Abstract
Sensory environments are commonly characterized by specific physical features, which sensory systems might exploit using dedicated processing mechanisms. In the tactile sense, one such characteristic feature is frictional movement, which gives rise to short-lasting (<10 ms), information-carrying integument vibrations. Rather than generic integrative encoding (i.e., averaging or spectral analysis capturing the “intensity” and “best frequency”), the tactile system might benefit from, what we call a “temporally local” coding scheme that instantaneously detects and analyzes shapes of these short-lasting features. Here, by employing analytic psychophysical measurements, we tested whether the prerequisite of temporally local coding exists in the human tactile system. We employed pulsatile skin indentations at the fingertip that allowed us to trade manipulation of local pulse shape against changes in global intensity and frequency, achieved by adding pulses of the same shape. We found that manipulation of local pulse shape has strong effects on psychophysical performance, arguing for the notion that humans implement a temporally local coding scheme for perceptual decisions. As we found distinct differences in performance using different kinematic layouts of pulses, we inquired whether temporally local coding is tuned to a unique kinematic variable. This was not the case, since we observed different preferred kinematic variables in different ranges of pulse shapes. Using an established encoding model for primary afferences and indentation stimuli, we were able to demonstrate that the found kinematic preferences in human performance, may well be explained by the response characteristics of Pacinian corpuscles (PCs), a class of human tactile primary afferents.
Significance Statement
Sensory systems may exploit specific physical features characteristic for the sensory signal at hand. Recent evidence from rodents showed that very short (“local”) signatures in vibrotactile signals may carry significant amount of texture information; this motivated us to investigate whether the human tactile system is able to exploit information carried by short-duration pulse shapes. We demonstrate that the humans are indeed able to extract information from local pulse shapes. We also present evidence that local cues may be perceptually more effective than the classic “global” variables-intensity and frequency. We tag this encoding scheme as “temporally local code” in analogy to the “spatially local code,” long assumed to be implemented in the visual system.
Introduction
Sensory systems evolved in contexts that are characterized by physical features of incoming signals. To overcome any constraints that limit the usage of sensory signals, there exists dedicated, non-generic sensory mechanisms that extract special features to accomplish sensory processing. One well-known and illustrative example is local contour processing in mammalian vision. The feature could certainly be captured to some degree by generic approaches, globally integrating across the visual field, as has been suggested (Campbell and Maffei, 1974). However, today it is commonly thought that special purpose local decoding strategies are at work, for instance, small receptive fields, tuned to orientation and short line segments (Hubel and Wiesel, 1968). Similar ideas recently have attracted heightened attention in the tactile field, as frictional movements are increasingly recognized as a physical constraint specific for fine texture discrimination (Hollins and Risner, 2000; Ritt et al., 2008; Wolfe et al., 2008; Jadhav et al., 2009; Arabzadeh et al., 2014; Delhaye et al., 2014; Barrea et al., 2018; Oladazimi et al., 2018). Frictional movements impose ultra-short-lasting vibrational events of tactile sensors, often referred to as stick-slip events (“slips”; Schwarz, 2016). Rodent whiskers convey information about touched textures, encoded in the kinematic outline of slips, which are easily detected as they stand out well against the noise (Wolfe et al., 2008). Importantly, information about texture in short slip waveforms appeared to be even superior to the one conveyed by the more generic analyses integrating the signal across time, “intensity” (signal averaging) or “best frequency” (spectral decomposition; Oladazimi et al., 2018). In analogy to the mentioned spatial local and global coding principles in vision, we use the terms local/global to label vibrotactile coding principles in the time domain (i.e., temporally local or temporally global). Figure 1A illustrates some core characteristics of temporally local coding. First, it employs short integration windows (for the expected duration of those, see below), and therefore, is (quasi-)instantaneous. Second, it uses detection of outstanding events. The simplest implementation of event detection is thresholding, but there is a host of algorithms that serves the same function in more sophisticated ways (Poor and Hadjiliadis, 2008). These two core characteristics enable temporally local coding schemes to discriminate the shape of short-lasting events. In contrast, temporally global coding uses summing of either signal or frequency contributions across a comparably longer stimulus section. This is not to say that temporally global coding neglects local event shapes, but it is affected by those only in more indirect ways (Fig. 1A; Schwarz, 2016).
Here, we set out to investigate whether humans can use temporally local shapes of vibrotactile waveforms for perception. To be able to compare with classical experimental (LaMotte and Mountcastle, 1975; Johansson, 1978) and modeling work (Saal et al., 2017), we report about perception of pulsatile indentations of the fingertip skin. Indentation, at the first glance, seems not related as much to frictional movements, which often are thought to relate to tangential sideways movement of the fingertip. There can be no doubt, however, that even during tangential movement across surfaces with asperities, the skin is engaged in vertical (indenting) directions as well. The pulsatile nature of our stimuli allowed us to precisely manipulate local shape of single pulses and trade their effect on temporally global coding variables, like intensity and frequency, which rely on cross-pulse integration, as was done before in whiskers (Waiblinger et al., 2015a). The analytic nature of pulsatile stimuli dictates the time criterion differentiating local versus global coding: we presented pulses at rates of 90 and 30 Hz, the interpulse duration of which confined local coding schemes to windows shorter than 11 and 33 ms, respectively. The presentation of these two ranges of pulse rates were firstly motivated to demonstrate possible local coding in two encoding ranges classically defined using long sinusoidal stimuli, the “flutter range” (20–80 Hz), thought to be captured largely by rapidly adapting skin receptors of type 1 and the “Pacinian range” (larger than 150 Hz), thought to be encoded solely by rapidly adapting receptors of type 2 (Talbot et al., 1968; Bolanowski et al., 1988). Second, both windows are compatible with biomechanical measurements of slips in skin and whiskers (Johansson and Westling, 1987; Wolfe et al., 2008; Delhaye et al., 2014; Oladazimi et al., 2018), integration times of primary afferents (Johansson and Birznieks, 2004; Chagas et al., 2013; Pruszynski and Johansson, 2014; Birznieks and Vickery, 2017; Laturnus et al., 2021), and behavior (Gerdjikov et al., 2010, 2018; Stüttgen and Schwarz, 2010; Waiblinger et al., 2015a,b).
In this study, we ask whether humans use local pulse shape for detection of changes in pulsatile stimuli. To this end, the tested stimulus systematically traded changes in pulse shape against changes in rate, as illustrated in Figure 1B.
Materials and Methods
Participants
We recruited a total of 20 neurologically-healthy (self-reported) participants (age: 20–40 years, median 26 years; eight females). Two participants withdrew from the study without providing any reason. Our participant recruitment advertisement discouraged all individuals from participation if they were diagnosed with dyslexia (because it adversely affects tactile acuity (Grant et al., 1999; Laasonen et al., 2001), diabetes (which could result in peripheral neuropathy and action potential conduction delays; Hyllienmark et al., 1995), learning disabilities, nervous system disorders, or had any calluses or injury to the left index fingertip (the tested finger). Based on questions modified from the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), we classified 19 participants as right handed. The study was approved by our institutional research ethics board; all the participants signed the informed consent form and were paid for their participation in the study.
To identify the stimulus feature that the participants used to perform the perceptual tasks, we required the participants to perform well so that we could generate their psychometric function for each task (see below, Perceptual tasks and psychophysics procedure). If any participant performed poorly on any task (session) and we failed to generate a viable psychometric function for those tasks (passing 50% correct), we ran them again on those specific tasks. The reasoning was that if a critical feature varies (see Fig. 2B, orange section, iso-feature-lines a–g) and is absolutely essential to perform the task then eliminating differences in this feature should consistently hinder task performance (see Results, Stimulus space, iso-feature-lines, and terminology). The repetition therefore served as a control whether the inability to perform on the task was because of the assumed lack of variation of a critical feature or because of other reasons. In no case did we detect a systematic inability to perform any of the experiments. We therefore conclude that among the features tested, there is no single feature which is absolutely necessary for performance. One participant was disqualified from the study because the total percent correct for all seven sessions of the pulse shape change experiment (stimulus Range I) was <50%. The data of the disqualified participant and the participants who withdrew from the study are not included in Results. In summary, 10 participants were tested in the pulse shape change (stimulus Range I) experiment, 10 participants were tested in the pulse shape change (stimulus Range II) experiment (three of them also participated in the stimulus Range I experiment); nine of the 10 participants from the stimulus Range I experiment were additionally tested in the pulse rate change experiment as well as the pulse rate and shape (combined) change experiment (Table 1 gives an overview).
Vibrotactile stimulation
We applied passive vibrotactile stimuli (i.e., no finger movement) to the distal pad of the left index finger, using a plastic circular disk of 2.9-mm diameter attached to a galvo-motor (Cambridge Technology; model 6220H). The galvo-motor was driven by a custom-made amplifier, which reproduced highly precise displacements. We calibrated the displacements of the galvo-motor using a laser distance estimator that is sensitive to displacements at the micron resolution. We used MATLAB to generate the stimulus waveform and control the galvo-motor movement by passing voltage waveforms through an analog output channel digitized at 40,000 samples/s at 12-bit resolution via a National Instruments PCI-MIO-16E-1 I/O board.
Pulsatile stimuli were constructed by using one period sinusoids (waveform of a sinusoid extracted from one of its minima to the next) as was done before (Gerdjikov et al., 2010). Two manipulations were done that involved a change in pulse width and/or amplitude. For pulse width, the sinusoids used were drawn from two different frequency ranges: in stimulus Range I, frequencies chosen were between 170 and 240 Hz in steps of 5 Hz, resulting in 15 pulse waveforms that varied in pulse width between 1 s/170 Hz = 5.882 ms and 1 s/240 Hz = 4.167 ms, and the pulse amplitude ranged between 20 and 40 μm, whereas in stimulus Range II, frequencies chosen were between 35 and 65 Hz in steps of 3 Hz, resulting in 10 pulse waveforms that varied in pulse width between 1 s/65 Hz = 15.385 ms and 1 s/35 Hz = 28.571 ms, and the pulse amplitude ranged between 30 and 100 μm. A third manipulation left pulse waveform untouched but changed the pulse frequency (rate), i.e., the inverse of interpulse intervals. Thus, we created three experimental conditions; namely, pulse shape condition (that involved manipulations 1 and 2, mentioned above), pulse rate condition (that involved manipulation 3), and pulse rate-and-shape condition (which is a combination of all the above manipulations; Table 1).
The stimulus for each trial was a seamless concatenation of two pulse trains both 500 ms in duration. The first train of pulses in all trials throughout this study was called the reference stimulus; the pulse rates of the reference stimulus in stimulus Ranges I and II were 90 and 30 Hz, respectively. The pulse shape of the reference stimulus had a width of 5.882 ms and an amplitude of 40 μm for stimulus Range I; in the case of stimulus Range II, the width and amplitude was 28.571 ms and 100 μm, respectively. The second train of pulses was either the same as the reference stimulus (which created the “no-change” trial type) or it involved one of the manipulations mentioned above, which are called the comparison stimuli and this created the “change” trial type (see Fig. 2A, the traces are to scale, but for purposes of visualization the traces contain only three pulses prechange and postchange).
Participants rested their arm on a platform which could be raised or lowered depending on their comfort. To prevent finger movements, the index finger was clamped in a finger housing using a cleft as a rest for the fingernail, in addition, a double-sided tape affixed the plane of the fingernail to the ceiling of the housing. Once the testing finger was securely positioned, we used a tri-axis micromanipulator to adjust and attach the galvo-motor to the distal pad of the left index finger such that the circular disk area was completely covered by the fingertip skin. During the experiment, only the testing region of the fingertip touched the circular disk, we ensured that no other part of the galvo-motor touched any part of the participants’ finger. We asked participants to trim the nail of their testing finger to prevent any possibility of their nail touching any part of the stimulator. The arm platform as well as the galvo-motor platform were separate from each other and seated on an anti-slip and anti-vibration mat. To achieve our definition of “1-mm depth of indentation,” first the experimenter moved the tri-axis micromanipulator closer to the participants’ fingertip until the participant reported feeling the contact of the stimulation disk, and then, using the micromanipulator, the disk attached to the galvo-motor was further indented by 1 mm. From there on, the stimulus pulses further indented the skin.
Perceptual tasks and psychophysics procedure
Table 1 provides a quick glance at all the experimental conditions. The participants were instructed to indicate their decision in a yes/no fashion whether they perceived a change in the stimulus. In other words, if the comparison stimuli followed the reference, then it was a change trial, and “yes” would be the correct response; however, when the reference stimulus continued for the whole stimulus duration (i.e., instead of a comparison stimulus, the reference stimulus followed the reference again), it was a no-change trial, and “no” would be the correct response. Participants pressed one of two buttons (yes/no) on a wireless presenter clicker with their right hand. They received auditory feedback tones (high pitch for correct and low pitch for incorrect responses) after each trial, delivered through wireless headphones. All experiments were conducted in a block-wise fashion. Each experimental session contained three blocks, where each stimulus level was presented 10 times per block (i.e., 3 × 10 trials per stimulus level for the whole session, regardless of the experimental condition). There were equal number of no-change trials in total in every block, and the change as well as the no-change trials were presented in a pseudo-random order. Apart from the auditory feedback tones, participants also saw their performance as the total percent correct for that block. The intertrial interval started after the participant’s response and lasted for 5 s. After each block participants had to take a minimum of 2-min break during which they were encouraged to stand up and walk around.
Participants were tested in 3 different experimental conditions – pulse shape change (in two stimulus ranges), pulse rate change, and pulse rate-and-shape change (Table 1). In the pulse shape change experiment, we tested each participant on stimulus changes drawn from seven iso-feature-lines shown in Figure 2B (seven sessions were performed, testing one iso-feature-line per session; see Table 1). Each session, in the stimulus Range I, contained 420 change and 420 no-change trials, and in the stimulus Range II, there were 300 each of change and no-change trials. The iso-feature-lines contained stimuli, from which we extracted three local and six global encoding variables. The three local variables were (maximum position, maximum velocity and maximum acceleration). In the stimulus space depicted in Figure 2B, the three local encoding variables were identical to the one obtained from the reference stimulus on iso-feature-lines a, d, and g, respectively. The six global encoding variables quantified intensity, e.g., the mean of the vibrotactile signal; they were calculated always across the entire 500-ms stimulus. The following intensity formulations were tested (in brackets we detail the respective iso-feature-line in the stimulus crossing space; Fig. 2B): mean absolute velocity (a); mean squared velocity (b); mean absolute velocity taken to the power of three (c); mean absolute acceleration (d); mean squared acceleration (e); mean absolute velocity taken to the power of 3 (f). In two instances an iso-feature-line of a local encoding variable was found to be congruent with one of the global encoding variables (“maximum position” = “mean absolute velocity”; “maximum velocity” = “mean absolute acceleration”); hence, only seven iso-feature-lines were tested in total. In the pulse rate change experiment, stimulus changes were exclusively based on changes in pulse frequency (
Participants wore padded headphones throughout the study to reduce ambient acoustic noise and receive auditory feedback. In experiments that involved a change in pulse rate, there was a very low acoustic signal emitted by the galvo motor reflecting the change in pulse rate, hearable when locating the unpadded ear next to the motor. To securely rule out the possibility of unwanted auditory cues as experimental confound, we played out loud white noise from a speaker next to the galvo in the two experiments containing pulse rate changes. Participants were uninformed about the details of stimulus change. Following the completion of pulse rate and shape change experiment, when asked, none of the participants was aware that the pulse rate (along with the pulse width and amplitude) of the target stimuli changed.
Statistical analysis
To each participant’s performance (proportion reported “change”) in each task, using the dedicated analysis software psignifit (Wichmann and Hill, 2001), we fitted a mixture model cumulative normal psychometric function P(x) of the following form:
Next, to estimate the population mean for each iso-feature-line, we implemented a Bayesian Hierarchical analysis (see Tong et al., 2016). We represented the participants’ threshold as normally distributed over the whole stimulus range (pulse width from 5.882 ms down to 4.167 ms) with unknown mean μ and SD σ (ranging between 0.5 and 30). The probability of a participant’s data given (μ, σ) can be written as:
Here, the first term in the integrand is proportional to the threshold PDF of each participant (acquired from psignifit), because we assume a uniform prior over all possible values of θ (i.e., 5.882–4.167 ms). Thus, the likelihood of joint distribution of each (μ, σ) of complete dataset D, including all 10 participants can be represented as:
Finally, we marginalized over σ and obtained a PDF of the population mean; we report the mode of this distribution as the mean of the population along with the Bayesian 95% confidence interval.
Simulation of primary afferents
We used TouchSim (v1.1; available at https://github.com/bensmaiaLab/touchSim), a freely available simulation tool based on indentation biomechanics followed by integrate and fire neurons (Saal et al., 2017). The algorithm allows to selectively model the primary afferents in the glabrous skin across the entire index finger with realistic densities of end organs of the three primary afferent classes slowly adapting type 1 (SA1), rapidly adapting type 1 (RA1), and rapidly adapting of the Pacinian corpuscle (PC)-associated type (also called rapidly adapting type 2). The model accepts skin indentations of adjustable size and at location on the glabrous skin of the finger. Our simulations used the approximate location ([−10,1], in coordinates of the algorithm) and extent of the indenting disk (diameter of 2.9 mm). The stimulation disk was placed at a preindentation depth of 1 mm below the skin surface. Stimuli presented were identical to those applied to the humans’ fingertip. The model ran simulations of 564 SA1 (Merkel cells), 933 RA1 (Meissner Corpuscles), and 192 PCs, distributed across the fingertip.
The model returned spike trains from each of the simulated primary afferents, which we analyzed further. We modelled responses for all stimuli (reference and comparison) used in this study (Figs. 5, 6; Extended Data Fig. 5-1). All analyses presented are based on averages of 100 model runs (= stimulus presentations). The first coding symbol extracted was either the “global” spike count, i.e., the sum of spikes evoked across the stimulus length (500 ms) of reference and comparison stimulus. This gave an ensemble of 100 spike responses (one per run/trial). The second coding ensemble read-out was composed of “local” spike responses to each pulse [this gave an ensemble of 45 × 100 responses (number of pulses times trials)]. Neurometric performance was estimated by calculating the area under the curve (AUC) of the receiver operating characteristic (Stüttgen et al., 2011) using the response distributions obtained from the model runs with reference and comparison stimulus. AUC is the probability of a correct stimulus identification of a binary classifier confronted with a random pick of spike responses from the total sample. In the current framework, where only a change of stimulus had to be detected AUC values of 1 and 0 are equivalent and indicate optimal performance, while a value of 0.5 indicates random performance. We therefore calculated discriminability
d [ranging from 0 (random) to 1 (100% discrimination)] as
Extended Data Figure 5-1
Population spike responses of all three modelled primary afferent classes. A, The model contained 564 slowly adapting primary afferents type 1 (SA1, green), and 933 rapidly adapting primary afferents of type 1 (RA1, blue), all located close to the indented site on the fingertip (conventions of the hand drawing as in Fig. 5, for RA1 only the distal phalanx of the index finger is shown). Spiking in response to one trial (reference stimulus of Range I, duration 500 ms) is presented as raster plots. Each line represents the spiking of one modelled neuron; note that a majority of neurons did not spike at all to this stimulus (for PC data see Fig. 5). B, Summary response profiles for all three modelled primary afferent populations (rows of graphs). Each graph plots on the ordinate either the spike numbers (top, mean ± SD) or the mean absolute difference spike numbers between reference and comparison stimulus (bottom), summed across the neuronal population. The abscissae hold the reference stimulus (leftmost data point) as well as all comparison stimuli (to the right with decreasing pulse width). Red color labels data from Range I, green indicates data from Range II. Each column of graphs presents the responses to one iso-feature-line (labeled as in Fig. 2). The iso-feature-lines yielding worst population performances of participants for the two ranges (d, g) are marked at the bottom (compare Fig. 4). The ovals mark the indiscriminate responses of the PC population across reference and comparison stimuli to these iso-feature-lines. SA1 and RA1 afferents show indiscriminate responses to stimuli on line a (SA1) and b (RA1), and thus, by themselves, are unable to explain human performance. Download Figure 5-1, TIF file.
Primary afferents are known to lack spike adaptation (Bensmaïa et al., 2005; Leung et al., 2005). Therefore, local and global coding symbols gave near identical results as long as the pulse frequency was not varied. As soon as differences in pulse rates were applied, however (data presented in Fig. 6), the global coding symbol (sum of spikes across 500 ms) of all classes of primary afferents would indicate all changes in near error-less fashion (i.e., discrimination close to 1). This has been noted before (Stüttgen et al., 2006; Gerdjikov et al., 2010; Arabzadeh et al., 2014), and excludes that the perceptual system is able to use the full global information present on the level of primary afferents. Figure 6, therefore, reports exclusively results obtained with calculating the ensemble generated by response to single pulses.
Results
We used a yes/no psychophysical change detection task to test whether humans can discriminate pulsatile skin indentations. The participants were presented with a 1-s-long pulsatile stimulus and were asked whether they felt a change in the middle of this vibration. The stimuli were composed of two 500-ms-long pulse trains that were seamlessly concatenated, a “reference” followed by a “comparison” stimulus (correct response “yes”), or the “reference” again (correct response “no”). Stimuli were delivered orthogonal to (i.e., indenting into) the skin of the left index fingertip. The comparison stimuli changed from the reference in three different ways– a change in pulse rate, shape, or rate-and-shape (Fig. 1B; for an overview about all experimental variables used in this study, see Table 1).
Stimulus space, iso-feature-lines, and terminology
The first variable characterizing a change between reference and comparison stimulus, pulse rate, determines the number of pulses per time interval (Fig. 2A, blue), and can only be decoded by integrating across pulses. It, therefore, requires “temporally global” decoding, either summing signal (intensity) or frequency contributions (spectrum). The second variable was the shape of the individual pulses, which came in two variants, namely changes in pulse amplitude and/or width (Fig. 2A, magenta). It is noteworthy that a seemingly local manipulation, in principle detectable instantaneously by observing pulse kinematics, also affects the global variable, the intensity (i.e., the signal summed across pulses; Fig. 1B). Individual pulses always had the sinusoidal shape (single-period sine-wave starting from one minimum and ending at the next), starting from the resting position (at 1 mm skin indentation), and protruding further toward larger depths. For pulse shape, two parameters were manipulated independently, pulse width and amplitude, by changing the frequency and/or amplitude of the sinusoid that was used for constructing the pulse (
Pulse amplitudes and widths were chosen from a two-dimensional stimulus space shown in Figure 2B (pulse rate:
The orange stimulus space covers a wide range of pulse width and amplitude combinations, from the upper left corner (our reference stimulus) where the pulses are wider and higher compared with the ones down in the lower right corner where the pulses are lower and narrower (i.e., steeply rising pulses). That is, the orange stimulus space is characterized by stimuli that trade decreasing amplitude with increasing steepness. In fact, starting from the reference stimulus in the upper left corner, it is possible to trace out lines through the orange stimulus space, on which important coding variables (“features”) remain unchanged, we tag them as “iso-feature-lines.” In this study, we considered seven such iso-feature-lines (labeled a–g in Fig. 2B). Amongst them are six iso-feature-lines (a to f) that hold variants of global intensity variables constant, defined as “mean velocity” (line a), “mean squared velocity” (line b), “mean cubic velocity” (line c), “mean acceleration” (line d), “mean squared acceleration” (line e), and “mean cubic acceleration” (line f). Note that to increase clarity, we omitted the attribute “absolute” (i.e., directions and signs of velocity and acceleration are not considered – all variables are positive valued). Moreover, the seven iso-feature-lines also comprise three lines that hold local variables constant describing the “maximal position” (i.e., maximal displacement), “maximal velocity,” and “maximal acceleration” of a pulse (iso-feature-lines a, d, and g, respectively). The first two respective local iso-feature-lines are in fact congruent with the global iso-feature-lines mean velocity and mean acceleration (lines a and d), whereas “maximal acceleration,” the third local variable, is characterized by the bottom-most iso-feature-line (g) in Figure 2B. Note that throughout this article we use the letters a–g to label iso-feature-lines, but also to refer to the intensities and kinematic variables held constant on them.
Having identified the relevant stimulus range we decided to pick comparison stimuli located on the seven iso-feature-lines. We reasoned that if one of these encoding variables a critical for performing the change detection task, the performance on the corresponding iso-feature-line should be poor compared with the other ones. To quantify this, we designed our testing sessions under the presumption that decreasing pulse width (from the pulse width of the reference stimuli) will increase the participants’ probability of detecting a change and the pulse width at which the probability to detect the change is 50% will be the threshold. In other words, in all the iso-feature-line sessions we reduced the pulse width of the comparison stimuli (while adjusting for the pulse amplitude as guided by the iso-feature-line definition) to finally identify the iso-feature-line session that yielded the highest threshold. We tested each participant through seven sessions corresponding to each iso-feature-line; the sessions were randomized such that each participant was tested on a unique sequence of sessions probing the seven iso-feature-lines (Table 1).
Comparison between isolated changes in pulse rate and shape
In anticipation of results that we will report in detail further below, we found that among the sessions on the seven iso-feature-lines, stimuli on line d gave rise to the highest threshold, i.e., worst performance. To start comparing the effect of local and global cues on performance, we selected stimuli on line d, with the worst performance among the stimulus sets containing local cues (Fig. 1A, orange; compare Table 1 under “shape change condition”), and pitted them against stimuli that are devoid of local cues, i.e., reference and comparison stimuli containing pulses of identical shape but different rate (Fig. 1A, blue; compare Table 1 under “rate change condition”). In the blue stimulus set the pulse rates changed from 90 Hz (reference) to 135 Hz in 10 stimulus steps (i.e., 90, 95, …, 130, 135 Hz). We present data obtained from eight of nine people tested, as the ninth participant did not give us any logistic fit that crossed the probability of 0.5 of detected change. On average, participants required a pulse rate change of 35% (SD 13%) to reliably detect it 50% of the time. The orange stimulus set included 15 pulse shapes, whose width varied from 5.882 to 4.167 ms (the interpulse intervals of reference and the comparison stimuli were identical), and amplitudes were defined by line d (Fig. 2A, right, and panel B). For detection of changes in the pulse shape task, we obtained valid psychometric curves (threshold higher than detection probability of 0.5) from all 10 participants.
Although one set of experiments dealt with changes in pulse rate and the other with pulse shapes, the performance on the two types of stimulus changes can be plotted in one graph according to their intensity content. Figure 2C presents the mean performance of the population of participants in the two experiments plotted across intensity formulated as mean velocity. The success of 8 out of 9 participants to detect pulse rate changes first of all demonstrated that humans in principle are able to use global variables as a basis for their decision. However, the comparison of the two behavioral datasets based on change of intensity in Figure 2C suggested that pulse shape changes (magenta) were detected in a different way (i.e., had a lower intensity threshold) than the ones based on pulse rate (blue).
However, the premises for the comparison made in Figure 2C are, first, that intensity is actually a coding variable, and second, that its formulation as mean velocity is correct. The first premise is firmly supported by a host of studies starting from early times of tactile research (LaMotte and Mountcastle, 1975). The second premise is definitely weaker, but has been supported by work comparing some of the possible formulations (Arabzadeh et al., 2003; Hipp et al., 2006; Gerdjikov et al., 2010). Nevertheless, in many studies on the tactile system the formulation of intensity was chosen for reasons of parsimony or tradition. In order to strengthen our argument, we took the opportunity that was provided by the outline of our relevant stimulus range and the set of iso-feature-lines running through it (Fig. 2B), and performed a more systematic analysis: we reasoned that the true intensity formulation for indentation stimuli (if it exists) must be associated within this stimulus range, where relevant intensity formulations do not vary considerably and performance accordingly is relatively poor. We therefore repeated the plot of Figure 2C with combinations of data from all seven iso-feature-lines (colored curves in Fig. 2D), and scaling them to the six intensity formulations a–f, whose iso-feature-lines span the whole stimulus range (the six graphs in Fig. 2D). We did not find any intensity formulation that would group psychometric curves consistently close to the one from pulse rate changes. They are either concentrated at sites indicating far better performance than rate changes (e.g., mean velocity and mean acceleration, labeled a and d), or they are dispersed across a large intensity range (e.g., “mean squared” or “cubic acceleration,” intensities e and f). We argue that a consistent grouping of curves around the one found with rate changes would be required to conclude that the respective intensity formulation was perceptually dominant. The absence of such a grouping suggests that none of the intensity variables singularly determined the performance on the present tasks. The most conspicuous mismatch was the one found when scaling to intensity d (mean acceleration; Fig. 2D, lower left). Actually, the poor performance on line d, mentioned before, had made mean acceleration a major candidate for intensity-based coding. However, the huge difference in performance between rate and shape change stimuli, when scaling with this variable, casted serious doubts on its relevance. From these initial results we, thus, suggest that the observed performance differences are likely based on differences in local pulse shapes rather than intensity.
Comparison of changes when presenting simultaneous changes in pulse rate and shape
So far, we tested shape and rate changes in different sessions, leaving room for the possibility that the participants (or their tactile systems, respectively), were able to adapt to these different tasks. We therefore aimed to support the finding in a more direct way, trading pulse rate against shape within the same comparison stimulus (Fig. 3A). As a starting point for this experiment, we chose again iso-feature-line d (Fig. 3B). The poor performance on this line would predict that the variables kept constant on it (i.e., global mean acceleration or local “maximal velocity”) are likely relevant for coding. Further, the poor performance would provide added pulses a good chance to result in measurable improvements in performance (conversely, basing the experiment on iso-feature-lines with stronger performance would run the risk that performance there is already saturated preventing the assessment of improvement by adding pulses). Adding pulses of identical shape to comparison stimuli on line d generated line d’ (Fig. 3, gray; also see Table 1, pulse rate-and-shape change experiment), which keeps being a local iso-feature-line identical to line d (because of identical pulses), but introduces differences in global intensity (via the increased pulse rate). We selected an increase of pulse rate of 15 Hz (i.e., comparison stimuli on line d’ have a pulse rate of 105 Hz as opposed to 90 Hz for line d), which is subthreshold based on participants performance in the pulse rate change experiment (compare Fig. 2C, the stimulus is indicated by the leftmost blue hexagon). Expectedly then, increasing pulse rate did not significantly improve the performance in iso-feature-line d’ over what was seen in iso-feature-line d [change of threshold from 1.272 ms (SD 0.27 ms) to 1.101 ms (SD 0.19 ms); t test: t = −2.084, n = 8, p = 0.08; effect size: discriminability = 0.74]. In other words, the participants largely failed to use the increased global intensity variable in the comparison stimuli and continued informing their perceptual decisions using local cues. Therefore, our next manipulation aimed at deciding whether the same intensity change, this time realized by reducing pulse shapes further (thereby, strengthening local cues), would have a better effect on performance. To test this, we compensated intensity (that was previously increased via rate change in line d’) by reducing the pulse shape. This resulted in line d’’ (Fig. 3, red; also see Table 1). Performance on line d’’ significantly improved in comparison to line d’ [gray to red; change of threshold from 1.101 ms (SD 0.194 ms) to 0.764 ms (SD = 0.368 ms); t test: t = 2.868, n = 8, p = 0.024; effect size: discriminability = 0.98]. That is, relative to the reference (d, magenta), increasing the pulse rate had no effect on performance (d’, gray) whereas changing the pulse shape enhanced it (d’’, red). In other words, there is no significant difference in performance between d’ and d, likely because there is no difference in the local cue. In contrast, there is a significant difference in performance between d’ and d’’ likely because here local cues vary, while intensities are kept the same, and rate change alone has been shown before to be perceptually ineffective.
The tactile system does not prefer a specific kinematic variable
Translating the width of our sinusoidal pulses into frequency, i.e., the variable classically considered when testing the tactile system with long sinusoidal stimuli, it becomes evident that so far we only probed a small portion of the wide tactile range accessible to the human tactile perceptual system (Bolanowski et al., 1988; for a justification of the comparison of stimulus ranges composed of sinusoids in the classical studies and our sinusoidal pulses, see Discussion). The subrange investigated thus far (Range I in Fig. 4A; pulse widths were between 5.882 and 4.167 ms) corresponds to sinusoidal frequencies between 170 and 240 Hz, which is well within the range of frequencies classically called “the Pacinian range”; a range that is dominated by responses of rapidly adapting primary afferents type 2, which innervate the PCs. We therefore aimed, firstly, to quantify performance on all iso-feature-lines of Range I, and secondly, compare it to a second range, the so-called “flutter range”, located at much lower frequencies, known to predominantly activate rapidly adapting primary afferents type 1 (Range II in Fig. 4A; amplitudes 100–30 μm and widths
The performances of exemplary participants on the stimulus Ranges I and II (Fig. 4B,E), already hint that the preference does not generalize across the entire stimulus space. As mentioned before, iso-feature-line d (maximum velocity) yielded the worst performance in Range I (Fig. 4B), while in Range II the worst performance was seen with iso-feature-line g (maximum acceleration; Fig. 4E). Population data, thresholds as well as psychometric curves, confirm this impression (Fig. 4C,D,F,G). For statistical appraisal we computed the discriminability (ranging from 0 to 1; see Materials and Methods), as well as the p values obtained from a t test (two-tailed, paired samples), comparing thresholds from iso-feature-line d to all others for the Range I (discriminability = [0.96, 0.64, 0.06, ∼, 0.66, 0.82, 0.80]; order: line [a, b, c, d, e, f, g], note the fourth position is marked with “∼” as it is the comparison of line d with itself). The corresponding p values were p = [0.015, 0.044, 0.624, ∼, 0.357, 0.049, 0.055]; and number of tested participants were n = [10, 9, 9, ∼, 9, 10, 10]. Similarly for Range II, we computed discriminability comparing the iso-feature-line g (maximum acceleration) to all the other iso-feature-lines, and ordered the results as above: discriminability = [0.88, 0.78, 0.68, 0.64, 0.38, 0.14, ∼]; the corresponding p value in the t tests were p = [0.027, 0.086, 0.134, 0.166, 0.413, 0.989, ∼], and the number of participants were n = [10, 10, 10, 10, 10, 10, ∼]. The best fit logistic functions corrected for false alarm rates to the pooled data from 10 participants for the experiments on isolines (a, d, g) are shown in Figure 4D,G. The different sequence in performance can be readily observed from them (a→g→d in Fig. 4D vs a→d→g Fig. 4G). We note that both tasks were challenging for the participants. This fact was reflected by a consistently high false alarm rate of [0.12, 0.18, 0.16, 0.14, 0.09, 0.04, 0.07] in stimulus Range I, and [0.03, 0.03, 0.04, 0.04, 0.02, 0.04, 0.05] in Range II (same order as before). In summary, for human vibrotactile perception, the relative performance across iso-feature-lines was distinct in the two studied stimulus ranges, a result that does not argue in favor of a single consistent weighing of kinematic parameters for encoding; instead, participants seemed to switch between encoded variables to perform the task in the different stimulus subspaces.
How is change detection related to primary afferent activity?
The schematic in Figure 5A plots again our stimulus subspaces within the larger perceptual range, but now adds the threshold curve of PC primary afferents (Freeman and Johnson, 1982). As our stimulus spaces are quite small, relative to the curvature of the threshold curve, it is possible to approximate the curve with straight line segments, and thus, we arrive at “iso-response-lines” (Fig. 5A, green and blue lines). The primary afferent response to a pair of discriminanda located on an iso-feature-line is then governed by the angle with which an iso-feature-line runs relative to the afferent’s iso-response orientation. For example, a pair of stimuli located on an iso-feature-line would engage a class of afferents similarly if the iso-feature-line is parallel to the iso-response orientation, whereas the stimuli would engage the afferents most differently, if the iso-feature-line and iso-response orientation are orthogonal. The schematic comparison of the cited classical work to our data in Figure 5A demonstrates how the orientation of the PC threshold line can be transferred to our stimulus ranges and respective iso-feature-lines. It turns out that, to a first approximation, the angle between iso-response-lines and iso-feature-lines is different in the two stimulus subspaces (Fig. 5A, blue vs green lines). From here it is straight forward to hypothesize that minute changes in the angle of iso-response-lines and iso-feature-lines are responsible for the observed difference in preferred coding variables in Subspaces I and II (Fig. 4).
We put this hypothesis to the test by taking advantage of an established model of primary afferent responses based on quasi-static and dynamic skin mechanics linked to an integrate-and-fire neuronal model (Saal et al., 2017). The model very precisely recreates the classical threshold and entrainment curves of the three most important classes of primary afferents in primates (Saal et al., 2017; see their Fig. 7), and its output therefore, as argued above, can be compared with our human behavioral data. Importantly, the model accepts any waveform of indentation stimuli, it is sensitive to stimulation location, and it realistically simulates multiple end organs distributed across the glabrous skin. We only quantified the discriminability carried by spikes of the PC class of primary afferents (also called rapidly adapting type 2), as they qualitatively reproduced the main aspects of our behavioral results (Fig. 5), while the other two (SA1 and RA1) did not. (The spike responses of all modelled primary afferent classes are presented in the Extended Data Fig. 5-1.) We modelled the spike output of 192 PC afferents to our pulsatile stimuli, distributed in spatially realistic fashion across the index finger as shown in Figure 5B. We ran the model presenting the reference stimuli and all stimuli on the seven iso-feature-lines, covering both stimulus ranges presented to the human participants in this study. We plot the discriminability of population PC spike counts calculated from the number of spikes evoked by the reference stimulus versus that obtained with the comparison stimuli for three isolines in each stimulus range. We performed this analysis by summing across responses to all pulses contained in one stimulus (reference vs all comparison stimuli), to be consistent with the psychophysical experiments. However, because of the known lack of spike rate adaptation in primary afferents (for details, see Discussion), captured by the model, we did not find relevant differences when presenting single sinusoidal pulses or 500-ms-long pulsatile sequences to the model (the differences were smaller than the line thickness of the plots in Fig. 5C,D). So, for discrimination analysis, the quantification of primary afferent activity based on responses to single and repetitive pulses can be used interchangeably. Couching these long-known facts in the terminology introduced in this study, we can call primary afferents as “temporally local encoders.” The neurometric curves of the modelled PC population recreated the discriminability of comparison stimuli picked from different iso-feature-lines in the same sequence as observed with human psychometric curves. In the stimulus Range I the sequence was a→g→d and in Range II it was a→d→g (Fig. 5C,D; compare Fig. 4D,G). This relationship is predicted by the co-linearity of iso-feature-lines and iso-response line of PCs approximated in Figure 5A. In summary, the modeling exercise suggested that human performance on iso-feature-lines, as studied here, may be largely determined by PC population spiking activity.
Next, we aimed at recreating our experimental findings with mixed changes of pulse rate and shape as well using the model PC spiking. Again, by feeding the respective stimuli to the same model as before, we successfully recreated major characteristics of the psychophysical finding (Fig. 6). In this approach, we only modeled local coding (i.e., measuring spike responses to single pulses, typically consisting in 0 or 1 spike per pulse) because integrating the response across pulses rendered PC-based discriminability error-free (i.e., invariably reaching a discriminability value of 1), a phenomenon consistent with our notion (and with the results of numerous previous studies) that afferents are indeed temporally local encoders (Gerdjikov et al., 2010; Stüttgen and Schwarz, 2010; Birznieks et al., 2019). A trivial consequence of measuring responses to single pulses, from neurons devoid of spike adaptation, is that increasing pulse frequency does not affect discriminability. This is expressed by the coincident magenta and gray curves [Fig. 6, right; note, this is a discrepancy to the experimental data because human tactile system does integrate to a certain degree (compare Fig. 2C), very likely using neurons upstream of primary afferents]. The interesting part was thus modeling the second step of our experiment, compensating intensity increment (caused by additional pulses) by reduction of pulse amplitudes (red curve). Compared with the reference stimulus (magenta), reducing pulse amplitudes indeed increased discriminability by ∼0.2 (red), a fact that qualitatively matches human performance (compare Fig. 3C), and is compatible with the hypothesis that locally encoded information in primary afferent spikes is used as such, without relevant further integration, by the human tactile system up to the perceptual level.
Discussion
Using change detection of pulsatile stimuli, we present evidence that humans are able to discriminate shapes of single pulses in an instantaneous, and thus temporally local fashion. Our results favor the hypothesis that tactile encoding of pulsatile stimuli is sensitive to the shape of pulses to an extent not thought before, and that encoding of pulse rate, a purely global variable and major candidate encoding variable discussed in classical work, contributes relatively little. Moreover, our results are difficult to reconcile with intensity playing a major role in discrimination of pulse series, another global variable, and major classical contender. Interestingly, discrimination performance was not based on a unique kinematic variable describing the pulse shape. Rather, the psychophysical performance was predicted by computer simulations incorporating classical threshold and entrainment curves of the population of a digit’s PC primary afferents (Freeman and Johnson, 1982; Saal et al., 2017). In fact, we found that classical primary afferent and behavioral threshold curves can be well explained within a framework of temporally local coding, simply by considering instantaneous kinematics of long sinusoids, the classical stimuli, rather than their frequency. Based on such reinterpretation of classical data, we suggest that PCs, and in fact the ascending human tactile system including the perceptual levels, encode indentation pulses in a temporally local fashion. Such local coding is surely not exclusive, but our results are compatible with the notion of a dominant effect on perception (with the qualification that we presented only analytical stimuli that are quite distinct from natural skin stimulation). The encoded kinematic variable is not unique. Rather, it seems to depend on pulse width, reflecting “acceleration” with slow, “velocity” for the middle, and “position” with the fastest pulses.
Local coding
As the three coding symbols “global intensity,” global “rate,” and “local shape” (Fig. 1) are partially dependent on each other, our experimental logic relied on trading them against each other as done before in the rat whisker-related system (Waiblinger et al., 2015a,b). The central role is taken by intensity, which in Figure 1A is defined as the “mean signal.” We eliminate intensity from the analysis by plotting behavioral results across it, thus providing a common base, on which the effect of the other two variables, local shape and global rate, can be compared. However, we had to deal with the problem that the exact formulation of intensity is far from clear. The early finding that subjective intensity is not only dependent on the sine amplitude but also on the frequency (LaMotte and Mountcastle, 1975) gave rise to more general definitions that are applicable to random vibrotactile stimuli, like mean velocity (Arabzadeh et al., 2003; Gerdjikov et al., 2010), or “power” which is proportional to mean squared velocity (Hipp et al., 2006). Here, we exploited the delineation of a relevant stimulus range, marked by poor responses, to systematically investigate a more extensive and relevant selection of intensity formulations. The six intensity formulations chosen are the relevant ones because their variance within the poor-response stimulus range is small, i.e., their iso-feature-lines span (and quite evenly) cover that range (Fig. 2B). Based on this logic, other intensity formulations are rendered implausible to play a role, at least for this particular set of stimuli: for one, intensity formulations based on position traverse the first quadrant of the plot (characterized by increasing pulse amplitude and decreasing width). This quadrant gives rise to very high detection rates, making position-based intensity formulations unlikely candidates to affect perception. Similar reasoning applies to derivatives of position higher than acceleration (e.g., jerk, etc.) and the usage of higher powers (i.e., >3). Those iso-feature-lines are located in the lower left of the fourth quadrant, another high-performance region (Fig. 2B, the gray region, in which changes of pulse amplitudes dominate those of pulse widths). Finally, velocity or acceleration can be taken to powers picked from rational numbers before summing, and thus define additional, non-tested iso-feature-lines within the relevant stimulus range. However, the possibility that we missed outstandingly relevant intensity formulations is small because the coverage of the stimulus space by the iso-feature-lines investigated here is quite dense and exhaustive. From the participants’ performance on these iso lines, we have no reason to assume that their performance is mapped in grossly non-smooth ways across the stimulus range; therefore, it is likely that intensity formulations with rational powers would smoothly interpolate the results obtained by the six formulations picked here.
The exercise of scaling psychophysical data from separate shape and rate changing experiments down to these intensity formulations did not yield an unambiguous explanation in terms of intensity coding: the curves obtained from these experiments plotted on the exhaustive list of possible intensity formulations (discussed in the previous paragraph) never consistently matched (Fig. 2D). A first conclusion from this finding was that intensity coding did not play a dominant role in our experiments, and that the other two coding variables, shape and/or rate, must have played a significant role for the generation of responses. Secondly, minimum and maximum detection probabilities reached in shape-change experiments exceeded those observed with rate-change stimuli, suggesting that participants mastered the former at greater ease as compared to the latter, pointing to a dominance of local cues over rate cues.
One argument barring the conclusion of importance of local cues would be that the sessions probing the stimuli on different iso-feature-lines and the rate-change experiments were conducted in a block-wise (one session one stimulus set) fashion, and the participants, or rather their tactile systems, may have been adaptive enough to read out a different cue each session. This possibility was excluded by combining shape and rate changes in the same stimulus. As a starting point for this experiment, we chose stimuli located on the iso-feature-line for mean acceleration (line d) for the following reasons: first, possible experiments based on other iso-feature-lines are less meaningful, as the response on all other iso-feature-lines is much better and may offer little chance to measure potential perceptual effects of our intensity manipulations via shape and rate. Second, scrutinizing the stimuli for their kinematic content revealed that changing mean acceleration led to identical proportions of change in in the intensity formulation based on the two neighboring derivatives of position (mean velocity and mean jerk). Thus, a grossly different contribution of these alternative intensity formulations cannot explain the results. Our finding of a largely asymmetric perceptual effect of such intensity-balanced rate-increase and shape-decrease supports our conclusion that the local shape cue is unarguably important for perception.
Role of primary afferent responses for tactile perception
We found that human change detection performance cannot be explained by one uniquely encoded kinematic variable. Instead, performance was qualitatively explained by spiking of modeled PC afferents. The principal finding that primary afferent spiking characteristics directly determine behavioral decisions has long been reported in classic studies comparing psychometric and neurometric curves (Talbot et al., 1968; Johansson et al., 1982; Bolanowski et al., 1988). However, these researchers interpreted their results uniquely in the framework of global coding: the expectation that the end-point of vibrotactile perception was either intensity or frequency led to the view that a sine wave is an entity, the neuronal responses to which had to be temporally integrated to feed to perception. A second important aspect of these works was that detection thresholds (not discrimination thresholds) were compared with primary afferent spiking. That is, the ability to discriminate minute difference in local shape as investigated in the current study was not directly addressed in the previous studies.
The present data support the notion that (1) local shape is used by the human tactile perceptual system; and (2) this information, at least for the stimuli used here, may be carried by PC spiking. We wish to emphasize that local coding is not mutually exclusive with global coding: in fact, we explicitly show that pulse rate changes, requiring purely global coding, can be perceived. Rather, we argue that local coding may come on top of global ones to improve perceptual capabilities. Based on our findings, we suggest a reinterpretation of the results of the classical studies to include aspects of local coding. We argue, firstly, that primary afferents are local encoders, i.e., in the framework of pulsatile stimuli, they use integration windows that are smaller than interpulse intervals. Thus, in the primary afferent’s view, a sine wave is not considered as an entity, but rather as a series of pulses to which spikes are generated according to instantaneous shape. This view is widely supported by the observation that myelinated primary afferents do not show considerable spike rate adaptation across repetitive stimuli of durations used here (i.e., <1 s; Freeman and Johnson, 1982; Whitsel et al., 2001; Deschênes et al., 2003; Leung et al., 2005; Bensmaïa et al., 2005; Chagas et al., 2013). Further, neuronal encoding studies failed in supporting the use of long integration windows (Vallbo et al., 1984; Jones et al., 2004; Chagas et al., 2013). Second, we argue that local shape information is preserved on ascending tactile pathways of rodents (Laturnus et al., 2021), a fact that may find its expression in the common finding of entrainment to repetitive events up to primate S1 (Mountcastle et al., 1969; Salinas et al., 2000). That way information of local shape may become accessible as well to the perceptual system. Moreover, neurometric performance based on decoding spike trains by integration is commonly found to be far superior than the actual psychophysical performance (Gerdjikov et al., 2010; Stüttgen and Schwarz, 2010; Birznieks et al., 2019), a finding at odds with the dominant use of spike integration as the basis for perception.
The schematic in Figure 7 attempts to visualize this reinterpretation using the PC-driven detection performance reported by Bolanowski et al. (1988; it is largely consistent with neurophysiological PC recordings by Freeman and Johnson, 1982). The threshold curve is typically rendered by using frequency of the sinusoidal stimulus mapped on the abscissa and its amplitude on the ordinate. We added information about the local variables, i.e., maximal position, velocity, and acceleration as color gradients, calculated from the sinusoidal waveform (Fig. 7A). In the local coding framework, the degree of co-linearity of the threshold curve with the contours of local kinematics would determine where a given local kinematic variable is expected to contribute the best as a viable cue. At sites of co-linearity, a certain stimulus deviation away from the threshold would fall on a steep gradient and thus would maximally engage primary afferent spiking. Inversely, changes in stimuli along or parallel to the threshold line would have minimal perceptual effect. We confirmed this effect by testing change detection (also known as discrimination) by picking stimuli from isolines (Fig. 4), while it was missed by the classical studies using detection psychophysics. Optimal alignment can be seen for maximal acceleration in the low frequency range; for velocity in the center range, and for position at high frequencies. In Figure 7B, we explicitly plot the change of local kinematics along the PC threshold curve. To do this we first translated the frequency axis to pulse width (i.e., wavelength; essentially reinterpreting the sinusoids used in the classical study as series of local pulses). The ordinate scales the difference of slope between threshold curve (= iso-response-line) and kinematic contours (= iso-feature-lines). The line labeled “best fit” indicates where this difference is zero, i.e., pointing to perfect alignment of threshold curve and kinematic contour (marked by ovals). Seen from a local coding perspective, the contribution of PC afferents can be termed “acceleration coding” at pulses wider than >14 ms, “velocity coding” around a pulse width of 10 ms, and finally “position coding” with very narrow pulses of widths smaller than 5 ms. Our results in the two subranges (Fig. 4) are consistent with the first two coding principles (we did not test the suggested position coding using the narrow extremes of pulse widths).
The predominant engagement of model PCs and qualitative fit with psychometrics does not come as a surprise as it is a common finding that PCs are the most sensitive afferents to indenting vibrotactile stimuli (Johansson, 1978) as well as to touch of more natural surfaces (Weber et al., 2013). It is not clear why SA1 and RA1 afferents did not recreate the behavioral results. The SA1 and RA1 end organs are closely related to papillary ridges (Cauna, 1954), and may be much more sensitive to tangential shear (Johansson and Westling, 1987; Delhaye et al., 2021) than to indentation, a question that needs to be further clarified using spike recordings in the future. We did not model the fourth class of human tactile skin receptors, the slowly adapting afferents of type 2 (Ruffini). They are thought to be specialized more to shear, and were shown to be less responsive to indentation stimuli (Johansson and Vallbo, 1979).
What is the functional advantage of a local code?
The search for local coding is motivated by the assumed presence of frictional movements in objects that engage in moving contact (Schwarz, 2016). Moving contact is at the heart of tactile processing, as palpation movements are indispensable for the perception of fine textures (Hollins and Risner, 2000; Hollins et al., 2001; Skedung et al., 2013). Prototypical expressions of frictional contact are stick-slip events, short lived elastic deformations of the contacting materials coming about by sticking to surface elements, storing energy in elastic deformation, and releasing them quickly into sudden slips when frictional force is overcome by the driving movement of the contact (Schwarz, 2016). In the rodent vibrissa-based tactile system, frictional slips have been shown to exist (Arabzadeh et al., 2005; Ritt et al., 2008; Wolfe et al., 2008), and to encode texture information (Wolfe et al., 2008; Oladazimi et al., 2018). Primary sensory cortex in these animals has been shown to be selective for slip-based tactile inputs (Jadhav et al., 2009). Stick-slip events are short-lived, typically exceed noise in the signal, and therefore are accessible by local decoding schemes (Fig. 1A). In support of this notion, manipulations of pulsatile whisker deflections in a psychophysical study, have provided strong evidence in favor of local coding in rats’ tactile system (Waiblinger et al., 2015a). The present study extends this insight to the human fingertip system.
We wish to emphasize that the present study does not speak to the presence or absence of frictional movements or their use for perception in the human fingertip. However, it clearly provides supporting evidence for the perceptual hypothesis of temporally local stimulus read-out, a possible adaptation to make optimal use of information in frictional processes. Direct evidence about frictional movements of glabrous skin has so far been scant, but by no means negligible: papillary ridges undergo complex shearing deformations at the onset of a finger movement (Delhaye et al., 2016), and friction has been shown to be a determinant of roughness estimation, most of all for the discrimination of microscopic surface elements (Verrillo et al., 1999). Research on prehension has unearthed unequivocal evidence that sudden slip movement does occur in the skin, and is readily detected by humans to adjust grip forces (Johansson and Westling, 1987). Finally, so-called rate hardness (an instantaneous measure of change of force and speed when tapping surfaces) relates tightly to hardness perception suggesting that it may be based on local elements of the tactile signal as well (Lawrence et al., 2000; Han and Choi, 2010).
Our results are based on indentation stimuli, while frictional processes are often associated with tangential movement contact and ensuing skin shear (Johansson and Westling, 1987; Delhaye et al., 2014). In the whisker-related system latitudinal deflection, a sideways movement that can be compared with tangential skin movement, principally results also in longitudinal forces (along the whisker shaft), readily sensed by the neurites (Stüttgen et al., 2008; Quist et al., 2014; Oladazimi et al., 2021). We posit that papillary ridges activated by tangential skin movement would similarly generate force and moments that act in the direction normal to the surface of the skin. Moreover, real-world surfaces with asperities that stand out of the surface in 3D, are supposed to indent the skin locally, even if the main movement direction is tangential. In summary, the finding of local coding with indentation stimuli is expected as a partial requirement to assess frictional movement.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft Grants SCHW577/14-1 and SCHW577/14-3. Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. We acknowledge support by the Open Access Publishing Fund of the University Tübingen.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.