and P14 (B) experimental groups. Box plots in panels C, D show the relative EPSC amplitude at 25 min after the HFS, indicating that LTP was successfully induced in neurons from animals that were treated with BayK at P7 or P14, regardless of previous performance of behavioral tests. E, Ratio of EPSC amplitude relative to baseline before HFS obtained from naive noninjected P28 male rats. Acute BLA slices were perfused in ACSF containing 2 μm BayK for 10 min before baseline stimulation. Under these experimental conditions BayK did not induce LTP. Data are presented as mean ± SEM; *p < 0.05.
In P28 animals that received BayK or DMSO injections at P14 without behavioral tests or environmental disturbance before the recordings, similar quantitative differences compared with the experimental groups with behavioral tests were observed: BLA neurons from animals treated with BayK displayed reduced inward rectification in the I-V relation and an upward shift in the f-I relationship but with no change in input resistance (data not shown). The data indicate that without performing behavioral tests the effects of BayK on LTP induction (Fig. 6B,D; normalized EPSC amplitude at 25 min: DMSO 0.85 ± 0.1, n = 4, BayK 1.54 ± 0.18, n = 4, p = 0.019) were more moderate compared with results following behavioral testing (Fig. 6D), consistent with previous findings that behavioral experiences can modify synaptic plasticity. Overall, the data support the notion that LTCC-driven activity changes within BLA circuitry are sufficient to induce LTP albeit prior behavioral experience can further modify the magnitude of the LTCC-mediated effects. It is important to note that the acute application of BayK in vitro was not sufficient to elicit an increase in synaptic strength at the LA to BLA input in response to HFS paired with postsynaptic depolarization (Fig. 6E).
Increasing LTCC activity during development alters the expression of ionotropic glutamatergic receptor genes
The subunit composition of ionotropic glutamatergic receptors can influence synaptic plasticity. For instance, the AMPA receptor subunit GluA2 has been implicated in the induction of LTP (Jia et al., 1996; Whitehead et al., 2013). While for NMDA receptors, the ratio of GluN2A/2B in adults has been suggested to regulate the threshold for synaptic plasticity (Yashiro and Philpot, 2008; Paoletti et al., 2013). Given that we found an increase in the induction of LTP in rats injected at P7 and P14, we hypothesized that BayK injection at these critical periods might lead to changes in the subunit composition of ionotropic glutamatergic receptors. Here, we examined the mRNA expression of AMPA and NMDA receptor subunits at P28 in rats injected at P7, P14, and P21. There were no significant expression changes in animals treated with BayK at P7 for AMPA receptor subunits (Fig. 7A; GluA1, DMSO 0.073 ± 0.006, n = 4, BayK 0.074 ± 0.09, n = 4, p = 0.889; GluA2, DMSO 0.083 ± 0.009, n = 4, BayK 0.082 ± 0.003, n = 4, p = 0.961; GluA3, DMSO 0.038 ± 0.002, n = 4, BayK 0.037 ± 0.002, n = 4, p = 0.985) or NMDA receptor subunits (Fig. 7C; GluN1, DMSO 0.107 ± 0.02, n = 4, BayK 0.124 ± 0.017, n = 4, p = 0.488; GluN2A, DMSO 0.005 ± 0. 0004, n = 4, BayK 0.004 ± 0.0003, n = 4, p = 0.45; GluN2B, DMSO 0.048 ± 0.007, n = 4, BayK 0.052 ± 0.007, n = 4, p = 0.672). Contrastingly, in the P14 experimental groups, the expression of AMPA GluA2 subunits was lower in BayK-injected rats than control (Fig. 7B; DMSO 0.096 ± 0.006, n = 4, BayK 0.076 ± 0.003, n = 4, p = 0.032), and no differences were found in GluA1 (DMSO 0.074 ± 0.007, n = 4, BayK 0.067 ± 0.012, n = 4, p = 0.692) or GluA3 (DMSO 0.039 ± 0.004, n = 4, BayK 0.032 ± 0.002, n = 4, p = 0.17). The expression of NMDA GluN1 and GluN2B subunits were also significantly lower in rats injected with BayK at P14 (Fig. 7D; GluN1: DMSO 0.157 ± 0.006, n = 4, BayK 0.094 ± 0.015, n = 4, p = 0.019; GluN2B: DMSO 0.067 ± 0.004, n = 4, BayK 0.04 ± 0.004, n = 4, p = 0.012), whereas no difference was present in the expression of GluN2A (DMSO 0.004 ± 0.0004, n = 4, BayK 0.004 ± 0.001, n = 4, p = 0.909). As such, the enhanced ability to undergo LTP at P28 for rats injected at P14 may be linked to regulation of functional synaptic receptors expressed in glutamatergic synapses in the BLA.
Bilateral microinjection of BayK during development has long-term consequences on mRNA expression of NMDA and AMPA receptor subunits. BLA samples were collected at P28 from rats injected with BayK (red) or DMSO (black) at P7 (left) or P14 (right), and mRNA expression of glutamatergic receptors AMPA (upper panel) and NMDA (lower panel) was quantified by RT-PCR. No significant changes were observed in rats injected at P7 for the expression of both AMPA and NMDA receptor subunits (A, C). In rats injected with BayK at P14, expression of AMPA receptor subunit GluA2 was significantly decreased (B; p = 0.032). Further, NMDA receptor subunits GluN1 and GluN2B expression was also decreased (D; GluN1 p = 0.019; GluN2B p = 0.011). mRNA expression was calculated as a ratio to GAPDH and expressed as mean ± SEM. Asterisks indicate significant difference between BayK and control group using ANOVA and post hoc Bonferroni test.
Expression of IEGs is transiently altered by BayK bilateral injection at early postnatal ages
Transient expression of IEGs can translate into long-term physiological effects and we hypothesized that a BayK-mediated increase in calcium influx may induce a transcriptional response in relevant BLA circuits. Here, we examined the expression of IEGs as indirect reporters of LTCC-mediated calcium influx evoked by intra-amygdala infusion of BayK. qRT-PCR was performed to determine mRNA levels of the rapid response IEGs c-fos, arc, and Homer1a in BLA samples obtained at different time intervals after intra-BLA infusion of BayK.
c-fos IEG has been widely used as an indirect reporter of neuronal activity as its expression is triggered by calcium influx through both activated NMDA receptors and LTCCs (Rajbhandari et al., 2016; Hudson, 2018). P7 rat pups showed a slightly decreased c-fos mRNA expression 1 h after BayK injection (Fig. 8A; DMSO 0.061 ± 0.019, n = 4, BayK 0.030 ± 0.004, n = 4, p = 0.163), that was followed by a significant increase at 3 h after injection (Fig. 8A; DMSO 0.022 ± 0.008, n = 4, BayK 0.089 ± 0.025, n = 4, p = 0.041). The level of c-fos expression was back to control levels at 12 h (Fig. 8A; DMSO 0.036 ± 0.02, n = 4, BayK 0.052 ± 0.038, n = 4, p = 0.72). Notably, P14 pups showed no significant differences in c-fos expression at any time points following BayK injection (Fig. 8B; 1 h DMSO 0.06 ± 0.019, n = 4, BayK 0.03 ± 0.004, n = 4, p = 0.856; 3 h, DMSO 0.022 ± 0.008, n = 4, BayK 0.09 ± 0.025, n = 4, p = 0.49; 12 h, DMSO 0.036 ± 0.017, n = 4, BayK 0.052 ± 0.017, n = 4, p = 0.0436).
Quantification of IEGs mRNA expression in BLA at different time points after local application of BayK. Amygdala samples were collected at P7 (upper panels) or P14 (lower panels), at three different time points (1, 3, and 12 h) after local microinjection of BayK (red) or DMSO vehicle (black), and relative gene expression of c-fos (left), arc (middle), and Homer1A (right) was quantified by RT-PCR. Significantly increased levels of c-fos mRNA expression following intra-amygdala infusion of BayK were observed in tissue from P7-treated rat pups (A). After a mild drop 1 h after BayK injection, increased mRNA levels were seen at 3 h, followed by a reduction to control values at 12 h. Arc exhibited very low levels of expression at both ages, and showed no increase after BayK exposure (C, D). Homer 1a mRNA levels remained unchanged regardless of treatment (E, F). Individual samples were processed in triplicate, and results of each time point were obtained from four independent experiments (n = 12 animals per target gene). Average mRNA expression was calculated as ratios of IEGs to GAPDH and values are expressed as means ± SEM. Asterisks indicate significant difference between BayK and control group using ANOVA and post hoc Bonferroni test, with a level of significance of 0.041 for P7 c-fos expression at 3 h; 0.032 and 0.006 for P7 Arc at 1 and 3 h, respectively, and 0.048 for P14 Arc at 12 h.
Activity-regulated cytoskeletal (arc) is involved in AMPA-type glutamate receptor trafficking which regulates long-term synaptic plasticity and memory (Korb and Finkbeiner, 2011). Here, BayK injected P7 pups exhibited a transient decrease in arc expression at 1 h (Fig. 8C; DMSO 0.007 ± 0.001, n = 4, BayK 0.004 ± 0.0008, n = 4, p = 0.032) followed by an increase at 3 h (DMSO 0.003 ± 0.0002, n = 4, BayK 0.005 ± 0.0004, n = 4, p = 0.007) and that had returned to control levels at 12 h (DMSO 0.006 ± 0.001, n = 4, BayK 0.007 ± 0.003, n = 4, p = 0.793). Contrastingly, BayK-injected P14 exhibited a significant decrease in arc expression at 12 h (Fig. 8D; DMSO 0.009 ± 0.002, n = 4, BayK 0.005 ± 0.0005, n = 4, p = 0.048) with no differences at other time points (1 h, DMSO 0.006 ± 0.0004, n = 4, BayK 0.005 ± 0.001, n = 4, p = 0.688; 3 h, DMSO 0.004 ± 0.0005, n = 4, BayK 0.004 ± 0.0008, n = 4, p = 0.38).
Homer1 proteins play a significant role in the organization and intracellular signaling at glutamatergic synapses (Shiraishi-Yamaguchi and Furuichi, 2007). Homer expression is bimodal, with constitutive (Homer1b and c) and/or transient (Homer1a and Ania3) components triggered by neuronal activity (Bottai et al., 2002). Here, measuring all Homer1 transcripts combined, there were no detectable differences between DMSO and BayK at P7 (Fig. 8E; 1 h, DMSO 0.061 ± 0.003, n = 4, BayK 0.06 ± 0.006, n = 4, p = 0.087; 3 h, DMSO 0.061 ± 0.008, n = 4, BayK 0.069 ± 0.008, n = 4, p = 0.51; 12 h, DMSO 0.07 ± 0.007, n = 4, BayK 0.064 ± 0.007, n = 4, p = 0.507), or P14 (Fig. 8F; 1 h, DMSO, 0.077 ± 0.003, n = 4, BayK, 0.083 ± 0.013, n = 4, p = 0.668; 3 h, DMSO 0.072 ± 0.004, n = 4, BayK 0.079 ± 0.008, n = 4, p = 0.484; 12 h, DMSO 0.081 ± 0.008, n = 4, BayK 0.102 ± 0.02, n = 4, p = 0.356).
Discussion
Intrinsic excitability plays a fundamental role in neuronal information processing with firing properties being tightly regulated to maintain a functional range that allows appropriate responses to specific stimuli while also adapting to temporary fluctuations and changes during development (Schulz, 2006; Tien and Kerschensteiner, 2018). The anomalous modification of neuronal excitability has been implicated in various pathologic conditions (Beck and Yaari, 2008). For example, in a mouse model of Rett syndrome, a neurodevelopmental disorder with autistic features, changes in membrane conductance leading to hyperexcitability have been reported in locus coeruleus neurons (Taneja et al., 2009), suggesting that alteration of intrinsic properties might contribute to early onset circuit disruption.
In the rat BLA, application of BayK to acute brain slices from animals in the P7–P21 critical period of development resulted in age-dependent changes in firing properties, consistent with a crucial role of LTCCs in regulating neuronal excitability (Zhang et al., 2020). However, that application of BayK in vivo could trigger permanent alterations in membrane properties of BLA principal neurons which could functionally impact amygdala excitability or associated behaviors at subsequent life stages remained to be explored. In the present study, the electrophysiological properties of BLA principal neurons from juvenile rats at P28 were compared between three experimental groups; each animal cohort receiving BayK or DMSO treatment by stereotaxic bilateral microinjection at P7, P14, or P21.
At P28, there was a detectable trend toward higher firing frequency in response to current injections in principal neurons from animals injected with BayK at P7, although f-I parameters did not show significant statistical differences between BayK and control treatment in any of the P7, P14, or P21 experimental groups. For injections at P7 and P21, there were also no significant long-term changes in gain or rheobase at P28, thus long-term membrane excitability appears not to be affected by BayK administration at these stages.
In contrast, BayK-mediated effects were observed in P28 BLA neurons from animals treated at P14, including a more depolarized membrane resting potential compared with controls. An apparent but nonsignificant increase in Ro, in addition to a marked reduction of time-dependent sag in response to membrane hyperpolarizations was also evident at P28 in BLA neurons from P14 BayK-injected animals.
BLA circuits that mediate behaviors have been characterized using standardized paradigms that evaluate anxiety, social skills and repetitive behaviors. Here, animals receiving bilateral BLA injection at P14 exhibited a significant decrease in time spent with a stranger rat and also increased time with a novel object, indicating impairment in social affiliation abilities. In contrast, animals receiving BayK treatment at P7 or P21 did not show any significant differences compared with DMSO control groups. These results are consistent with the ontogeny of social behaviors in rats showing the emergence of social interest activities between P14 and P28 (Wood et al., 2003).
The three-chamber test provides only partial information regarding social approach since the experimental animal and the stranger rat are not allowed to freely engage in reciprocal social interaction. Therefore, the tendency to explore a conspecific and the ability to sustain social interactions was further evaluated in an open field chamber. In rodents, reciprocal face-to-face sniffing can be related to social dominance, whereas chasing and sniffing of the flank or the anogenital region are exploratory activities indicating propensity to social interaction (Wesson, 2013). Regardless of the age group tested, no significant differences were observed in the duration of sniffing/chasing behaviors of BayK-treated or DMSO-treated animals when allowed to interact with a previously unknown conspecific animal.
The self-grooming behavior of rats confined in a restricted space was also evaluated. Only animals receiving bilateral BLA injection of BayK at P7 exhibited an increased duration of self-grooming periods compared with the DMSO group. No significant differences were found in the other age groups, suggesting that the consolidation of BLA circuitry mediating repetitive grooming behaviors occurred within the first two postnatal weeks. A similar age-dependent profile was observed with the open field test, a behavioral assay measuring anxiety that has been extensively used to examine amygdala function (Ishikawa et al., 2015). Taken together, the results from the behavioral tests support the hypothesis that a functional increase in LTCC activity at certain postnatal critical periods during early neurodevelopment can induce long-term changes in behaviors.
BayK treatment might affect multiple functional levels within the amygdala circuitry. Previous data show that during the critical period of BLA development there is a temporal association in the expression of synaptic plasticity and an ability to learn behavioral responses induced by fear conditioning (Thompson et al., 2008). LTP was examined as a potential cellular mechanism underlying the behavioral alterations observed.
It is well described that local amygdala networks mediate the acquisition, expression and extinction of conditioned fear, and regulate social behaviors (Duvarci and Pare, 2014; Ferrara et al., 2021). Also, that experience-dependent increases in synaptic strength of excitatory projections from the lateral nucleus of the amygdala into the BLA (LA to BLA) controls encoding of anxiety-like behaviors and other emotion-related behavioral responses (Nonaka et al., 2014; Bocchio et al., 2017).
Here, P21 animals regardless of treatment did not exhibit LTP after HFS paired with postsynaptic depolarization. Contrastingly, BayK treatment at either P7 or P14 showed LTP induction with a two-fold amplitude increase in EPSCs in brain slices from P28 animals previously examined in behavioral tests (Figs. 5, 6).
It is known that environmental novelty and/or the exposure to behavioral tests may impact the subsequent induction of LTP (Roman et al., 1999; Moncada and Viola, 2007; Eckert and Abraham, 2010). This process, known as behavioral tagging, is thought to involve the synthesis of synaptic plasticity-related proteins important for the induction and expression of LTP (Moncada and Viola, 2007; Moncada et al., 2011). Here, we tested two additional groups of animals treated at P7 or P14 but that were not exposed to environmental novelty or behavioral testing before electrophysiological recordings. Compared with DMSO controls LTP was significantly induced in the BLA from both P7 and P14 animals that received BayK treatment (Fig. 6). Interestingly, while the P7 BayK group showed potentiation of almost identical magnitude regardless of prior exposure to behavioral experience, the magnitude of LTP in the P14 group was slightly less compared with the P14 BayK group that underwent behavioral tests (Fig. 6B,D). Overall, our results are consistent with a previous study using a valproate rat model of autism where the induction of LTP at thalamic inputs into the LA after behavioral testing, measuring fear conditioning and extinction, did not show behavioral-dependent changes in LTP (Lin et al., 2013).
The data suggest that pharmacological activation of LTCCs during the P7 to P14 critical period produces long-term changes in synaptic plasticity which cannot be replicated in an adult BLA synapse by direct application of an LTCC agonist. It is also possible that the permissive functional changes driving LTP induction at P14 can be further influenced by behavioral tagging triggered by behavioral experiences.
The proportion and subunit composition of ionotropic glutamatergic receptors appears plastic and can undergo changes during development (Lopez de Armentia and Sah, 2003) and with life experiences (Suvrathan et al., 2014; Yi et al., 2017). In the amygdala, stress can generate NMDA containing silent synapses (Suvrathan et al., 2014) as well as alter the expression of AMPA receptor subunits (Yi et al., 2017; Guadagno et al., 2020), resulting in enhanced synaptic plasticity. Given the enhanced induction of LTP at P28 in rats injected with BayK during the critical developmental periods of P7 and P14, we sought to determine whether a change in the expression of glutamatergic receptors in the BLA were associated with this effect. PCR from BayK-injected rats at P14 showed a decrease in the expression of GluA2AMPA receptor subunit. A decrease in GluA2 expression may suggest a shift in AMPA receptors to GluA2 lacking AMPA receptors, which are calcium permeable and capable of inducing LTP (Jia et al., 1996). Accordingly, the enhanced LTP observed here could result from an additive contribution of GluA2 lacking AMPA receptors to LTP at LA-BLA synapses (Whitehead et al., 2013). Interestingly, a Cav1.3-dependent increase of calcium permeable AMPA receptors has been previously shown in the ventral tegmental area-nucleus accumbens circuit of cocaine exposed mice (Martínez-Rivera et al., 2017). AMPA receptor dysregulation has also been linked to the social deficits seen in mice models of ASD, without impacting repetitive behaviors (Kim et al., 2019), consistent with our behavioral data from rats injected with BayK at P14 (Fig. 4A).
Reduced NMDA receptor function has been implicated in the pathophysiology of neuropsychiatric diseases; for instance, a mouse model with decreased GluN1 expression exhibits behavioral traits associated with ASD (Gandal et al., 2012). In line with this, we found a reduction in NMDA receptor subunits GluN1 and GluN2B in rats injected with BayK at P14, consistent with the social deficits exhibited by these rats (Fig. 4A). Conversely, the enhanced synaptic plasticity observed in this group (Fig. 5B), might seem counterintuitive with a reduction in GluN1 as in the BLA NMDA receptors are largely assembled as heterotrimers containing GluN1/GluN2B/GluN2A with the GluN2B subunit being essential for LTP (Delaney et al., 2013). Thus, a reduction in GluN1 and GluN2B would be predicted to reduce the induction of LTP. Nonetheless, combined with our AMPA receptor subunit data, these results suggest that LTCC manipulation at P14 alters glutamatergic receptor subunit expression which impacts synaptic plasticity and social behaviors at maturation. Lastly, in contrast to P14, BayK-injected rats at P7 did not show changes in expression of glutamatergic receptor subunits. Yet, BayK treatment led to an enhancement in LTP induction. This suggests that although there is a similar outcome when LTCCs are acutely enhanced during development, the underlying mechanisms that trigger this likely depends on the stage of development.
The molecular mechanisms underlying plasticity of glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the BLA have been studied under conditions triggering anxiety-like behavioral responses and/or the acquisition and consolidation of fear memories (Manzanares et al., 2005; Ryan et al., 2018). Furthermore, it has been reported that activation of BLA triggered by behavioral training induces a transient increase in mRNA expression of IEGs c-fos, Homer 1a, and arc (Knapska et al., 2007; Imamura et al., 2011; Nonaka et al., 2014; Rajbhandari et al., 2016). In the valproic acid (VPA) model of autism, high levels of Homer 1a mRNA and protein expression in the BLA have been correlated with an impairment of social behavior and fear conditioning (Banerjee et al., 2016), effects evident in adult animals treated at the embryonic stage, consistent with a long-term manifestation of transcriptional and functional changes. c-fos IEG has been widely used as an indirect reporter of neuronal activity as its expression is triggered by calcium influx through activated NMDA receptors and voltage-sensitive LTCCs (Rajbhandari et al., 2016; Hudson, 2018), mRNA expression levels were examined immediately after intra-amygdala injection of BayK during the critical period of development.
Here, BayK produced a significant transient increase of c-fos at 3 h after injection compared with the DMSO control group. No significant changes were observed in the mRNA expression of Homer1a in any experimental group. Statistically significant transient changes in Arc expression (1–3 h) were found between BayK and control groups, although the overall level of Arc expression was comparatively low. The lack of a BayK-evoked response in c-fos expression at P14 is difficult to explain although one could speculate that transcriptional regulation of c-fos at this age is more sensitive to the pattern of stimulus provoking neuronal activation (Tyssowski et al., 2018).
Overall, our results support the notion that altering LTCC activity at certain time points (P7 and P14) within the critical period of postnatal BLA development can produce long-lasting effects in adolescent life. Enhancing Ca2+ influx though LTCCs to then affect firing rates may alter the cues and sequence of events that ordinarily drive BLA immature networks to fully develop. Such dysregulation could eventually modify synaptic strength of LA-BLA inputs and reshape patterns of behavior linked to the amygdala even after a relatively long-time interval following drug treatment. Further, if these Ca2+-dependent alterations occur after the critical period (P21) the mature BLA neural circuits are not affected.
The present study may aid future research aimed at discerning the extent and mechanistic nature of enhanced LTCC activity within the BLA circuitry. Recent optogenetic studies have shown that the encoding of specific behavioral tasks (e.g., retrieval of fear memories or social interactive behavior) requires the activation of distinct subpopulations of BLA neurons (Bocchio et al., 2017). As such, further input-specific approaches using genetic strategies and employing relevant postnatal developmental stages (as demonstrated here and in Zhang et al., (2020) are needed to examine distinct BLA neuron-circuit ensembles and to target specific subsets of neurons expressing LTTCs (DeNardo and Luo, 2017).
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements: We thank Dr. Cathy Schuppli and Dr. Shelly McErlane for advice and supervision of surgical procedures on neonate rats. We also thank Rayshad Gopaul for help with animal care and Kelly Chen for carrying out the blinded scoring of behavioral tests.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
The work was supported by grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (#10677), the Koerner Foundation, and the Canada Research Chair in Biotechnology and Genomics-Neurobiology (T.P.S.).
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.