Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Cognition and Behavior

μ-Opioid Receptor Stimulation in the Nucleus Accumbens Increases Vocal–Social Interactions in Flocking European Starlings, Sturnus Vulgaris

Alyse N. Maksimoski, Brandon J. Polzin, Sharon A. Stevenson, Changjiu Zhao and Lauren V. Riters
eNeuro 2 September 2021, 8 (5) ENEURO.0219-21.2021; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0219-21.2021
Alyse N. Maksimoski
Department of Integrative Biology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin 53706
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Brandon J. Polzin
Department of Integrative Biology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin 53706
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sharon A. Stevenson
Department of Integrative Biology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin 53706
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Changjiu Zhao
Department of Integrative Biology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin 53706
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lauren V. Riters
Department of Integrative Biology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin 53706
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Visual Abstract

Figure
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint

Abstract

Social connections in gregarious species are vital for safety and survival. For these reasons, many bird species form large flocks outside the breeding season. It has been proposed that such large social groups may be maintained via reward induced by positive interactions with conspecifics and via the reduction of a negative affective state caused by social separation. Moreover, within a flock optimal social spacing between conspecifics is important, indicating that individuals may optimize spacing to be close but not too close to conspecifics. The μ-opioid receptors (MORs) in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) are well known for their role in both reward and the reduction of negative affective states, suggesting that MOR stimulation in NAc may play a critical role in flock cohesion. To begin to test this hypothesis, social and nonsocial behaviors were examined in male and female European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) in nonbreeding flocks after intra-NAc infusion of saline and three doses of the selective MOR agonist d-Ala2-N-Me-Phe4-Glycol5-enkephalin (DAMGO). DAMGO in NAc dose-dependently increased singing behavior and facilitated social approaches while at the same time promoting displacements potentially used to maintain social spacing. These findings support the hypothesis that MORs in NAc promote social interactions important for group cohesion in nonsexual contexts and suggest the possibility that MORs in the NAc play a role in optimizing the pull of joining a flock with the push of potential agonistic encounters.

  • flocking
  • mesolimbic reward pathway
  • nucleus accumbens
  • opioids
  • social behavior
  • songbirds

Significance Statement

Social interactions with conspecifics are vital for safety and survival. Group cohesion in gregarious species may be maintained by social reward and reduced negative affect, yet underlying mechanisms have not been well studied. The μ-opioid receptors (MORs) in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) are strongly implicated in reward. Here, we demonstrate in flocks of European starlings that MOR stimulation in the NAc increases affiliative singing and approach behaviors as well as displacements. These results demonstrate a role for MORs in the NAc in both affiliative behaviors and behaviors used to maintain social spacing. Findings suggest that opioids in the NAc may play a role in optimizing the pull of joining a flock with the push of potential agonistic encounters.

Introduction

In many species, social connections outside a breeding context are vital for both safety and survival. For instance, schooling allows teleost fish to avoid predators (Magurran, 1990) and coordinated hunting allows clans of spotted hyenas to more effectively hunt and defend large prey (Holekamp et al., 1997). Many bird species form remarkable flocks, often outside the context of breeding, that offer protection from predators and increase foraging efficiency (Powell, 1974; Sullivan, 1984). It has been proposed that such large social groups may be maintained by rewarding positive interactions with conspecifics and by reducing negative states caused by social separation (Emlen, 1952; Riters et al., 2019a). Moreover, within a flock optimal social spacing between conspecifics is important, indicating that individuals may optimize spacing to be close but not too close to conspecifics. Neuropeptides in the oxytocin and vasopressin family have been a major focus of research on nonsexual social bonding and flocking in birds (Goodson et al., 2009). However, opioids also underlie social behavior and are well known for both inducing reward and reducing negative affective states (Panksepp et al., 1980; Olds, 1982; Goeders et al., 1984). This suggests that opioids may play a central role in the maintenance of social groups.

Across species, μ-opioid receptor (MOR) stimulation facilitates nonsexual prosocial interactions. MOR activation is proposed to alleviate distress from maternal separation (Herman and Panksepp, 1978) and to reward social play (Normansell and Panksepp, 1990; Vanderschuren et al., 2016). Opioids are also strongly implicated in social interactions that promote cohesion in large groups, such as schools of fish (Kavaliers, 1981) and songbird flocks. In zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, administration of the nonselective opioid receptor antagonist naloxone suppresses undirected song—a type of song proposed to play a role in flock cohesion (Khurshid et al., 2010). In European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris, MOR stimulation facilitates singing behavior in flocks, referred to here as gregarious song, as well as the positive affective state associated with gregarious song (Stevenson et al., 2020). Additionally, gregarious singing behavior is associated with opioid-mediated analgesia (Kelm-Nelson et al., 2012). These findings together suggest that opioids released in association with social interactions may act at MORs to promote social cohesion by inducing reward and reducing the pain of being alone (Kelm-Nelson et al., 2012; Riters and Stevenson, 2012; Riters et al., 2014; Hahn et al., 2017; Stevenson et al., 2020).

Opioids act at MORs in numerous brain regions to induce reward and to alleviate negative affect. In songbirds, several studies demonstrate that the medial preoptic area [mPOA (commonly abbreviated as POM in birds)] is a crucial site in which MOR activation stimulates song in flocks and underlies song-associated reward (Riters et al., 2005, 2014; Stevenson et al., 2020). In mammals, the majority of studies on the role of MOR in reward, including nonsexual social reward (e.g., social play; Vanderschuren et al., 1997; Trezza et al., 2011; Manduca et al., 2016), focus on the nucleus accumbens (NAc), an integral part of the mesolimbic pathway that underlies motivation and reward (Salgado and Kaplitt, 2015). A few correlational studies have examined a potential role for the avian NAc in social behaviors such as pair bonding and female responses to male courtship (Svec et al., 2009; Earp and Maney, 2012; Riters et al., 2013), but none have tested the effects of experimental manipulations of MOR in the NAc on avian social behavior.

It could be that MORs in the avian mPOA are important for facilitating nonsexual social interactions in flocks, and a separate system involving MORs in the NAc underlies similar behaviors in mammals. However, the mPOA is part of a circuit that accesses the NAc through projections to the ventral tegmental area (Balthazart et al., 1994; Riters and Alger, 2004), suggesting that studies of social interactions in birds and mammals may be focusing on separate parts of an evolutionarily conserved circuity. Recent evidence in support of this idea comes from a study in juvenile rats that demonstrates a role for MORs in the mPOA in social play (Zhao et al., 2020). If MORs in the NAc are part of a conserved circuitry that stimulates social connections outside of a breeding context across vertebrates, then we predict that MOR stimulation in the NAc will facilitate prosocial behaviors in songbirds. To test this, we administered the selective MOR agonist d-Ala2-N-Me-Phe4-Glycol5-enkephalin (DAMGO) into the NAc of male and female European starlings (S. vulgaris) and measured vocal–social behaviors in nonbreeding flocks.

Materials and Methods

Animals and housing

All animal procedures were performed in accordance with the University of Wisconsin-Madison animal care committee regulations and adhere to National Institutes of Health guidelines.

Fourteen European starlings (S. vulgaris), eight males and six females, were included as experimental birds in this study. Additional birds were also included as unmanipulated flock mates to bring each flock to a total of either six or eight birds. The starlings were trapped from a local farm in Madison, Wisconsin and housed in same-sex cages with an 18 h/6 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 6:00 A.M.) until molting was complete. This photoperiod induces a condition referred to as “photo-refractoriness” that is characteristic of early fall when birds begin to sing in large mixed-sex flocks (Dawson et al., 2001). Birds were then housed with flockmates for the duration of the study in indoor aviaries (size: 2.13 × 2.4 × 1.98 m) that were decorated with tree branches and supplied with food, drinking water, and bathing water ad libitum. Talk radio was playing outside the aviaries during daylight hours to habituate birds to sounds outside the room.

This study was conducted from September 2019 through March 2020. After birds were tested and removed from the aviaries, new birds were added to each aviary. A single observer watched the flock daily to identify singing birds for inclusion in the study. During observation periods, audio recordings of starling song were played to facilitate singing behavior (Marius Travell, YouTube). Once singing birds were identified and selected, they underwent surgery to implant a cannula guide targeting the NAc (detailed below). A maximum of two birds from each aviary were selected to be tested on alternate days, and there was always at least one unmanipulated singing bird present in the aviary to facilitate flock song.

NAc cannula surgery

The cannula targeted the location of the songbird NAc proposed by Reiner et al. (2004). This region is in the rostral striatum, located medially surrounding the ventral tips of the lateral ventricles, which in our sections appears in coronal sections in which Area X is relatively small and round (Fig. 1). An 8 mm, 26 gauge cannula guide (C315G/SPC, Plastics One) was placed unilaterally into either the left or right NAc following procedures similar to those cited in the study by Kelm-Nelson et al. (2013). Birds were given an intramuscular injection of 0.10 ml of ketofen (Zoetis), anesthetized with isoflurane/oxygen gas (isoflurane: Patterson Veterinary; oxygen: Airgas), and secured in a stereotaxic apparatus (model 995, Kopf) using ear bars in the most rostral position of the ear, with the beak ∼45° below the plane of the ear bars. The dorsal head feathers were trimmed, and a small incision was made in the skin to visualize the skull. The cannula guide tip was laterally zeroed on the midvein, in a position 1.2 mm rostral to the ear bars. Then the guide was laterally rotated 4.5° and adjusted 1.0 mm to the left or the right, and vertical zero was again taken from the midvein. Three holes were drilled in the skull, one for the guide and two superficial holes for the screws. The guide tip was lowered ventrally 6.1 mm from the skull zero (the coordinate for dorsal NAc) and was secured using screws and instant dental cement (Ortho-Jet dental cement powder and acrylic liquid, Lang Dental Manufacturing). Once the dental cement was solidified, a dummy cannula (Plastics One) was fitted into the guide. The animal was then allowed to recover under monitoring until alert, then was transported and released into its home aviary. The birds were monitored that evening and again the following morning. Experimental treatments and observations began after the bird resumed singing in its flock, approximately 7 d after surgery.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

A–C, Location of DAMGO infusion sites. Illustration of one hemisphere of starling brain, with hits represented by filled-in shapes and misses represented by open shapes. Males are represented by squares, and females are represented by circles. D, Photomicrograph of 2× magnification of a Nissl-stained brain section that demonstrates a hit with the cannula tip located in the NAc. Dotted line indicates boundaries of NAc. Scale bar, 500 μm (or 0.5 mm). AC, Anterior commissure; MSt, medial striatum; Rt, nucleus rotundus.

Pharmacological manipulations

The experimental birds were behaviorally tested on 4 d separated by at least 1 d. If two birds from an aviary were tested, they were tested on alternate days, such that only one bird per aviary was tested on a single day. A habituation treatment of vehicle (saline) was injected at least 2 d before the commencement of the treatment sequence to habituate birds to the procedure (details for the injection procedure are provided below). After this habituation injection, each treatment sequence lasted ∼3 weeks, consisting of the following four treatments: vehicle (sterile saline, 0.85%; 0.50 μl) and three doses of DAMGO (Sigma-Aldrich) [low dose, 0.025 μg; intermediate dose, 0.25 μg; or high dose, 2.5 μg (all dissolved in 0.50 μl sterile saline); catalog #100929–53-1]. Each bird received each treatment once in a counterbalanced order, and a minimum of 48 h separated treatments. The treatments were color coded by another researcher so that the observer would be blind to treatment conditions during all observations.

On each test day, injections began between 9:30 A.M. and 3:30 P.M. Experimental birds were swiftly caught in a net and anesthetized with isoflurane/oxygen using a nose cone. The cannula dummy was removed and replaced with a 33 gauge cannula connected to PE50 tubing (catalog #C232CT, Plastics One) containing the color-coded treatment solution. The cannula extended 2 mm beyond the tip of the cannula guide. A vacuum syringe (Hamilton) connected to a Nanomite Syringe Pump (Harvard Apparatus) injected 0.50 μl of the treatment solution over a 120 s period. The cannula was left in place for 180 s to allow for the equalization of pressure and diffusion from the cannula tip. Infusion volume was verified by following the movement of an air bubble in the tubing. The cannula was removed, the cannula dummy was replaced, and the bird was placed into a draped recovery cage for 15 min before being released into its home aviary.

Behavioral observations

A single researcher observed each focal animal using a continuous sampling technique. The following behaviors were recorded for 40 min: social approaches (the focal individual approaches to within 10 cm of another bird and remains near that bird); displacements (the focal individual approaches to within 10 cm of another bird followed by the receiving bird leaving proximity within 1 s); gregarious singing (sum of introductory whistles and song bouts separated by at least 1 s); calls (sum of calls; however, we did not identify specific call types); perch changes (sum of movement along or between branches separated by at least 1 s); beak wipes (beak wipes in starlings are not used as part of courtship and are considered to be a sign of stress; Bauer et al., 2011); and feeding (sum of bouts of feeding separated by at least 1 s or a complete head lift). After the first 20 min observation period, a high-reward food (i.e., horse feed, which attracts large flocks in local barns) was added to examine the possibility that MOR stimulation in NAc would stimulate hedonic feeding, as observed in rats (Bakshi and Kelley, 1993; Zhang et al., 1998); however, this food was ignored by the birds and will not be discussed further.

Cannula tip verification

Following the final treatments and observations, the birds were injected with 1.0 μl of Chicago Blue 6B dye (Thermo Fisher Scientific) or with a tract tracer using the same injection procedure as above, to identify the location of the tip of the cannulae. For the birds infused with blue dye, after infusion the birds were rapidly decapitated, and their brains were removed and frozen on crushed dry ice. The brains were stored at −80°C and then sectioned at 50 μm using a cryostat (catalog #CM1850, Leica Biosystems). Sections were mounted on slides, dehydrated, Nissl stained, and coverslipped. The slides were analyzed under a microscope to determine whether the blue dye was contained within the NAc (“hit”) or elsewhere (“miss”). For three birds, 0.20 μl of the neuronal tracer biotinylated dextran amine (molecular weight, 3000; lot #2089930, NeuroTrace) at 10% concentration was infused to confirm the site of injection (these infusions were part of a study not reported here). Ten days after infusion of the tracer, birds were perfused and the tracer was visualized as described in the study by Riters and Alger (2004). Sex and nonbreeding condition were verified immediately following death via the presence and size of testes.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted using GraphPad Prism for Windows (version 8.0.0; GraphPad Software; www.graphpad.com). The proportion of each behavior produced on each day was used for analysis to control for high levels of individual variation in behavior across birds (Table 1, untransformed descriptive statistics). Specifically, for each bird and each behavioral measure, we took the sum of that behavior for each test day and divided it by the total sum of that behavior for all days. Difference scores between behavior frequency after treatments and behavior frequency under saline were also examined, and results were consistent with proportions of behavior; here we report the proportions. A general linear model (GLM) was run for each behavior, with treatment entered as repeated measures, cannula location entered as a factor, and proportion of behavior entered as response variables, with separate analyses run for each behavior. Tukey’s HSD post hoc tests were run following significant (p < 0.05) GLM results. Some of the misses targeted areas in which we expected MORs to regulate social behavior (see Discussion), which reduced the likelihood of seeing a significant interaction, potentially precluding post hoc comparisons of doses within treatment groups. However, the goal of this study is to characterize the role of MOR specifically in the NAc in social behaviors. Thus, to provide additional insight, in cases where there was a dose effect but no dose × cannula interaction, we ran exploratory Dunnett’s post hoc analyses on hits only to compare the effects of the most effective dose of agonist (i.e., the highest dose) against the other treatments. A Levene’s test was run to test the assumption of homogeneity of variance, and a Lilliefors test was run to test the assumption of normality. When assumptions were violated, analyses were run on log-transformed data. For instances where there were zero occurrences of a behavior, a formula of log(x + 0.05) was used. When transformation did not correct the violation of assumptions, nonparametric Friedman’s ANOVAs were used to examine treatment differences for hits and for misses. Effect sizes were calculated using η2. Confidence intervals at 95% were determined using the untransformed proportions of each behavior. Pearson correlations for all behaviors at baseline (saline) were run to examine potential relationships between behaviors.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Untransformed behavior measurements for each treatment and condition

Results

The cannula tip was contained within the bounds of the NAc in six birds (hits; four males, two females) and outside the NAc in eight birds (misses; four males, four females; Fig. 1). For the misses, the cannula tips were contained within the striatum lateral to NAc (n = 4), the lateral hypothalamus (LH; n = 1), the mPOA (n = 1), and the tractus septopallio-mesencephalicus (TSM; n = 1), the ventricle (n = 1). There was not adequate statistical power to analyze sex, but results indicate that the same trends were observed in males and females, so males and females were combined, but figures indicate males and females (Stevenson et al., 2020).

Effects of MOR stimulation on social behaviors

Singing behavior

Statistics were not conducted on singing behavior because most birds did not sing, driving variance down to zero for most doses (Fig. 2A, Table 1, untransformed descriptive statistics). Of six hits, three birds (50%; two males, one female) sang when treated with the highest dose of MOR agonist. Of eight misses, only two birds sang at any dose (one male, one female). The male sang at the lowest dose of the MOR agonist (0.025 μg DAMGO), and the cannula tip was located in the striatum lateral to NAc. The female sang at the highest dose (2.5 μg DAMGO), and the cannula tip was located in the mPOA.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Effects of MOR stimulation in NAc on song, calls, social approaches, and displacements (mean ± SEM). A–D, The number of song bouts (A), number of calls (B), proportion of approaches (C), and proportion of displacements (D) in male (squares) and female (circles) starlings in which the cannula tip missed the NAc (n = 8) or hit the NAc (n = 6). Analyses were run on log-transformed data for both C and D to correct for assumptions, but here we show untransformed proportions. Dotted lines represent pairwise significance for intra-NAc dose effect. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

Calling behavior

For calls, a GLM revealed a significant main effect for dose (F(3,36) = 6.95, p = 0.013, η2 = 0.317). No significant main effect was found for cannula location (F(1,12) = 1.37, p = 1.0, η2 = 0.0), but there was a significant dose × cannula location interaction (F(3,36) = 3.32, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.188). Post hoc analysis revealed that birds with a cannula tip in the NAc treated with the highest dose of MOR agonist called significantly more than when treated with the lowest dose of MOR agonist (p = 0.023), but not with saline (p = 0.064) or the intermediate dose of MOR agonist (p = 0.15). There were no significant differences found between any dose for misses [p > 0.05 for all comparisons; Fig. 2B (also see Fig. 4A), Tables 1, 2, untransformed descriptive statistics and confidence intervals].

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2

95% Confidence intervals based on the proportions of the occurrences for each behavior and condition

Social approaches

A GLM run on the log-transformed approach data revealed a significant main effect for dose (F(3,36) = 4.84, p = 0.007, η2 = 0.233). No significant main effect was found for cannula location (F(1,12) = 0.17, p = 0.689, η2 = 0.001), and there was no dose × cannula location interaction (F(3,36) = 1.84, p = 0.157, η2 = 0.089). Post hoc analysis of the dose effect revealed that birds treated with the highest dose of MOR agonist approached significantly more often than birds treated with saline (p = 0.019). Post hoc analysis run on the hits demonstrates that the highest dose of intra-NAc MOR agonist was significantly higher than that of saline (p = 0.032) and the lowest dose (p = 0.034), but not the intermediate dose, of MOR agonist [p = 0.523; Fig. 2C (also see Fig. 4B), Tables 1, 2].

Displacements

A GLM run on the log-transformed sum of displacements revealed a significant main effect for dose (F(3,36) = 5.33, p = 0.009, η2 = 0.248). No significant main effect was found for cannula location (F(1,12) = 0.14, p = 0.712, η2 = 0.001) or the dose × cannula location interaction (F(3,36) = 2.27, p = 0.097, η2 = 0.106). Post hoc analysis of the dose effect revealed that birds displayed a significantly higher proportion of displacements when treated with the highest dose of MOR agonist than when treated with the low dose (p = 0.015) or intermediate dose (p = 0.034) of MOR agonist. Post hoc analysis run on the hits demonstrates that the highest dose of intra-NAc MOR agonist yielded a significantly higher proportion of displacements than saline (p = 0.040) and the lowest dose (p = 0.022) but not the intermediate dose (p = 0.088) of MOR agonist [Fig. 2D and (also see Fig. 4C), Tables 1, 2].

Effects of MOR stimulation on nonsocial behaviors

Perch changes

For perch changes, a GLM revealed a significant main effect for dose (F(3,36) = 18.94, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.560). No significant main effect was found for cannula location (F(1,12) = 2.56, p = 0.135, η2 = 0.0), but there was a significant dose × cannula location interaction (F(3,36) = 4.70, p = 0.007, η2 = 0.139). Post hoc analysis revealed that birds with a cannula tip in the NAc treated with the highest dose of MOR agonist had a significantly higher proportion of perch changes than when treated with saline (p = 0.008) or the lowest dose of MOR agonist (p = 0.003), but not the intermediate dose of MOR agonist (p = 0.103). There were no significant differences found between any dose for misses (p > 0.05 for all comparisons; Figs. 3A, 4D, Tables 1, 2).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Effects of MOR stimulation in NAc on nonsocial behaviors (mean ± SEM). A–C, Proportion of perch changes (A), feeding (B), and beak wipes (C), in male (squares) and female (circles) starlings in which the cannula tip missed the NAc (n = 8) or hit the NAc (n = 6). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Effects of MOR stimulation in locations in which the cannula tip was located outside NAc (mean ± SEM). A–F, Proportion of calls (A), social approaches (B), displacements (C), perch changes (D), feeding (E), and beak wipes (F) in birds with cannula located in the mPOA, TSM, lateral ventricle, and striatum lateral to NAc (Fig. 1). n = 1 for each location except for the striatum (n = 4) where the mean ± SEM is given.

Feeding

A transformation of feeding data did not correct assumptions needed to run parametric statistics. Nonparametric Friedman’s ANOVA tests yielded a significant effect for dose for hits (ANOVA, χ2 test = 7.48 (n = 6, df = 3, p = 0.048), but not misses [ANOVA, χ2 test = 7.78 (n = 8, df = 3, p = 0.051); Figs. 3B, 4E, Tables 1, 2]. Post hoc analysis yielded no significant differences between doses for either hits or misses (p > 0.05 for all comparisons), although for hits the highest dose compared with saline was p = 0.06.

Beak wipes

For beak wipes, a GLM revealed a significant main effect for cannula location (F(1,12) = 7.71, p = 0.017, η2 = 0.0). No significant main effect was found for dose (F(3,36) = 1.26, p = 0.304, η2 = 0.092) or the dose × cannula location interaction (F(3,36) = 0.29, p = 0.831, η2 = 0.020; Figs. 3C, 4F, Tables 1, 2). However, post hoc analysis yielded no significant difference between hits and misses at any dose (p > 0.05 for all comparisons).

Correlation of behaviors

In addition, correlations were run for all behaviors listed above to determine whether the base levels of any behavior correlated with another behavior, possibly indicating that an increase in one would drive an increase in another. However, we found no evidence of any correlations between these behaviors (p > 0.05 for all comparisons; Table 3).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3

Correlation matrix for behavioral proportions at baseline (saline) in combined hits and misses

Discussion

This study is the first to demonstrate a causal role for the songbird NAc in the facilitation of social interactions in nonsexual gregarious contexts. Results demonstrate a role for MOR in NAc in behaviors considered important for group cohesion, including gregarious song, social approach, and displacements, with similar patterns observed in males and females. More broadly, these data provide evidence that the NAc may be part of a conserved circuitry that promotes social cohesion in nonsexual contexts across vertebrates.

MOR stimulation in the NAc increased social-spacing behaviors

The highest intra-NAc dose of the MOR agonist DAMGO facilitated social approach behaviors, offering support for a role for MOR in the NAc in prosocial behavior. Results of past studies in rodents also demonstrate a role for MORs in social proximity; however, in these studies peripherally administered MOR agonists decreased the amount of time rodents spent near conspecifics (Herman and Panksepp, 1978; Panksepp et al., 1979). Although the findings were in the opposite direction from our study, these past results were interpreted to suggest that the agonist replaced the need for reward that would normally be induced by opioids released by social contact (Herman and Panksepp, 1978). Indeed, many rewarding behaviors [e.g., social play (Trezza et al., 2010; Achterberg et al., 2019) and hedonic feeding (Berridge, 2009; Gosnell and Levine, 2009)] are facilitated by low doses and inhibited by higher doses of MOR agonists. This suggests that the dose of agonist and site-specificity in our study may have been sufficient to facilitate social behavior, but not sufficient to fully replace social reward. Future studies are needed to test this hypothesis.

The highest intra-NAc dose of the MOR agonist DAMGO also stimulated displacements, a mildly agonistic behavior. This is similar to past findings that show that peripheral administration of a MOR agonist facilitates agonistic behaviors in response to an intruder in mice (Campbell Teskey and Kavaliers, 1988); however, relative to territorial defense, the agonistic interactions in the present study were relatively nonthreatening. Starlings do not maintain strong, linear dominance hierarchies when in large flocks and are often observed sharing food sources. Therefore the function of displacements does not appear to relate to dominance, but may play a role in the maintenance of adequate social spacing (King, 1973). Starlings may space themselves to maintain optimal levels of natural MOR stimulation, and infusion of a MOR agonist may slightly reduce the need for close social contact, as previously stated, perhaps triggering mildly agonistic interactions to maintain social spacing. The finding that the activation of MORs in the NAc stimulates approach while at the same time increasing behaviors used to maintain social spacing suggests that MOR in the NAc may play a role in optimizing the pull of joining the flock with the push of potential agonistic encounters.

MOR stimulation may stimulate vocal behaviors

Although few birds sang, the administration of the highest intra-NAc dose of the MOR agonist DAMGO initiated gregarious singing behavior in 50% of birds tested. Song in this context is highly sensitive to stressors, and when a bird is caught and injected, it stops singing (Stevenson et al., 2020); therefore, restoring singing behavior with intra-NAc MOR stimulation is noteworthy. This type of gregarious singing behavior in a nonbreeding context is associated with an intrinsic reward state (Kelm-Nelson et al., 2012; Riters and Stevenson, 2012; Riters et al., 2014; Hahn et al., 2017; Stevenson et al., 2020) and is proposed to be a form of play behavior that allows birds to develop important social skills for use in more serious reproductive contexts (Riters et al., 2019b). MOR stimulation in the NAc also dramatically stimulates and rewards social play in rodents (Vanderschuren et al., 1997; Trezza et al., 2011; Manduca et al., 2016). Thus, the present study is the first to implicate MOR in the NAc in gregarious song, and it provides neuropharmacological support for the hypothesis that playful behaviors involve neural systems that are conserved across vertebrates.

In addition, the highest intra-NAc dose of the MOR agonist DAMGO stimulated calls. Functionally distinct calls are used by starlings in flocks (Feare, 1984); however, we did not distinguish between different call types. Therefore, although we can conclude that MOR stimulation increased calls in this study, future studies are needed to explore the functional role of opioids in calling behavior.

MOR stimulation of nonsocial behaviors

This is the first study to examine the effects of MOR agonist administration in the NAc of songbirds. The present study focused on the location of the songbird NAc proposed by Reiner et al. (2004). The functional homology of this site to the mammalian NAc has not been well studied. To both determine the degree to which our focal area is functionally similar to the mammalian NAc and to explore the specificity of effects on social behavior, we measured motor activity, feeding, and stress-related behaviors—all behaviors that are influenced by MOR agonist infusion into the NAc in rodents.

The highest intra-NAc dose of the MOR agonist DAMGO increased motor activity, as reflected in perch changes. Perch changes did not correlate with either approaches or social-spacing behaviors, therefore we do not consider motor activity to be driving the increase seen in either type of behavior. This increase in motor activity is consistent with studies that show similar effects in rodents (Vezina et al., 1987; Cunningham and Kelley, 1992; Bakshi and Kelley, 1993). Intra-NAc MOR stimulation also increased feeding, with the high dose, when compared with saline, being just shy of significance (p = 0.06). Several studies in rats demonstrate that intra-NAc DAMGO administration most powerfully increases hedonic feeding, including the consumption of highly palatable, high-fat food (Bakshi and Kelley, 1993; Zhang et al., 1998; Will et al., 2003). Therefore, it is possible that if the present study examined effects on a highly palatable food option, there may have been a more substantial increase in feeding behavior. Although peripheral MOR administration alters stress responses, the present data found that administration of MOR agonist in the avian NAc had no impact on beak wipes, which are considered an indication of stress in starlings (Bauer et al., 2011). These data suggest that opioids may act on MOR in other brain regions outside the NAc in flocking songbirds to reduce stress.

Effects of MOR stimulation outside NAc

Although birds with the cannula located outside the NAc were considered controls, for some of these birds the cannula tips were located in brain regions known to influence social and other behaviors. Sample sizes in most cases consist of a single bird, so individual cases are not analyzed; rather, a few cases are highlighted here that are consistent with past research. Specifically, for one of the birds the cannula tip was located in the mPOA. For this bird, the highest dose of the MOR agonist DAMGO stimulated gregarious song, which is consistent the findings of a recent study that showed that downregulation of MOR in the starling mPOA reduces gregarious singing (Stevenson et al., 2020). The highest dose of the MOR agonist DAMGO also stimulated feeding in birds with the cannula tip located in the striatum with a similar result observed for a bird with the tip located in the lateral hypothalamus, results that are similar to findings in rodents (Levine and Billington, 1989; Zhang and Kelley, 2000; Castro et al., 2015; Ardianto et al., 2016). The finding that displacement behaviors were higher for birds receiving high doses of MOR agonist in the lateral hypothalamus and mPOA is consistent with studies that implicate these regions in agonistic behavior (Panksepp, 1971; Hammond and Rowe, 1976; Schlinger and Callard, 1990; Zhang and Kelley, 2000; Nieh et al., 2016). These results suggest sites in which MOR may act to influence these behaviors; thus, the misses support past research and suggest potential roles for MOR in areas outside the NAc in social and nonsocial behaviors that can be tested in future research.

Conclusions

The results of this study suggest that a region identified as the NAc in birds (Reiner et al., 2004) is functionally homologous to the mammalian NAc, and that this region may be part of a core, conserved circuitry that underlies rewarding social behaviors across vertebrates. Future studies are now needed to examine proposed shell and core subdivisions of NAc in birds, which in mammals play distinct roles in reward (Kelley, 1999), as well as the extent to which the mesolimbic reward pathway is differentially involved in modulating social spacing behaviors seasonally in songbirds.

Acknowledgments

We thank the animal care staff—Chris Elliott, Jeffrey Alexander, and Kate Skogen—without whom this work would not be possible.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01-MH-119041.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Achterberg EJM, van Swieten MMH, Houwing DJ, Trezza V, Vanderschuren LJMJ (2019) Opioid modulation of social play reward in juvenile rats. Neuropharmacology 159:107332. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2018.09.007 pmid:30218673
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Ardianto C, Yonemochi N, Yamamoto S, Yang L, Takenoya F, Shioda S, Nagase H, Ikeda H, Kamei J (2016) Opioid systems in the lateral hypothalamus regulate feeding behavior through orexin and GABA neurons. Neuroscience 320:183–193. pp doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.02.002 pmid:26855191
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Bakshi VP, Kelley AE (1993) Feeding induced by opioid stimulation of the ventral striatum: role of opiate receptor subtypes. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 265:1253–1260. pmid:8389860
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    Balthazart J, Dupiereux V, Aste N, Viglietti-Panzica C, Barrese M, Panzica GC (1994) Afferent and efferent connections of the sexually dimorphic medial preoptic nucleus of the male quail revealed by in vitro transport of DiI. Cell Tissue Res 276:455–475. doi:10.1007/BF00343944 pmid:8062340
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Bauer CM, Glassman LW, Cyr NE, Romero LM (2011) Effects of predictable and unpredictable food restriction on the stress response in molting and non-molting European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol 160:390–399. doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2011.07.009 pmid:21801846
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Berridge KC (2009) “Liking” and “wanting” food rewards: brain substrates and roles in eating disorders. Physiol Behav 97:537–550. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.044 pmid:19336238
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Campbell Teskey G, Kavaliers M (1988) Effects of opiate agonists and antagonists on aggressive encounters and subsequent opioid-induced analgesia, activity and feeding responses in male mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 31:43–52. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(88)90309-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Castro DC, Cole SL, Berridge KC (2015) Lateral hypothalamus, nucleus accumbens, and ventral pallidum roles in eating and hunger: interactions between homeostatic and reward circuitry. Front Syst Neurosci 9:90. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2015.00090 pmid:26124708
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Cunningham ST, Kelley AE (1992) Opiate infusion into nucleus accumbens: contrasting effects on motor activity and responding for conditioned reward. Brain Res 588:104–114. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(92)91349-j pmid:1327405
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    Dawson A, King VM, Bentley GE, Ball GF (2001) Photoperiodic control of seasonality in birds. J Biol Rhythms 16:365–380. doi:10.1177/074873001129002079 pmid:11506381
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Earp SE, Maney DL (2012) Birdsong: is it music to their ears? Front Evol Neurosci 4:14. doi:10.3389/fnevo.2012.00014 pmid:23226128
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Emlen JT (1952) Flocking behavior in birds. Auk 69:160–170. doi:10.2307/4081266
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  13. ↵
    Feare C (1984) The starling. Oxford, UK: Oxford UP.
  14. ↵
    Goeders NE, Lane JD, Smith JE (1984) Self-administration of methionine enkephalin into the nucleus accumbens. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 20:451–455. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(84)90284-3 pmid:6324244
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Goodson JL, Schrock SE, Klatt JD, Kabelik D, Kingsbury MA (2009) Mesotocin and nonapeptide receptors promote estrildid flocking behavior. Science 325:862–866. doi:10.1126/science.1174929 pmid:19679811
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    Gosnell BA, Levine AS (2009) Reward systems and food intake: role of opioids. Int J Obes (Lond) 33 [Suppl 2]:S54–S58. doi:10.1038/ijo.2009.73 pmid:19528981
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Hahn AH, Merullo DP, Spool JA, Angyal CS, Stevenson SA, Riters LV (2017) Song-associated reward correlates with endocannabinoid-related gene expression in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Neuroscience 346:255–266. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.01.028 pmid:28147243
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Hammond MA, Rowe FA (1976) Medial preoptic and anterior hypothalamic lesions: influences on aggressive behavior in female hamsters. Physiol Behav 17:507–513. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(76)90115-3 pmid:1034941
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Herman BH, Panksepp J (1978) Effects of morphine and naloxone on separation distress and approach attachment: evidence for opiate mediation of social affect. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 9:213–220. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(78)90167-3 pmid:568801
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Holekamp KE, Smale L, Berg R, Cooper SM (1997) Hunting rates and hunting success in the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta). J Zool 242:1–15. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1997.tb02925.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  21. ↵
    Kavaliers M (1981) Schooling behavior of fish: an opiate-dependent activity? Behav Neural Biol 33:397–401. doi:10.1016/s0163-1047(81)91743-x pmid:6277292
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Kelley AE (1999) Neural integrative activities of nucleus accumbens subregions in relation to learning and motivation. Psychobiology 27:198–213.
    OpenUrl
  23. ↵
    Kelm-Nelson CA, Stevenson SA, Riters LV (2012) Context-dependent links between song production and opioid-mediated analgesia in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). PLoS One 7:e46721. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046721 pmid:23056422
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Kelm-Nelson CA, Stevenson SA, Cordes MA, Riters LV (2013) Modulation of male song by naloxone in the medial preoptic nucleus. Behav Neurosci 127:451–457. doi:10.1037/a0032329 pmid:23544595
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Khurshid N, Jayaprakash N, Hameed LS, Mohanasundaram S, Iyengar S (2010) Opioid modulation of song in male zebra finches (Taenopygia guttata). Behav Brain Res 208:359–370. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.003 pmid:20015456
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    King JA (1973) The ecology of aggressive behavior. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 4:117–138. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.04.110173.001001
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  27. ↵
    Levine AS, Billington CJ (1989) Opioids are they regulators of feeding? Ann N Y Acad Sci 575:209–220. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1989.tb53244.x pmid:2699188
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Magurran AE (1990) The adaptive significance of schooling as an anti-predator defence in fish. Annales Zoologici Fennici 27:51–66.
    OpenUrl
  29. ↵
    Manduca A, Lassalle O, Sepers M, Campolongo P, Cuomo V, Marsicano G, Kieffer B, Vanderschuren LJ, Trezza V, Manzoni OJ (2016) Interacting cannabinoid and opioid receptors in the nucleus accumbens core control adolescent social play. Front Behav Neurosci 10:211. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00211 pmid:27899885
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Nieh EH, Vander Weele CM, Matthews GA, Presbrey KN, Wichmann R, Leppla CA, Izadmehr EM, Tye KM (2016) Inhibitory input from the lateral hypothalamus to the ventral tegmental area disinhibits dopamine neurons and promotes behavioral activation neuron. Neuron 90:1286–1298. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.035 pmid:27238864
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Normansell L, Panksepp J (1990) Effects of morphine and naloxone on play-rewarded spatial discrimination in juvenile rats. Dev Psychobiol 23:75–83. doi:10.1002/dev.420230108 pmid:2160387
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Olds ME (1982) Reinforcing effects of morphine in the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res 237:429–440. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(82)90454-1 pmid:7083004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Panksepp J (1971) Aggression elicited by electrical stimulation of the hypothalamus in albino rats. Physiol Behav 6:321–329. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(71)90163-6 pmid:4948212
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Panksepp J, Najam N, Soares F (1979) Morphine reduces social cohesion in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 11:131–134. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(79)90002-9 pmid:504292
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Panksepp J, Herman BH, Vilberg T, Bishop P, DeEskinazi FG (1980) Endogenous opioids and social behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 4:473–487. doi:10.1016/0149-7634(80)90036-6 pmid:6258111
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Powell GVN (1974) Experimental analysis of the social value of flocking by starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) in relation to predation and foraging. Anim Behav 22:501–505. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(74)80049-7
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  37. ↵
    Reiner A, Perkel DJ, Bruce LL, Butler AB, Csillag A, Kuenzel W, Medina L, Paxinos G, Shimizu T, Striedter G, Wild M, Ball GF, Durand S, Güntürkün O, Lee DW, Mello CV, Powers A, White SA, Hough G, Kubikova L, et al. (2004) Revised nomenclature for avian telencephalon and some related brainstem nuclei. J Comp Neurol 473:377–414. doi:10.1002/cne.20118 pmid:15116397
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Riters LV, Alger SJ (2004) Neuroanatomical evidence for indirect connections between the medial preoptic nucleus and the song control system: possible neural substrates for sexually motivated song. Cell Tissue Res 316:35–44. doi:10.1007/s00441-003-0838-6 pmid:14968358
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Riters LV, Stevenson SA (2012) Reward and vocal production: song-associated place preference in songbirds. Physiol Behav 106:87–94. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.01.010 pmid:22285212
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Riters LV, Schroeder MB, Auger CJ, Eens M, Pinxten R, Ball GF (2005) Evidence for opioid involvement in the regulation of song production in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Behav Neurosci 119:245–255. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.245 pmid:15727529
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    Riters LV, Ellis JM, Angyal CS, Borkowski VJ, Cordes MA, Stevenson SA (2013) Links between breeding readiness, opioid immunolabeling, and the affective state induced by hearing male courtship song in female European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Behav Brain Res 247:117–124. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2013.02.041 pmid:23473880
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Riters LV, Stevenson SA, DeVries MS, Cordes MA (2014) Reward associated with singing behavior correlates with opioid-related gene expression in the medial preoptic nucleus in male European starlings. PLoS One 9:e115285. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0115285 pmid:25521590
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    Riters LV, Kelm-Nelson CA, Spool JA (2019a) Why do birds flock? A role for opioids in the reinforcement of gregarious social interactions. Front Physiol 10:421.
    OpenUrl
  44. ↵
    Riters LV, Spool JA, Merullo DP, Hahn AH (2019b) Song practice as a rewarding form of play in songbirds. Behav Processes 163:91–98. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2017.10.002 pmid:29031813
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Salgado S, Kaplitt MG (2015) The nucleus accumbens: a comprehensive review. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg 93:75–93. doi:10.1159/000368279 pmid:25720819
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Schlinger BA, Callard GV (1990) Aggressive behavior in birds: an experimental model for studies of brain-steroid inteactions. Comp Biochem Physiol A Physiol 97:307–316. doi:10.1016/0300-9629(90)90616-Z
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  47. ↵
    Stevenson SA, Piepenburg A, Spool JA, Angyal CS, Hahn AH, Zhao C, Riters LV (2020) Endogenous opioids facilitate intrinsically-rewarded birdsong. Sci Rep 10:11083. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-67684-1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  48. ↵
    Sullivan KA (1984) The advantages of social foraging in downy woodpeckers. Anim Behav 32:16–22. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(84)80319-X
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  49. ↵
    Svec LA, Licht KM, Wade J (2009) Pair bonding in the female zebra finch: a potential role for the nucleus taeniae. Neuroscience 160:275–283. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.02.003 pmid:19409212
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Trezza V, Baarendse PJ, Vanderschuren LJ (2010) The pleasures of play: pharmacological insights into social reward mechanisms. Trends Pharmacol Sci 31:463–469. doi:10.1016/j.tips.2010.06.008 pmid:20684996
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Trezza V, Damsteegt R, Achterberg EJ, Vanderschuren LJ (2011) Nucleus accumbens μ-opioid receptors mediate social reward. J Neurosci 31:6362–6370. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5492-10.2011 pmid:21525276
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    Vanderschuren LJ, Niesink RJ, Van Ree JM (1997) The neurobiology of social play behavior in rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 21:309–326. doi:10.1016/s0149-7634(96)00020-6 pmid:9168267
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Vanderschuren LJ, Achterberg EJ, Trezza V (2016) The neurobiology of social play and its rewarding value in rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 70:86–105. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.025 pmid:27587003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Vezina P, Kalivas PW, Stewart J (1987) Sensitization occurs to the locomotor effects of morphine and the specific μ opioid receptor agonist, DAGO, administered repeatedly to the ventral tegmental area but not to the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res 417:51–58. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(87)90178-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Will MJ, Franzblau EB, Kelley AE (2003) Nucleus accumbens μ-opioids regulate intake of a high-fat diet via activation of a distributed brain network. J Neurosci 23:2882–2888. pmid:12684475
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    Zhang M, Kelley AE (2000) Enhanced intake of high-fat food following striatal mu-opioid stimulation: microinjection mapping and fos expression. Neuroscience 99:267–277. doi:10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00198-6 pmid:10938432
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Zhang M, Gosnell BA, Kelley AE (1998) Intake of high-fat food is selectively enhanced by mu opioid receptor stimulation within the nucleus accumbens. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 285:908–914. pmid:9580643
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    Zhao C, Chang L, Auger AP, Gammie SC, Riters LV (2020) Mu opioid receptors in the medial preoptic area govern social play behavior in adolescent male rats. Genes Brain Behav 19:e12662.
    OpenUrl

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Karyn Frick, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Scott MacDougall-Shackleton.

The manuscript by Maksimoski et al. demonstrates that mu opioid receptors (MORs) in the nucleus Accumbens (NAc) are important for affiliative behaviors and to maintain social spacing. The reviewers felt that the authors have performed a well-designed study to investigate whether MORs in the NAc are involved in maintaining social connections in non-breeding flocks of European starlings. Because MORs in NAc are considered to underlie reward and the reduction of negative affective states, they have tested whether stimulating these receptors by injecting the MOR agonist DAMGO in NAc in non-breeding starlings of both sexes lead to changes in their social interactions. Their results suggest a dose-dependent increase in singing, social approaches, as well as mild agonistic behaviors for social spacing following infusions of DAMGO in NAc of both male and female birds. Their findings suggest that MOR in NAc are important for promoting social interactions while simultaneously maintaining social spacing for maintaining the flock in a non-breeding context.

Because earlier studies have shown similar functions for MORs in the rodent NAc, the reviewers agreed that the data significantly advance the field by suggesting homologous circuits between birds and mammals that are conserved across evolution. Moreover, the reviewers felt the paper to be very clear, well written, and well conducted, with explicitly stated objectives. In the words of one reviewer, “It is a beautiful piece of work”.

However, the reviewers did have some concerns that they felt must be addressed as follows:

Major Comments:

1. The authors summed behaviors within categories. The statistics they ran are fine, but summing to provide the data is not. When you sum, let’s say, 3 different behaviors , that gives each behavior equal weight. So a bird with score 3 may have two flights and a hop, and another bird with a score 3 may have 3 hops, and they are inappropriately treated as equivalent. The ‘raw’ data are not actually included in Table 1 as claimed. It just has the means and SEM. The raw data should be included or deposited online. The correct analysis here would be to combine the raw measures with a factor analysis, like principal components, and then run the GLM on the scores that are output from the factor analysis. Simply summing the behaviors is not statistically valid.

2. The authors have mentioned that experimental birds were behaviorally tested on 4 days separated by at least one day but they need to clarify whether each experimental bird was only tested once with each dose of DAMGO. Were the doses of DAMGO randomized across trials? How soon is DAMGO metabolized in starlings? Is there any possibility of cumulative effects of DAMGO across trials?

3. Other than song, were there any differences in the kinds of calls that the birds produced?

4. The authors suggest that social approach and social spacing may be part of the same behavior. While they have provided a good description of both, they should provide video recordings of these behaviors for further clarity.

5. Beak-wipes are important for grooming and courtship in most birds - the authors should include references of their being a sign of stress in starlings.

6. The results demonstrate that the increase in motor activity following DAMGO injections in NAc was not correlated with social approach or social spacing. Since both involve movement, this is difficult to understand and needs further clarification.

Minor Points:

7. Statistical Analysis: The sentence ‘Thus to provide additional insight, in cases where there was a dose effect but no dose-by-cannula interaction, we ran exploratory Dunnett’s post hoc analyses ...... ‘ is incomplete - an ‘of’ is missing here.

8. Discussion: In the section on ‘MOR stimulation of non-social behaviors’ (second paragraph):

The sentence ‘Several studies in rats demonstrate that intra-NAc DAMGO administration most powerfully increases consumption of hedonic feeding including highly palatable, high fat food’ should be replaced by:

‘Several studies in rats demonstrate that intra-NAc DAMGO administration most powerfully increases hedonic feeding including the consumption of highly palatable, high fat food’

9. Conclusions: Typo in the first sentence - The results of ‘his’ study should be ‘this’ study..

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 8 (5)
eNeuro
Vol. 8, Issue 5
September/October 2021
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
μ-Opioid Receptor Stimulation in the Nucleus Accumbens Increases Vocal–Social Interactions in Flocking European Starlings, Sturnus Vulgaris
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
μ-Opioid Receptor Stimulation in the Nucleus Accumbens Increases Vocal–Social Interactions in Flocking European Starlings, Sturnus Vulgaris
Alyse N. Maksimoski, Brandon J. Polzin, Sharon A. Stevenson, Changjiu Zhao, Lauren V. Riters
eNeuro 2 September 2021, 8 (5) ENEURO.0219-21.2021; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0219-21.2021

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
μ-Opioid Receptor Stimulation in the Nucleus Accumbens Increases Vocal–Social Interactions in Flocking European Starlings, Sturnus Vulgaris
Alyse N. Maksimoski, Brandon J. Polzin, Sharon A. Stevenson, Changjiu Zhao, Lauren V. Riters
eNeuro 2 September 2021, 8 (5) ENEURO.0219-21.2021; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0219-21.2021
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Visual Abstract
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • flocking
  • mesolimbic reward pathway
  • nucleus accumbens
  • opioids
  • social behavior
  • songbirds

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Release of extracellular matrix components after human traumatic brain injury
  • Action intentions reactivate representations of task-relevant cognitive cues
  • Functional connectome correlates of laterality preferences: Insights into Hand, Foot, and Eye Dominance Across the Lifespan
Show more Research Article: New Research

Cognition and Behavior

  • Transformed visual working memory representations in human occipitotemporal and posterior parietal cortices
  • Neural Speech-Tracking During Selective Attention: A Spatially Realistic Audiovisual Study
  • Nucleus Accumbens Dopamine Encodes the Trace Period during Appetitive Pavlovian Conditioning
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.