Abstract
Signal transmission in the brain propagates via distinct oscillatory frequency bands but the aperiodic component, 1/f activity, almost always co-exists which most of the previous studies have not sufficiently taken into consideration. We used a recently proposed parameterization model that delimits the oscillatory and aperiodic components of neural dynamics on lifespan aging data collected from human participants using magnetoencephalography (MEG). Since healthy aging underlines an enormous change in local tissue properties, any systematic relationship of 1/f activity would highlight their impact on the self-organized critical functional states. Furthermore, we have used patterns of correlation between aperiodic background and metrics of behavior to understand the domain general effects of 1/f activity. We suggest that age-associated global change in 1/f baseline alters the functional critical states of the brain affecting the global information processing impacting critically all aspects of cognition, e.g., metacognitive awareness, speed of retrieval of memory, cognitive load, and accuracy of recall through adult lifespan. This alteration in 1/f crucially impacts the oscillatory features peak frequency (PF) and band power ratio, which relates to more local processing and selective functional aspects of cognitive processing during the visual short-term memory (VSTM) task. In summary, this study leveraging on big lifespan data for the first time tracks the cross-sectional lifespan-associated periodic and aperiodic dynamical changes in the resting state to demonstrate how normative patterns of 1/f activity, PF, and band ratio (BR) measures provide distinct functional insights about the cognitive decline through adult lifespan.
- 1/f activity
- band ratio
- healthy ageing
- peak frequency
- self-organized critical state
- visual short-term memory
Significance Statement
Ageing is accompanied by the decline in cognitive functions and age itself is a major risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease and other neurologic conditions. Our study provides magnetoencephalogram (MEG) 1/f aperiodic and periodic markers across the healthy adult lifespan and shows that different frequency bands and their spectral features (aperiodic and periodic component) mediate age-related changes across different brain regions, in multiple cognitive and metacognitive domains, which not only provides us with a better understanding of the aging process but would also help in better prevention of cognitive impairments. A clear characterization of the association between baseline oscillatory component, 1/f activity, band ratio (BR), healthy aging and cognition, is established in this study.
Introduction
Spontaneous brain dynamics indexed by variation of neuroelectromagnetic potential may reflect the local as well global change associated with healthy aging processes that guide behavioral response through lifespan (Bishop et al., 2010; Foster et al., 2016; Sahoo et al., 2020). While the periodic component of spontaneous activity has been extensively studied as an objective measure for cognitive phenotyping (Klimesch, 1999; Knyazev, 2007; Schutter and Knyazev, 2012), the non-oscillatory/aperiodic background component also known as “1/f” activity received less attention compared with the former. This spontaneous 1/f aperiodic brain dynamics almost always pervasively co-exists in EEG/magnetoencephalography (MEG)/LFP signals (Lyne et al., 1992; Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 2001; Beggs and Plenz, 2003; Bédard et al., 2006; Voytek et al., 2015) and critically influence oscillatory signatures in the context of healthy aging and constrain task performance (Voytek et al., 2015; Donoghue et al., 2020). As we age, we are faced with the likelihood that our cognitive faculties will decline for example, ascertain memory (Craik, 1994; Ouyang et al., 2004; Nyberg et al., 2012), shift of sustained attention (Gazzaley et al., 2005), and processing speed (Gazzaley et al., 2007; Salthouse, 2010). There were recent attempts to relate 1/f activity, where it is being considered as a marker of “neural noise,” with N900 lexical prediction (Dave et al., 2017), working memory (Donoghue et al., 2020), and grammar learning (Cross et al., 2020).
The existence of ubiquitous 1/f activity in neuronal systems organized across various spatial scales is one of the key features of signal variability and collectively referred to as “noise.” However, a recent perspective suggests a serious reconceptualization is necessary to define what constitutes the “signal of interest” or noise in neuroscience (Uddin, 2020). The characteristics of 1/f component, slope and offset, of the ongoing oscillatory power has been found to be dynamic in nature. One possible mechanism for this dynamical change is an increased baseline activity (Voytek et al., 2015) or at a more fundamental level, a phenomenon called self-organized criticality (Bak et al., 1987). In neuroscience, the presence of critical brain states can shape an organism’s ability to optimally switch between task states (Buzsáki, 2006). However, some studies have postulated that the origin of 1/f may lie in the tissue properties (Bédard et al., 2006). Therefore, we hypothesized that aging is consonant with alteration of physiological properties in brain tissue, so any changes along aging dimension in 1/f owes its origin to the tissue properties. Subsequently, we propose that prospective age-corrected correlations between 1/f and behavioral performance in tasks will reveal whether the tissue properties can alter the self-organized criticality of functional brain states and subserve as a non-specific mediator of behavior and cognitive functions.
Other than the characteristics of 1/f component, slope and offset, physiological aging has been further characterized by progressive change in oscillatory power, central frequency (CF), and functional connectivity (Voytek et al., 2015; Murty et al., 2020; Sahoo et al., 2020; Tran and Reddy, 2020). For example, a consistent line of research associated higher individual peak α frequency (PAF) across adulthood with better working memory and better reading comprehension (Klimesch, 1999; Clark, 2004). There are also noticeable discrepancies among numerous existing aging studies based on the estimation of difference of power between younger and elderly individuals in the frequency band of interest (Klass and Brenner, 1995; Aurlien et al., 2004; Cummins and Finnigan, 2007; Stomrud et al., 2010; Ishii et al., 2017; Scally et al., 2018; Sahoo et al., 2020). Hence, it is often notoriously difficult to reconcile those age-associated oscillatory findings during spontaneous activity and trusting power changes in the relevant frequency band were estimated accurately. One possible reason for this inconsistency might be the mixing of oscillatory power with the aperiodic background 1/f activity, which was not taken into sufficient consideration by most of the studies.
On the other hand, frequency band ratio (BR) is a common measure in investigating attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Lubar, 1991; Snyder and Hall, 2006; Arns et al., 2014), executive functioning (Lubar, 1991; Angelidis et al., 2016; Gordon et al., 2018; van Son et al., 2019) and working memory capacity (Moran et al., 2010). We propose here that both shifts in individual peak frequency (PF) and BR index clearly different aspects of functional changes associated with aging impacting short-term working memory speed and accuracy of retrieval on one hand and cognitive capacity on the other.
Together, we suggest the three different measures, 1/f slope-offset reflecting aperiodic activity, PF and BR reflecting periodic activity provide distinct functional insights about the neural underpinning of the healthy aging process.
Materials and Methods
Participants
The Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) is a large-scale, multimodal, cross-sectional adult lifespan (18–88) population-based study. The Cam-CAN consists of two stages. In stage 1, 2681 participants had gone through general cognitive assessments at their home. Tests for hearing, vision, balance, and speeded response were also assessed. Additionally, measures taken in stage 1 served to screen participants for stage 2. Those with poor hearing, poor vision, with neurologic diseases such as stroke, epilepsy, or a score <25 in MMSE (cognitive assessment examination) were excluded from the further participation. From stage 1 to stage 2, 700 participants were screened (50 men and 50 women from each age band). All screened participants were recruited for testing at the Medical Research Council (United Kingdom) Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit (MRC-CBSU) in Cambridge. In this stage, MRI scans, MEG recordings, and cognitive task data were collected, all the participants performed a range of psychological tests and neuroimaging assessments, but only the MEG RS data and visual short-term memory (VSTM) task data are included in this study. Out of 700 participants, MEG data from 650 subjects were available. Age values of participants were divided into four age groups for categorical analysis (see statistical analysis): young adults (YA), middle elderly (ME), middle-late (ML), older adults (OA). Earlier studies have done similar grouping (Chan et al., 2014; Sahoo et al., 2020). A total of 70 participants were randomly chosen from each age group resulting in a total of N = 280 subjects comprising all four important stages of adult lifespan (Table 1).
Data acquisition
MEG resting-state data
MEG Data used for this study were obtained from the Cam-CAN repository (available at http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/datasets/camcan/; Taylor et al., 2017; Shafto et al., 2014). For all the 700 participants, MEG data were collected by Elekta Neuromag, Helsinki at MRC-CBSU using 306 channels, consisting of 102 magnetometers and 204 orthogonal planar gradiometers. MEG data collection was done in a light magnetically shielded room (MSR). A high pass filter of 0.03-Hz cut-off was used to sample the data at 1000 Hz. Head-position indicator (HPI) coils were used to continuously assess the head position within the MEG helmet. To monitor blinks and eye movements, two pairs of bipolar electrodes were used to record horizontal and vertical electrooculogram signals. To monitor pulse-related artefacts, one pair of electrodes was used to record electrocardiogram signals. MEG data collected for resting state required the participants to sit still for a minimum of 8 min and 40 s with their eyes closed. From this subset, 280 participants were included in the present study (70 in each group).
VSTM stimuli and task
In Cam-CAN, the design was adapted from Zhang and Luck (2008; Fig. 1). On each trial, participants were presented with one, two, three, or four colored discs (mimicking different memory load conditions) for 250 ms. Following that, a blank screen was presented for 900 ms to hold those colors in memory. One of the original locations was highlighted by a thick black border (acting as a probe for participants to remember the color at that location), and at the same time, a response color wheel was presented. Participants had as much time as required to report by touching or clicking, as accurately as possible the remembered hue of the highlighted disk. No feedback was given. After every trial, there was a 830-ms fixation period. Participants complete two blocks of 112 trials, with memory load (1, 2, 3, or 4) counterbalanced and randomly intermixed. For each set size (memory load), the following measures (Table 2) were estimated by fitting the error distribution with a mixture model of von Mises and uniform distributions, proposed by Zhang and Luck (2008) and modified by Bays and Husain (2008; for detailed analysis, see Zhang and Luck, 2008; Mitchell et al., 2018). In brief, as a model-free index of performance, the response error, the angular difference between the target color presented and the color reported, was calculated. This model-free index cannot be used to distinguish errors because of imprecise memory of an item, from errors because of reporting the wrong item, or guessing when an item is not kept in memory at all. To estimate these, Maximum likelihood estimation was used to decompose the data from each subject into three parameters that represent a mixture of a uniform distribution of errors and a von Mises distribution of errors. The von Mises distribution is the circular analog of the Gaussian distribution and was used because the tested color space was circular. The uniform distribution was represented by a single parameter, Pm, which is the probability that the probed item was present in memory at the time of the probe. K is calculated by multiplying the memory load by Pm. The von Mises distribution was represented by two parameters, its mean (μ) and its Standard Deviation (SD); μ reflects any systematic shift of the distribution away from the original color value. The “precision” of each item held in memory is reported as the reciprocal of the SD of the fitted von Mises distribution. Subjects indicated their uncertainty in their choice of color by the length of time they touched the wheel: as they held their finger down, white confidence intervals spread out around the selected point indicating greater uncertainty about their selection. To assess metacognitive awareness, the angular width of the reported confidence intervals provided a trial-by-trial measure of subjective uncertainty. To summarize overall uncertainty for each individual and condition, the mean was taken across trials. Participants with smaller values thus reported more confidence in their responses.
MEG data preprocessing
MEG processed data were provided by Cam-CAN. Preprocessing pipeline included temporal signal space separation, applied on continuous MEG data to remove noise from the HPI coils, environmental sources and continuous head motion correction. For removing the main frequency noise (50-Hz notch filter) and to reconstruct any noisy channel, max filter was used. More details about data acquisition and preprocessing have been presented elsewhere (Shafto et al., 2014; Taylor et al., 2017). Additionally, we performed independent component analysis (ICA) to get rid of the artifacts and removal of higher order harmonics present in different frequency bands in the signal following previous studies (Taylor et al., 2017; Sahoo et al., 2020).
Data analysis
We analyzed the MEG and behavioral data in MATLAB and python using custom made scripts. In this study, we used Python MNE for preprocessing; standard python libraries including Scipy, Pandas, and NumPy for data management and processing; and python-matplotlib and seaborn for data visualization. The analysis pipeline is presented in Figure 2.
Power spectral density (PSD) using Welch’s periodogram method
The Power spectrum Sxx(f) of a signal x(t) capture how the strength of the signal is distributed in the frequency domain. Using fast Fourier transform (FFT; a variant of Fourier analysis), the representation of raw signal (time or space) is transformed into a frequency representation of the signal. Processed MEG data provided in .fif format was analyzed using Fieldtrip toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011). Data for each N = 280 subjects were first downsampled from 1000 to 250 Hz. The frequency resolution was held at 0.05 Hz. PSD was estimated using Welch’s periodogram method (pwelch function) implemented in MATLAB 2019b. For each participant, 102 magnetometer sensor’s time series data resulted in a matrix of size
For each participant, resulted power spectrum matrix was
Extracting periodic and aperiodic features using a parameterization model
To separate the periodic (oscillatory) component from the aperiodic component of the signal power spectra, we used a recently proposed parameterization model, fitting-oscillations-and-one-over-f (FOOOF toolbox; for full description, see Donoghue et al., 2020). In brief, the PSDs calculated using pwelch were given as an input to the FOOOF model, which considers PSDs as a linear sum of aperiodic 1/f like characteristics of neural power, and it is entirely described by the aperiodic “exponent” and “offset.” Periodic components describe putative oscillations that describe power above aperiodic component [so-called “peaks,” simulated as Gaussian function; are described by PF in Hertz, peak power over and above the 1/f signal in arbitrary units (au) and bandwidth (BW; Hz)]. Larger BW in given frequency band indicates the deviation of power from the baseline and the spread across a wider frequency range. This global spread in power across frequency band further quantifies the strength of a signal at a specific frequency and allows for information transfer across wider frequency range. The simulation, for a power spectrum P is described as follows:
Where P is the linear sum of the aperiodic signal L and N Gaussian peaks Gn. For each peak, Gaussian function
Where a denotes the amplitude, c denotes the CF, w denotes the BW of the Gaussian. F is the frequency vector. Subsequently, all fitted Gaussians were subtracted from the original power spectrum to get a peak-removed power spectrum (PRPS). Finally, a 1/f signal is estimated from this PRPS using Equation 4, representing the actual cortical noise. Exponential function in semilog-power space (logged power values and linear frequencies) is used to model the aperiodic signal (initial and final fit both), L, as:
Where b denotes the broadband offset, χ is the slope, and k is the knee parameter, which depends on the bend in the aperiodic signal. F is the frequency vector. The FOOOF model described in Equation 2 fits the power of a given sensor signal by estimating a linear function L for the aperiodic component of the signal and each oscillatory contribution Gn is modelled as Gaussian peaks. Moreover, estimated power was fitted across the entire frequency range of 1–45 Hz by as no knee was expected in the MEG recordings across the 1- to 50-Hz frequency range (Miller et al., 2009). The number of oscillatory components is determined from the data, with the option to set a maximum number of components as a parameter. The general model assumption here is that oscillatory and aperiodic processes are distinct and separable. Algorithm was implemented using custom python scripts on the python3 version.
The model was fitted for individual subject and output parameters were averaged across subjects for each group (Fig. 3A). The settings for the algorithm were set as: (1) peak_width_limits = [0.5, 12]; (2) min_peak_height = 0; (3) max_n_peaks = 12; (4) peak_threshold = 2; (5) aperiodic_mode = “fixed”; and (6) verbos = “true.” Oscillations were post hoc grouped into θ (4–8 Hz), α (8–12 Hz), and β (13–30 Hz). For estimating the topographical dynamical changes, the brain was segmented into five non-overlapping regions: frontal (number of sensors = 26), parietal (number of sensors = 26), occipital (number of sensors = 24), and right and left temporal (number of sensors = 26).
Extended Data Figure 3-1
A, Power spectrum without model fitting. B, C, FOOOF-power spectrum model for ME and ML age groups, along with its statistics. Download Figure 3-1, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 3-2
Power spectrum in log-log space. Download Figure 3-2, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 3-3
A, Aperiodic component of different age groups. B, Boxplot of aperiodic features across different age groups derived from the component. Download Figure 3-3, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 3-4
Variability of aperiodic component across individual subjects in each age group. Download Figure 3-4, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 3-5
Association between 1/f aperiodic slope with periodic components of the dominant oscillations. Download Figure 3-5, EPS file.
BR measures
Additionally, we estimated the BRs which reflect the quantitative measure of oscillatory activity and are investigated in different cognitive processes; however, they also get impacted by the 1/f background noise (Donoghue et al., 2020). After removing the aperiodic signal using a parametrization method proposed by the FOOOF toolbox, periodic values were estimated. Thus, after implementing appropriate parametrization of the aperiodic component of the signal power spectra BR values were re-estimated to indicate the true power changes and finally, were grouped into different frequency bands of interest. For each participant, we calculated the ratio of periodic components of different frequencies and averaged across participants for age bin-wise distribution. BR of all the periodic components for each frequency band was then calculated by dividing the average of low band periodic features by the average of high band periodic features. We calculated frequency-specific BRs of all periodic features.
where X = PW, CF, BW.
Statistical analysis
We performed both categorical as well as continuous analyses to capture different aspects of age-associated functional differences. For the continuous analysis, we divided the total number of participants into bins of five years starting from 18 years, a total of 14 bins, and the center value was taken to be the representative age for each bin. For the categorical analysis, we divided data into the following age stratifications (18–35, 36–50, 51–64, and 66–88 years) to get insights about different important stages of adult lifespan and comparison with previous works (Chan et al., 2014; Sahoo et al., 2020). To clearly delineate the effects of periodic and aperiodic features on VSTM features, it was necessary to illustrate the effect in different load conditions. Only set size 2 and set size 4 are reported in the main manuscript, where we have categorized the set sizes in two load conditions: set size 2 as a low load condition and set size 4 as a high load condition. We have provided the findings for behavioral features of other set sizes as Extended Data Figs 9-2, 9-3, 9-4.
Correlation analysis
Depending on the data distribution, Pearson or Spearman’s correlation was used to estimate the strength between two variables. Estimated functional changes (PF, aperiodic, and BR measures) and VSTM task measures were correlated with age. Finally, VSTM task measures were correlated with the functional changes that occurs with aging to elucidate the role of these functional measures in determining behavioral responses.
Regression analysis
Linear and nonlinear regression were performed separately considering each power, CF, BW, slope, offset, and BRs of periodic features, as the estimated measures (R) of functional changes and precision, reaction time (RT), metacognitive awareness (d), and memory capacity (k), as the estimated measures (R) of the VSTM behavioral task, while keeping age as an explanatory variable:
Linear regression was performed using fitlm MATLAB function. To capture the potential nonlinear effects of age, we also added second order polynomial terms to the model, such as:
Linear regression was also performed considering each VSTM task measure as response variable (R) and the functional measure as the explanatory variable (E).
All regression tables are provided in the supplementary document (Extended Data Tables 8-1, 9-1, 10-1, 10-2, 11-1, 11-2, 11-3, 11-4). For estimating the significance, first normality of the data distribution was assessed using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Based on the data distribution, parametric (t test) or nonparametric (Wilcoxon rank-sum test) was performed.
Code accessibility
The codes for all the analysis conducted in this paper is freely available to download from https://drive.google.com/drive/folders/1__74tFI1_VnHaV_-kJ46VAGMGiI04T2i?usp=sharing.
Results
Using the FOOOF model, we fitted the PSD, and from the parametrization model fit, all the simulated Gaussian peaks were removed to analyze the background signal. Thereafter, the aperiodic component of the signal was fitted in the log-log space line (Extended Data Fig. 3-2), from which 1/f Slope and offset were extracted for each participant. Periodic features, CF, power (PW), BW, were estimated using peak parameters from the fitted model (see Materials and Methods). To check whether the parameterization using the simulated FOOOF model is able to capture lifespan associated changes, we first simulated the model for young and old adults. The model well captured the well-established lifespan associated with slowing down of PAF (Fig. 3). Original spectrum, aperiodic fit and full model are being depicted in Figure 3B,C for YA and OA group, respectively (extended analyses are shown in Extended Data Figs. 3-1, 3-2, 3-3, 3-4, 3-5 for model’s output parameters of ME and ML groups). To capture the dynamical changes in the dominant oscillations (highest power peak across all frequencies) across the adult lifespan, the CF, PW, and BW of the dominant oscillations were also extracted for young and old adults.
Topographical distribution of aperiodic 1/f component of the signal with age
Increase in aperiodic 1/f slope and decrease in 1/f intercept
Aperiodic 1/f slope increases significantly when age was treated as a pseudo-continuous variable (
Topographical distribution of PF with age
Age-associated slowing of central α frequency (CAF) and β frequency
For each participant, PF was quantified by estimating the peak power value within the 8–12 and 13–30 Hz for α and β range, respectively. Each participant’s PF was then averaged to get the group-wise estimation of CAF. Visual inspection revealed bin 65 to be the outlier (for CAF). After removing the outlier, significant linear age-related decline was found (
Extended Data Figure 5-1
Dominant frequency (A) and respective power (B) for YA and OA. Download Figure 5-1, EPS file.
Age-associated functional power change in α, θ, and β frequency
We found a robust decline of α power with age (
Extended Data Figure 6-1
Frequency specific power as a function of age across different sensors. For Frontal sensors, θ power is not shown as only few age groups showed peaks in θ range. Error bar represents SEM. Download Figure 6-1, EPS file.
Increase in β BW with age
BW reflects the spread of power in the respective frequency range, which for the β band was found to be increased across the adult lifespan (
Extended Data Figure 7-1
Frequency specific BW for different age groups. Download Figure 7-1, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 7-2
Spatial topography of θ BW (A) and α BW (B) for YA and OA. Download Figure 7-2, EPS file.
Topographical Distribution of BRs with age
BR measures have been argued to be a marker of various cognitive measures in healthy adults as well as in pathologic conditions (Schutter and Van Honk 2005; Kamiński et al., 2011; Trammell et al., 2017) which also gets affected by 1/f activity. We investigated how these global BRs change with age after effectively removing the background 1/f activity. We looked at θ/α, θ/β, and α/β BRs, where the ratio of all periodic features (PW, CF, BW) was analyzed for each frequency band. For all BR measures, we calculated correlations between the spectral features of each oscillation-band and age. Here, we showed the global change (averaged across all sensors) in the BR measures across the lifespan.
The age-associated nonlinear change was mostly observed in frontal and parietal sensors (Fig. 8A). For age categories, we found a significant difference between OA versus ME (t(134) = 2.38, p = 0.018), OA versus ML (t(134) = 3.19, p = 0.0018), YA versus ME (t(138) = 3.30, p = 0.0012), and YA versus ML (t(138) = 4.09, p = 0.00007). For the CF ratio, we found α/β ratio to vary nonlinearly (quadratic) with age (β1 = −0.0059138, R2 = 0.61 p = 0.005), whereby first decreases for middle age and subsequently an increase for older age participants suggesting an overall U-shaped response of α/β ratio through lifespan (Fig. 8B). No significant difference was found between the categorical age groups for θ/α and θ/β peak ratios (Extended Data Figs. 8-1, 8-2; Extended Data Table 8-1).
Extended Data Figure 8-1
θ/α PF ratio (A) and θ/β PF ratio (B) for YA and OA. Download Figure 8-1, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 8-2
θ/β power ratio for YA and OA. Download Figure 8-2, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 8-3
A, θ/β BW. B, θ/α BW as a function of age. Download Figure 8-3, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 8-4
A, Topoplots of θ/β BW. B, θ/α BW for YA and OA. Download Figure 8-4, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 8-5
Association between 1/f slope with α/β power ratio. Download Figure 8-5, EPS file.
Extended Data Table 8-1
Regression table for global frequency BRs with age. F value, β coefficient, goodness of fit, and significance of the model are reported. Download Table 8-1, DOC file.
Power Ratio of θ/α was found to be positively correlated with age (β1 = 0.0057613, R2 = 0.40, p = 0.02) whereas α/β power ratio was negatively correlated with age (β1 = −0.018116, R2 = 0.85, p = 0.000001; Fig. 8B). Significant categorical difference was found for θ/α power ratio between YA versus OA (t(136) = 4.9615, p = 0.000002), YA versus ME (t(138) = 2.75, p = 0.0067), YA versus ML (t(138) = 4.92, p = 0.000002), ME versus OA (t(134) = 2.24, p = 0.02). No significant correlation was found for θ/β power ratios with age (R2 = 0.2, p = 0.1). For α/β power ratio, significant categorical difference was found between YA versus OA (t(76) = −4.6, p = 0.00001), ME versus OA (t(59) = −3.33, p = 0.0015), and ML versus OA (t(62) = −2.46, p = 0.01). No significant difference was found between the categorical age groups for θ/β power ratio.
BW ratio of θ/β and θ/α was found to be negatively correlated with age (extended analysis is shown in Extended Data Figs. 8-3, 8-4). Categorical analysis revealed differences between the YA versus OA (t(80) = 2.21, p = 0.029) for θ/β BW ratio. No significant difference was found between the categorical age group for α/β BW ratio.
After characterizing the normative pattern of true oscillatory changes across age, we tested our hypothesis by carrying out regression analysis whereby keeping 1/f noise, periodic features as an explanatory variable and behavioral measures as response variable (see Materials and Methods). All correlations were performed after regressing out the age.
We first analyzed the behavioral responses of the same participants in the VSTM task to replicate the well-established cognitive decline with age. Grouping of participants in the age groups and bins were done similarly.
Behavioral results: age-related cognitive decline reflected in performance
Precision
As expected, precision becomes worse with memory load and age. Overall precision was high for the set size 1 (61.1% SEM 2%) as compared with set sizes 2 (48.7% SEM 1.9%), 3 (39.6% SEM 1%), and 4 (39% SEM 0.7%). Continuous analysis revealed significant decrease in precision with age in both low and high load conditions (low load, r = −0.85, p < 0.01, high load, r = −0.61, p < 0.05; Fig. 9A; extended analysis is shown in Extended Data Fig. 9-1). Categorical analysis between the groups revealed significant differences in the mean of YA versus OA (YA = 0.48 ± 0.008, OA = 0.30 ± 0.005, p < 0.0001), YA versus ME (YA = 0.48 ± 0.008, ME = 0.45 ± 0.007, p < 0.001), YA versus ML (YA = 0.48 ± 0.008, ML = 0.43 ± 0.007, p < 0.0001), and ME versus OA (ME = 0.45 ± 0.007, OA = 0.30 ± 0.005, p < 0.0001) groups. Within-group analysis also revealed significant increase in precision with increase in memory load (Extended Data Fig. 9-2; Extended Data Table 9-1).
Extended Data Figure 9-1
VSTM measures as a function of age. Participants with same age were grouped together (dots), size of the dot represents the SEM of the group. Shaded area is the 95% confidence interval. Download Figure 9-1, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 9-2
Precision across age groups and set size. Download Figure 9-2, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 9-3
Uncertainty across age groups and set size. Download Figure 9-3, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 9-4
VSTM capacity across age groups for different set sizes. Download Figure 9-4, EPS file.
Extended Data Table 9-1
Regression table for VSTM measures with age. F value, β coefficient, goodness of fit, and significance of the model are reported. Download Table 9-1, DOC file.
Reaction Time
Overall RT was higher for the set size 4 (910.2 ± 21.6 ms) as compared with set size 1 (878.7 ± 19.9 ms), 2 (870.4 ± 19.9 ms), and 3 (882.6 ± 21.4 ms) but increases significantly with age [low load (r) = +0.57, p < 0.05, high load (r) = +0.56, p < 0.05; Fig. 9B]. Group analysis also revealed significant difference between YA versus OA (YA = 668 ± 35.4, OA = 1009 ± 38.9, p < 0.00001), YA versus ME (YA = 668 ± 35.4, ME = 828.8 ± 41.5, p = 0.002), YA versus ML (YA = 668 ± 35.4, ML = 886 ± 33.4, p < 0.0001), ME versus ML (ME = 828.8 ± 41.5, ML = 886 ± 33.4, p = 0.03), ME versus OA (ME = 828.8 ± 41.5, OA = 1009 ± 38.9, p < 0.001), ML versus OA (ML = 886 ± 33.4, OA = 1009 ± 38.9, p = 0.05).
VSTM capacity (k)
VSTM capacity was found to decrease with age [low load (r) = −0.81 p < 0.001, high load (r) = −0.87, p < 0.001; Fig. 9C]. Categorical analysis between the group revealed significant difference between YA versus OA (YA = 1.84 ± 0.01, OA = 1.66 ± 0.02, p < 0.0001), YA versus ML (YA = 1.84 ± 0.01, ML = 1.79 ± 0.01, p = 0.004), ME versus OA (ME = 1.83 ± 0.01, OA = 1.66 ± 0.02, p < 0.0001), and ML versus OA (ML = 1.79 ± 0.01, OA = 1.66 ± 0.02, p < 0.001; results for all the set sizes are reported in Extended Data Fig. 9-4).
Mean uncertainty (metacognitive measure)
Subjective uncertainty was higher in set 4 (29.7 ± 1) as compared with set size 1 (11.8 ± 0.39), 2 (15.6 ± 0.5), and 3 (20.7 ± 0.67). After performing regression and correlation analysis, we found that subjective uncertainty significantly decreases with age in low load condition [low load (r) = −0.56, p < 0.05; Fig. 9D]. Suggesting that OAs tend to be more confident about their erroneous answers when the load is less. Categorical analysis revealed significant differences in the mean of YA versus OA (YA = 18.5 ± 0.97, OA = 14 ± 1, p < 0.001), YA versus ME (YA = 18.5 ± 0.97, ME = 16.07 ± 0.95, p = 0.02), YA versus ML (YA = 18.5 ± 0.97, ML = 14.85 ± 1.05, p < 0.001), and ME versus OA (ME = 16.07 ± 0.95, OA = 14 ± 1, p = 0.003). Within-group analysis also revealed significant increase in subjective uncertainty with increase in memory load (Extended Data Fig. 9-3)
Aperiodic 1/f slope: predictive of all measures of VSTM
We then assessed whether the VSTM performance was impacted by 1/f slope. As hypothesized, RS aperiodic 1/f noise was found to be predictive of decreased precision (low load: r = −0.74, p = 0.002, high load: r = −0.48, p = 0.08), memory capacity (low load: r = −0.68, p = 0.0007, high load: r =+0.82, p = 0.0003), mean uncertainty (low load: r = −0.58, p = 0.03, high load: r = −0.6, p = 0.02) and increased RT (low load: r = +0.56, p = 0.00005, high load: r = +0.57, p = 0.00005) in VSTM task (Fig. 10). However, we did not find any correlation between 1/f offset and behavioral measures (Extended Data Tables 10-1, 10-2). As aperiodic 1/f noise mediated a global effect on the VSTM performance, we further wanted to investigate how different oscillatory components mediate changes in the specific behavior measures in VSTM performance through lifespan.
Extended Data Table 10-1
Regression table for VSTM measures with aperiodic slope. F value, β coefficient, goodness of fit, and significance of the model are reported. Download Table 10-1, DOC file.
Extended Data Table 10-2
Regression table for VSTM measures with aperiodic 1/f offset. F value, β coefficient, goodness of fit, and significance of the model are reported. Download Table 10-2, DOC file.
Precision increases with increase in α power and α/β power ratio
Precision was found to be positively correlated with the α power for both low (
Extended Data Table 11-1
Regression table for specific oscillatory features with VSTM measures. F value, β coefficient, goodness of fit, and significance of the model are reported. Download Table 11-1, DOC file.
Extended Data Table 11-2
Regression table for α/β CF with VSTM measures. F value, β coefficient, goodness of fit, and significance of the model are reported. Download Table 11-2, DOC file.
Extended Data Table 11-3
Regression table for α/β Power ratio with VSTM measures. F value, β coefficient, goodness of fit, and significance of the model are reported. Download Table 11-3, DOC file.
Extended Data Table 11-4
Regression table for θ/α power ratio with VSTM measures. F value, β coefficient, goodness of fit, and significance of the model are reported. Download Table 11-4, DOC file.
Power ratio was also found to be a significant predictor of precision in low (
Speed of processing predicted by α speed
Speed of α is often related to the speed of processing which is generally measured as RT. As we observed that speed of α decreases and RT increases with age, we wanted to investigate whether this decrease in α speed affected the speed of processing in OAs. α Speed significantly predicted the speed of processing for both low (
VSTM capacity predicted by θ power and θ/α band power ratio
We found a significant negative correlation of VSTM capacity with θ power (low load: r = −0.729, p = 0.004, high load: r = −0.679, p = 0.01) and θ/α power ratio (low load: r = −0.64, p = 0.01, high load: r = −0.75, p = 0.001), suggesting that these two play an important role in storing items in working memory. Regression analysis also revealed a significant role of θ power and θ/α power ratio in predicting VSTM capacity in low (θ power:
Discussion
Using three different measures (aperiodic 1/f slope and offset, PF, and BR of power in various frequencies), we have systematically investigated the spontaneous temporal dynamics and dynamical changes during resting state associated with healthy adult lifespan. Subsequently, we have demonstrated how these measures potentially link distinct behavioral responses during short-term working memory processing. Many previous studies in aging literature have demonstrated that task-relevant oscillatory changes accurately demarcate task performance in various cognitive domains (Clark, 2004; Cummins and Finnigan, 2007; Tóth et al., 2014; Proskovec et al., 2016; Rondina et al., 2019). As the resting-state serves as a baseline/control for the diverse task-related changes, it is crucial to characterize how specifically aging alters the normative brain network dynamics to impact cognition.
The domain general effect of age-associated aperiodic 1/f activity
To track systematically healthy aging associated changes in neuronal oscillations through lifespan, a substantial number of previous studies have used narrowband power analysis that presumes that spectral power implies oscillatory power, without precisely separating the 1/f activity which in itself is dynamic and it impacts the oscillatory power which can lead to misinterpretation of the results. Extended analysis in Extended Data Figure 3-5 shows the relation between 1/f slope and dominant periodic features, indicating the interdependence of these two components and the necessity to detangle these. We approached this problem by applying a parameterization model (Donoghue et al., 2020), which detangles the periodic and aperiodic 1/f component.
Recent studies have considered 1/f slope as an index of noise in the brain (Ouyang et al., 2020; Donoghue et al., 2020). Ageing is associated with an increase in cortical neural noise, where studies have previously used RT as a proxy for the neural noise (Welford, 1981; Salthouse and Lichty, 1985; Cremer and Zeef, 1987). We observed 1/f slope of the MEG spectral power increases with age (flattening of PSD), which is suggestive of increased desynchronized neuronal activity (Hong and Rebec, 2012; Podvalny et al., 2015). Voytek and colleagues had suggested that this flattening of PSD slopes which they indexed for noise might be a hallmark of age-related cognitive decline (Voytek et al., 2015). Also, aperiodic 1/f noise is found to be very dynamic in nature and it has been shown to be predictive of performance in working memory tasks (Voytek et al., 2015), N400 lexical prediction (Dave et al., 2017), and in grammar learning (Cross et al., 2020). Our results depict the association between global change in the 1/f slope with capacity, speed, precision, and metacognition in short-term working memory processing. As 1/f activity is associated with several distinct domains of cognition, we further suggest that age-related increase in aperiodic 1/f slope does not necessarily mediate any domain-specific processing rather it affects domain general processing (Fig. 12).
Additionally, the increase in 1/f slope follows a monotonic nonlinear relationship with age suggesting that the rate of change in desynchronized neural activity is not necessarily constant across adult lifespan. We observe some deviation from the normal trend for both 1/f slope and offset in age-group 60–80, which might be because of the observed increased variance in the older group. Besides, aperiodic 1/f features were found to not only vary across subjects (more for elderly) but also across different sensors indicating substantial variability and idiosyncrasy. Although 1/f slope shows spatial heterogeneity in the young group, such as being less negative in the anterior sensors compared with the posterior sensors, older participants display a more homogenous distribution of less negative 1/f slope values. The broadband offset shows no significant deviation with age, but significant between-group differences were observed.
Aperiodic 1/f activity, self-organized criticality, and nonlinear relationship with adult lifespan
At a more fundamental level, 1/f scale reflects the self-similar temporal properties of the self-organized critical states. Although the aim of this study is not to resolve this debate, however, we argue that 1/f activity could arise from potentially number of factors, e.g., altered tissue properties or self-organized criticality and transient stability with aging and change in underlying excitation-inhibition (E/I) balance (Bédard et al., 2006; Voytek et al., 2015; Gao et al., 2017; Naik et al., 2017; Naskar et al., 2021).
Criticality hypothesis, which proposes that the brain operates in a critical state, and alteration in criticality could be symptomatic or causative for certain pathologies (Hesse and Gross, 2014) seems to be operative here in terms reorganization of brain dynamics based on 1/f slope and offset and their relationship with cognitive performance. Literature suggests that neural networks at criticality exhibit properties for optimal performance such as information transmission and storage, metastable state, dynamic range and computational power (Beggs, 2008; Shew and Plenz, 2013). However, 1/f scale cannot alone be explained by criticality; rather emergence may lie in the local tissue properties (Bédard et al., 2006). Healthy aging is consonant with alteration of physiological properties in brain tissue (Aalami et al., 2003; Peters, 2006), therefore, it seems plausible to say that change in 1/f scale and increase in 1/f slope exhibiting nonlinear relationship with aging has its origin in the local tissue properties.
Subsequently, the association of 1/f slope with cognitive and metacognitive aspects of VSTM demonstrate the domain general effects of local tissue properties affecting self-organized criticality of functional brain states which in turn affects behavior.
PF, BR relates to distinct aspects of memory processing
In oscillatory dynamics, we observe a significant decline in PAF with age as shown by previous studies (Voytek et al., 2015; Sahoo et al., 2020). This decrease in PAF was not found to be localized to specific sensors, rather a global significant decrease was observed (Fig. 5B). The speed of α is often associated with the speed of information processing therefore, higher α speed is needed for optimal performance in cognitive tasks (Surwillo et al., 1961) and determine the temporal resolution of visual perceptual integration (Samaha and Postle, 2015). Figure 11C shows that the RT of the participants or speed of memory retrieval is well predicted by global α speed. Hence, higher the speed of α, fast is the retrieval, and consequently lesser RT for younger adults. As aging is characterized by attentional difficulties in particular, a reduced capability to inhibit irrelevant information (McNab et al., 2015), therefore, α band power may have an important role in determining how accurately older individual’s recall the memorized items. However, a study by Vaden et al. (2012) demonstrated that older people do not use α power suppression to inhibit distractor’s information. In this work, we found that α power decreases with age, particularly over occipito-parietal sensors (Fig. 6), which significantly predicts the precision (Fig. 11A), along with α/β power which decreases with age (Griffiths et al., 2019; Fig. 8B). It plays a crucial role in suppressing irrelevant information, therefore, not being able to ignore distractions might be one of the reasons for low VSTM capacity found in OAs.
The relevance of θ CF in determining VSTM capacity in a task is well known in the literature. A study by Moran et al. (2010) shows that both slow and fast θ frequencies correlated to the high memory capacity, distributed across different networks. In the context of aging, we observe that θ CF slightly increases for older subjects as compared with younger adults which may itself affect the storing capacity. There are studies which have observed an increase in RS θ power in OAs (for review, see Klimesch, 1999; Klass and Brenner, 1995) others have reported θ power decrease in resting as well as in the task with age (Babiloni et al., 2006; Cummins and Finnigan, 2007; Leirer et al., 2011; Vlahou et al., 2014). However, we found an increase in θ power with age, which significantly predicted the VSTM capacity (Figs. 6A, 11E) along with θ/α power ratio (TAR), which also significantly increases with age (Fig. 8). Few studies including the study by Trammell et al. (2017) found decreased performance in RM correlated with increased TAR in old adults. We found substantial variability in the presence of θ power in participants. For instance, in young groups, the θ was not observed over frontal and left temporal sensors, whereas in older participants the θ power was observed only over temporal sensors. Additionally, we also observed an increase in β power with age which is well reported in the literature, generally associated with the movement-related activity (Ishii et al., 2017; Sahoo et al., 2020) but we also observed a significant decrease in β PF with age which was more localized to the central-parietal sensors (generally found in depression and other psychological disorders’ patients in open-eye condition; Roohi-Azizi et al., 2017). In the BW measure, only β BW was found to increase significantly with age which indicates higher variability in β frequency (Fig. 7). This increase was mostly observed in the central and temporal sensors. Differences in the regional attenuation of absolute and relative β power within specific high frequency bands may reflect the disparate neuropathologic processes of mild cognitive impairment associated with age, as well as the extent of brain dysfunction. We can further speculate the amount of spread of power in a particular frequency range that increases with age may suggest a state of fractured synchronization in elderly compared with younger adults. Mechanistically, this can be a result of decreased myelination in long-range fibers across aging, which introduces time delays in the resultant dynamical system governing such processes. Time delays are known to introduce phase lags in a group of coupled (and synchronized) oscillators resulting in lowering of synchronization indices such as increased bandwidth.
In conclusion, our results suggest that the age-associated change in aperiodic 1/f activity affects the global information processing and links with speed of information processing, cognitive capacity, precision, and metacognitive awareness (all behavioral measures). In contrast, periodic features; PF and BR of different frequency bands relate to more local processing and selective behavioral measures in VSTM task crucially impacting distinct aspects of memory processing with age. On that account, we suggest that the change observed in local tissue properties with aging is reflected as the global increase in aperiodic 1/f slope. This increase in 1/f slope seems to impact distributed processes of cognition as it alters the self-organized critical functional brain states, whereas oscillatory features mediate localized processing, that is relevant for the specific task (Fig. 12).
An important limitation of our study is that we have only tested aperiodic 1/f slope-offset, and periodic features PF and BR based on VSTM task; therefore, further investigation is warranted relating the RS 1/f slope-offset, PF, BR with the performance in different cognitive tasks (e.g., lexical processing, episodic memory encoding and retrieval, emotion regulation, fluid and crystallized intelligence). Additionally, we have also not looked at these three measures trial-wise instead related resting state brain dynamics (periodic and aperiodic) with behavioral responses from the same participants. Another major limitation was posed by the Cam-CAN dataset, because of the presence of harmonics of lower frequencies in higher frequencies, we were not able to systematically tease apart the effect of 1/f activity on γ-frequency band. From a recent study employing visual steady state response task it has been shown γ band power systematically weakens with age which may have a crucial impact on attentional processing (Murty et al., 2020). Lastly, we still do not know which sources are responsible for 1/f baseline shift and is currently investigated in a separate future work, where source reconstruction and applying computational modeling on the source level data give us mechanistic understanding about the generative processes.
Despite these limitations, we think there is no loss of generality by focusing on visual short-term working memory processing task alone as age-associated change in aperiodic 1/f activity is pervasively present in all goal directed tasks and same for oscillatory changes quantified by PF and BR. Therefore, all these three normative measures proposed in this study together can track vast majority of alterations associated with healthy and atypical neurodevelopment and healthy and pathologic aging conditions under a variety of task settings which is important for developing non-invasive biomarker in future clinical applications.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by National Brain Research Centre (NBRC) Core funds and Computing Facility and Ramalingaswami Fellowships (Department of Biotechnology, Government of India; BT/RLF/Re-entry/07/2014 (to D.R.). D.R. was also supported by the Department of Science and Technology (DST) Ministry of Science and Technology, Government of India Extramural Grant SR/CSRI/21/2016. D.R. and A.B. were also supported by the NBRC Flagship Program BT/ MEDIII/NBRC/Flagship/Program/2019: Comparative mapping of common mental disorders (CMD) over lifespan. Data collection and sharing for this project was provided by the Cambridge Centre for Aging and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN). Cam-CAN funding was provided by the United Kingdom Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council Grant BB/H008217/1, together with support from the United Kingdom Medical Research Council and University of Cambridge, United Kingdom. In accordance with the data usage agreement for CAam-CAN dataset, the article has been submitted as open access.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.