Abstract
Cells in the entorhinal cortex (EC) contain rich spatial information and project strongly to the hippocampus where a cognitive map is supposedly created. These cells range from cells with structured spatial selectivity, such as grid cells in the medial EC (MEC) that are selective to an array of spatial locations that form a hexagonal grid, to weakly spatial cells, such as non-grid cells in the MEC and lateral EC (LEC) that contain spatial information but have no structured spatial selectivity. However, in a small environment, place cells in the hippocampus are generally selective to a single location of the environment, while granule cells in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus have multiple discrete firing locations but lack spatial periodicity. Given the anatomic connection from the EC to the hippocampus, how the hippocampus retrieves information from upstream EC remains unclear. Here, we propose a unified learning model that can describe the spatial tuning properties of both hippocampal place cells and dentate gyrus granule cells based on non-negative sparse coding from EC inputs. Sparse coding plays an important role in many cortical areas and is proposed here to have a key role in the hippocampus. Our results show that the hexagonal patterns of MEC grid cells with various orientations, grid spacings and phases are necessary for the model to learn different place cells that efficiently tile the entire spatial environment. However, if there is a lack of diversity in any grid parameters or a lack of hippocampal cells in the network, this will lead to the emergence of hippocampal cells that have multiple firing locations. More surprisingly, the model can also learn hippocampal place cells even when weakly spatial cells, instead of grid cells, are used as the input to the hippocampus. This work suggests that sparse coding may be one of the underlying organizing principles for the navigational system of the brain.
Significance Statement
The brain can perform extremely complex spatial navigation tasks, but how it does this remains unclear. Here, we show that the principle of sparse coding can be used to learn the hippocampal place map in a way that efficiently tiles the entire spatial environment using inputs from the entorhinal cortex (EC), namely either grid cells or weakly spatial cells. This demonstrates that the hippocampus can retrieve spatial information from the EC using an efficient representation and that sparse coding may be one of the underlying principles of the navigational system of the brain.
Introduction
Since the Nobel-prize-winning discovery of place cells in the hippocampus (O’Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971; O’Keefe, 1976) and grid cells in the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC; Hafting et al., 2005; Rowland et al., 2016), brain regions involved in spatial awareness and navigation have attracted much attention from both experimental and computational neuroscientists.
In the hippocampus, place cells generally have a single specific firing location in a small environment (O’Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971; Park et al., 2011), and neighboring cells have firing fields at different locations, such that the local cell population in the hippocampus can represent the whole spatial environment (O’Keefe, 1976). In contrast, granule cells in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampal formation have multiple discrete firing locations without spatial periodicity (Jung and McNaughton, 1993; Leutgeb et al., 2007).
However, in the EC, cells that carry spatial information range from cells with structured selectivity to weakly spatial cells that have spatial information but no structured selectivity. For example, MEC grid cells have hexagonal firing fields that cover the entire environment (Hafting et al., 2005). The hexagonal grid is characterized by three grid parameters: spacing, orientation, and phase (see Fig. 1A–C). Grid cells are organized into discrete modules according to their spacings because the progression in grid spacing along the dorsal-ventral axis is geometric, with ratio around 1.42 (Stensola et al., 2012). Also, grid cells in each module have similar orientations but random phases (Stensola et al., 2012). Nevertheless, there are also other cells such as weakly-tuned cells (Zhang et al., 2013) in the MEC and Diehl et al. (2017) found that, apart from the well-characterized spatial cells in the MEC, nearly all of the remaining 2/3 MEC cells have spatial selectivity. In addition, cells in the lateral EC (LEC) also display weak spatial selectivity (Hargreaves et al., 2005; Yoganarasimha et al., 2011).
Followed by the experimental studies that indicate that EC inputs, including both grid cells and weakly spatial cells, are the principal cortical input to the hippocampus (Steward and Scoville, 1976; Tamamaki and Nojyo, 1993; Leutgeb et al., 2007; Van Strien et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2013), various models have been proposed to explain the emergence of place cells based on the feedforward connection from grid cells, from mathematical models that have no learning (Solstad et al., 2006; de Almeida et al., 2009) to models with plasticity (Rolls et al., 2006; Franzius et al., 2007a,b; Savelli and Knierim, 2010; Neher et al., 2017).
Among these learning models, Rolls et al. (2006) used a competitive learning procedure. However, only ∼10% of modelled hippocampal cells had a single firing location. Furthermore, the competition was introduced by manually setting the population activation to a small specified value. Similarly, Franzius et al. (2007a,b) applied independent component analysis (Hyvarinen, 1999) to learn place cells. However, the examples of learned place cells are mostly located at the border (Franzius et al., 2007a, their Fig 3C; Franzius et al., 2007b, their Fig. 1G). Additionally, the weights connecting grid and place cells can be positive or negative, and the place cell responses were manually shifted by a constant term to be kept non-negative, which puts into question the biological realization of the model. Furthermore, previous models do not investigate how well the learned hippocampal place map represents the entire spatial environment and how the weakly spatial cells in the EC can contribute to the formation of the place map.
Sparse coding (Olshausen and Field, 1996) provides a compelling explanation of many experimental findings of brain network structures. One particular variant, non-negative sparse coding (Hoyer, 2003), has recently been shown to account for a wide range of neuronal responses in brain areas (for review, see Beyeler et al., 2019). However, whether sparse coding can learn the hippocampal place map has not previously been investigated.
Here, we apply sparse coding with non-negative constraint, where neuronal responses and connection weights are restricted to be non-negative, to building a learning model of hippocampal cells using EC inputs. Our results show that, when grid cells are used as the entorhinal input, single-location hippocampal place cells that tile the entire environment can be learned, given the sufficient diversity in grid parameters. However, if there is a lack of diversity in any grid parameters, the learning of the hippocampal place cells is impeded; instead, more hippocampal cells with multiple firing locations are learned. Furthermore, a lower number of hippocampal cells in the network also results in learning hippocampal cells with multiple firing locations. Additionally, the competition generated by sparse coding naturally provides a global inhibition such that learned hippocampal place cells display single firing fields, suggesting that the proposed model can be implemented by biologically based neural mechanisms and circuits. Also, the learned hippocampal place cells tile the entire spatial environment efficiently by a hexagonal lattice, which is consistent with a recent study of conceptual state spaces (Mok and Love, 2019). Moreover, the model can still learn hippocampal place cells even when the grid inputs are replaced by the responses of weakly spatial cells, suggesting that sparse coding can retrieve information from EC inputs efficiently to form a cognitive map as long as there is sufficient spatial information in the upstream input. This also provides a plausible explanation of many experimental studies that suggest the emergence and maintenance of place cells does not require grid cells (Langston et al., 2010; Wills et al., 2010; Koenig et al., 2011; Brandon et al., 2014; Hales et al., 2014; Schlesiger et al., 2015).
Materials and Methods
The environment
The 2D spatial environment used in this study is a 1 × 1 m square box. A 32 × 32 grid with 1024 points is used to represent the entire environment. Therefore, a 1024 × 1 vector, denoted by r, with only one non-zero element of value 1 can be used to represent the spatial location of a virtual rat.
Model of entorhinal input
Since the environment is represented by a 32 × 32 grid, a 1024 × 1 vector, denoted by
Grid cells described by a mathematical model
The hexagonal firing fields of grid cells can be represented by the sum of three sinusoidal gratings (Solstad et al., 2006; Kropff and Treves, 2008; de Almeida et al., 2009), as described by
Since grid cells have different spacings, orientations and phases, Equation 1 is used to generate diverse grid cells. The value of the grid spacing, λ, ranges in value from 28 cm (Hafting et al., 2005; Solstad et al., 2006) and increases by a geometric ratio 1.42 that is consistent with experimental results (Stensola et al., 2012) and the optimal grid scale derived by a mathematical study (Wei et al., 2015). For example, if there are Nλ different grid spacings, the spacings will be 28 cm, 28 × 1.42 = 39.76 cm,
⋯, and
Some examples of grid cells described by Equation 1 are shown in Figure 1D. These grid cells have diverse grid spacings, orientations and phases.
The number of different grid parameters (Nλ, Nθ, Nx, and Ny) of grid cells defined above are assigned to different values to investigate the effect of the diversity of grid cells on the formation of place cells.
Although the firing fields of grid cells can be represented by the sum of three sinusoidal gratings (Eq. 1) given grid spacings, orientations, and phases and are used in many studies (Solstad et al., 2006; Kropff and Treves, 2008; de Almeida et al., 2009), this mathematical model cannot capture the variability in individual grid fields of the grid cells as reported by Ismakov et al. (2017). Therefore, in this article, we also adopt a more realistic model of grid cells that characterizes grid fields individually and embraces the variability between individual grid fields. This model of grid cells is used to investigate the robustness of our results obtained by using the mathematical model of grid cells (Eq. 1).
Grid cells described by a more realistic model
MEC grid cells have different spacings, orientations and phases, and are separated into discrete modules where grid cells are anatomically adjacent and have similar grid spacings, which is supported by experimental evidence (Hafting et al., 2005; Stensola et al., 2012). Moreover, grid cells in the same spacing module tend to have similar orientations and random phases (Stensola et al., 2012). Because of these properties of grid cells, we model grid cells using a similar approach in a previous study (Neher et al., 2017) as described below.
Values of the grid spacing in each module are randomly sampled from normal distributions: there are four discrete modules for grid spacings (λ) with mean of 38.8, 48.4, 65, and 98.4 cm, and a same SD of 8 cm. For grid orientation (θ), since grid cells in the same spacing module tend to have similar orientations, grid cells in the four discrete modules also have mean orientations 15°, 30°, 45°, and 0°, and a common SD of 3°. Because grid phases, (x0, y0), are random in each module, grid phase is randomly sampled from a uniform distribution between 0 and λ. Stensola et al. (2012) showed that 87% of grid cells belong to the two modules with small spacings based on their recordings. Therefore, we have 43.5%, 43.5%, 6.5%, and 6.5% of grid cells in the modules with mean spacings 38.8, 48.4, 65, and 98.4 cm, respectively (unless otherwise noted).
The firing field of each grid cell is modelled as the sum of multiple grid fields whose centers are located at the vertices of the hexagonal grid. The grid field at vertex (xv, yv) is described by a function with the following form (Neher et al., 2017)
After the mathematical model of grid cells are used as the input, the more realistic model of grid cells is then used to verify the results and investigate the robustness of the model.
Weakly spatial cells
Apart from grid cells in the MEC, other cells such as weakly-tuned cells (Zhang et al., 2013) and non-grid cells (Diehl et al., 2017) in the MEC, and LEC cells that contains spatial information (Hargreaves et al., 2005; Yoganarasimha et al., 2011), can also contribute to the formation of hippocampal place cells. In this article, we model these cells as weakly spatial cells. The firing field of weakly spatial cells is generated in the simulation by first assigning a random activation, sampled from a uniform distribution between 0 and 1, to each location, then smoothing the map with a Gaussian kernel with SD 6 cm, and normalizing the map such that the values are between 0 and 1 (Neher et al., 2017).
Weakly spatial cells are used as another type of entorhinal input to investigate how they contribute to the formation of the hippocampal place map.
Sparse coding with non-negative constraint
Sparse coding was originally proposed by Olshausen and Field (1996) to demonstrate that simple cells in the primary visual cortex represent their sensory input using an efficient neuronal representation, namely that their firing rates in response to natural images tend to be sparse (rarely attain large values) and statistically independent. In addition, sparse coding finds a reconstruction of the sensory input through a linear representation of features with minimal error, which can be understood as minimizing the following cost function
One common choice of
Non-negative sparse coding is simply sparse coding with non-negative constraint, i.e., the connection weights A and model responses s are restricted to non-negative values in the cost function Equation 4. Note that, when β in Equation 4 is set to zero, the cost function of non-negative sparse coding reduces to the cost function of non-negative matrix factorization (Lee and Seung, 1999).
Structure of the model
In this study, a two-layer network is proposed to model the activities of entorhinal cells (first layer) and hippocampal cells (second layer), respectively. Given a spatial location in the environment, modelled entorhinal cells respond according to their firing fields. Modelled entorhinal cell responses then feed into modelled hippocampal cells and the entorhinal-hippocampal network implements a sparse coding model with non-negative constraint. The model structure is shown in Figure 2.
Denote E as a
The code to run the model is available at https://github.com/lianyunke/Learning-Efficient-Hippocampal-Place-Map-from-Entorhinal-Inputs-Using-Nonnegative-Sparse-Coding.
Learning rule
The learning rule for updating the connection strength matrix A is similar to that in previous studies of sparse coding (Olshausen and Field, 1997; Zhu and Rozell, 2013), as given by
The model dynamics and learning rule described in Equations 8, 9 can be implemented in a biologically realistic network (Lian et al., 2019). Here, we simply use the equations described above to demonstrate that the principle of non-negative sparse coding can learn hippocampal cells with both single and multiple firing locations.
Training
For modelled entorhinal cells, when using grid cells described by a mathematical model (Eq. 1 in Materials and Methods) as the entorhinal input, the smallest grid spacing and grid ratio are taken to be 28 cm and 1.42, respectively. Since the environment used in this study is 1 × 1 m, the maximal grid spacing is taken to be smaller than 1 m, which leads to
There are 100 modelled hippocampal cells at the second layer in our simulations (unless otherwise noted), i.e.,
Recovering the firing fields of modelled hippocampal cells
Because the response of modelled hippocampal cells is computed by the dynamic equations in Equation 8,
K = 105 is used in this article.
Fitting firing fields to functions
In order to obtain the center and size of the firing field of the modelled hippocampal cell, F is fitted by a function Q(x, y) of the form
Selecting place cells
Some firing fields of modelled hippocampal cells have multiple firing locations and noise in the background, while others have a single location of firing. After fitting firing fields into the function described in Equation 11, a modelled hippocampal cell is categorized as a place cell if the following two criteria are satisfied for the firing field: (1) the fitting error is smaller than 15%; (2) the breadth, σ, is larger than 5 cm. These two rules exclude any modelled hippocampal cells with no obvious firing field or with multiple-location firing field. The firing field of a place cell is called place field.
Measuring the uniformity of place cell representation
For place cells that meets the criteria defined above, the field center (xc, yc) fitted by Equation 11 indicates the spatial location that the place cell responds to. We measured how well all place cells represent the entire environment using two measures.
The first measure is distance to place field, dPF, which indicates the Euclidean distance between each spatial location (px, py) in the environment and the nearest place field, described as
If the distance to a place field is large for a location, it means that there are no place fields near this location. Therefore, the distribution of this measure can tell us how well place fields of all place cells tile the entire spatial environment. When all spatial locations have small values of dPF, the entire environment is tiled by the place cells.
The second measure is nearest distance, dND. We define dND of place cell j as the maximal Euclidean distance of two nearest centers, described as
The distance to place field,
Results
When diverse grid cells are used as the entorhinal input, the model can learn place cells that tile the entire environment efficiently
The results shown in this section use grid cells described by the mathematical model as the entorhinal input. Our simulation shows that the non-negative sparse coding model proposed here can learn single-location place cells given diverse grid cells as the input. Grid cells with four different spacings (Nλ = 4), six different orientations (Nθ = 6), and five different phases in both x- and y-axes (
After learning, all 100 modelled hippocampal cells meet the criteria of a place cell (as defined in Materials and Methods). The firing fields, F (defined by Eq. 10), for 100 modelled hippocampal cells are shown in Figure 3A. The firing fields of modelled hippocampal cells are ordered by their spatial locations. All place cells have a single-location firing field. Furthermore, different place cells have spatially different firing fields.
The learned place cells tile the environment efficiently
The centers of all the place cells are displayed together in the 1 × 1 m spatial environment represented by a 32 × 32 pixel-like image (Fig. 3B), which shows that the centers of the 100 place cells tile the entire environment without any overlap. In addition, the box plot in Figure 3C shows that any location within the space is within a distance of no more than 8.2 cm from the nearest place fields. The histogram of nearest distance of all 100 place cells is displayed in Figure 3D, which shows that the distribution is centered around a mean value of 10.70 cm and SD 0.75 cm. Given that the learned place cells have mean radius 8.92 cm with SD 0.49 cm, Figure 3B–D illustrates that the learned place cells tile the whole environment rather evenly, i.e., the model learned by non-negative sparse coding can give an accurate neural representation of the spatial location in the environment.
Moreover, centers of all learned place cells (Fig. 3B) are positioned into a hexagonal pattern, which can be explained by the principle of the model. Because our model is based on sparse coding that finds an efficient representation of the input, the fact that all learned place cells altogether form a hexagonal pattern is efficient because the triangular lattice of the hexagonal pattern is known to be the optimal solution to the circle packing problem (Thue, 1892). The recurrent connection W, described in Equation 8 might provide the inhibition needed for generating an efficient population code of the space. This result is also consistent with a cluster learning method of concept learning in both spatial and conceptual domains (Mok and Love, 2019), suggesting that sparse coding might be an underlying principle of processing conceptual information as well.
The competition introduced by sparse coding provides the inhibition for place cells
The connectivity profile between 600 grid cells and 100 place cells is plotted in Figure 4A, which shows that each place cell selects a group of particular grid cells with different weights. As a result, the overall feedforward connection from the spatial environment to the place cells, namely the matrix product EA, has the spatial structure plotted in Figure 4B, which shows that each place cell is selective to one spatial location similar to the firing fields (Fig. 3A). However, EA has strong average offsets, which can be seen from the gray background in Figure 4B. The model of place cells proposed by Solstad et al. (2006) has an inhibition term to balance the excitation so that the place fields are responsive to a single location. As for the model of place cells proposed by Franzius et al. (2007b), an offset constant is added and signs of model units are adjusted to achieve single location place fields. Nevertheless, comparing Figures 3A and 4B, we can conclude that the network implemented by sparse coding naturally introduces the competition to inhibit place cells such that they have firing fields similar to those found in experiments. As stated earlier in Materials and Methods, the sparse coding model used in this article can be implemented by a biologically realistic network (Lian et al., 2019), suggesting that principle used here can be a potential mechanism used in the navigational system of the brain. The results presented in this study are not sensitive to different parameter values as long as there are diversities in spacing, orientation, and phase. Even 81 grid cells (Nλ = 3, Nθ = 3, and
In addition, the principle of sparse coding forces the model to learn an efficient representation of the entorhinal input. The average percentage of active modelled hippocampal cells in response to a spatial location is 5.59%. The sparse population activity is consistent with the experimental study that shows sparse ensemble activities in the macaque hippocampus (Skaggs et al., 2007).
The model can learn cells with multiple firing locations
In this section, we still use grid cells described by the mathematical model as the entorhinal input and show that the lack of diversity in any grid parameters or the lack of modelled hippocampal cells in the network will prevent the model from learning place cells and cells with multiple firing locations start to emerge, i.e., the same model can learn cells similar to dentate gyrus cells that have multiple firing locations.
When grid cells are less diverse
A lack of diversity in grid spacing results in the emergence of multiple firing locations of the modelled hippocampal cells, as illustrated in Figure 5A compared with Figure 3A. Similarly, compared with Figure 3B,C, the lack of diversity in grid orientation or grid phase will also cause the model to learn more cells with multiple firing locations (Fig. 5B,C). These modelled hippocampal cells are similar to dentate gyrus cells that are found to have multiple firing locations in experimental studies (Jung and McNaughton, 1993; Leutgeb et al., 2007).
Recall that the principle of sparse coding finds a linear representation of the input, namely the grid cell responses. Our results suggest that grid cells with less diversity in grid parameters are not sufficient to well represent the whole environment, so that the system gives an ambiguous representation of the spatial location. Therefore, the diverse grid cells found in the MEC are crucial to the emergence of hippocampal place cells if grid cells are the only entorhinal input to the hippocampus. The lack of diversity in afferent grid cells may be one possible factor explaining how cells with multiple firing locations emerge in the dentate gyrus.
When there are fewer modelled hippocampal cells
Simulations also show that a smaller number of modelled hippocampal cells, Nh, can also cause the model to learn cells with multiple firing locations, although the grid cells are diverse. This is illustrated in Figure 6, which shows the firing fields of modelled hippocampal cells when there are different numbers of modelled hippocampal cells in the network. The values of the remaining parameters are exactly the same as ones used in Figure 5, which shows well-learned place cells, except the number of modelled hippocampal cells, Nh. Figure 6 demonstrates that as the number of modelled hippocampal cells, Nh, decreases, cells with more firing-locations start to emerge. The less cells, the larger the proportion of cells with multiple firing locations that emerge. When Nh = 10 all modelled hippocampal cells have more than one firing location. When Nh = 20 there are five cells that are categorized as place cells. When Nh is larger than 30, almost all cells are found to have single-location firing fields.
Consequently, although a network with diverse grid cells can represent the spatial environment well, having less modelled hippocampal cells does not result in a unique representation of the spatial location with single-location place cells and the animal’s location might need to be encoded using more than one cell. This suggests more generally that hippocampal cells with multiple firing locations may be generated by having a small number of modelled hippocampal cells in the population that implements sparse coding.
The spatial resolution of the hippocampal place map increases as more place cells are used to represent the environment
As discussed above, when Nh is larger than 30, almost all modelled hippocampal cells have a single-location firing field. In addition, the learned place cells tile the whole environment rather well with small values of the nearest distance (Eq. 13). Furthermore, as Nh increases, the mean nearest distance and field breadth of the place field decreases (Fig. 7) with relatively small variations, indicating that the spatial resolution of the neural representation by place cells improves.
Model results are robust to realistic grid fields
When 600 more realistic grid cells sampled from four discrete grid modules (Materials and Methods) are used as the entorhinal input that incorporate the observed biological variability, the model can still learn a robust representation of the spatial location of the entire environment, as shown in Figure 8.
Figure 8A shows that when grid fields are diverse in grid spacing, orientation, and phase, each modelled hippocampal cell learns a single-location firing field such that centers of all place fields tile the entire spatial environment rather evenly. Furthermore, these place cells are positioned into a hexagonal pattern that packs the environment efficiently. The box plot of distance to place field shows fairly small values and indicates the whole environment is covered well. The distribution of nearest distance has mean 10.76 cm and SD 0.62 cm, qualitatively consistent with results shown in Figure 3C (mean 10.70 cm and SD 0.75 cm). Therefore, the learned place cells evenly tile the entire environment. Additionally, 600 grid cells sampled only from the two smallest grid modules are sufficient for place cells to emerge, as illustrated in Figure 9.
Figure 8B shows that realistic grid fields with less diversity will cause the model to learn hippocampal cells with multiple firing locations. The left plot displays the firing fields of 100 modelled hippocampal cells with less diversity in grid spacing. The SD of spacing in four grid modules is set to 0 cm instead of 8 cm while the SD of orientation is still 3° and the phase is random. The middle plot shows firing fields when there is less diversity in grid orientation, where in each grid module the SD of spacing is 4 cm, the SD of orientation is 0° and phase is random. The right plot is for the case of less diversity in grid phase, where in each grid module the SD of spacing is 8 cm, the SD of orientation is 3° and phase is (0, 0) for all grid cells. Although the model learns hippocampal cells with multiple locations when there is less diversity in grid parameters, the learned place cells still tile the entire environment.
Figure 8C shows that having fewer modelled hippocampal cells causes the model to learn cells with multiple firing locations. The six plots separated by dashed lines in Figure 8C represent the firing fields of modelled hippocampal cells when the number of modelled hippocampal cells, Nh, is 10, 20, 30, 40, 50, and 60, respectively. When
Similar to Figure 7, the neural representation of the spatial environment has better resolution (smaller radius and smaller nearest distance) as Nh increases, as seen from Figure 8D.
The model can generate large place fields using grid cells
As discussed in a previous study (Neher et al., 2017), most existing models of place cells cannot produce large place fields, such as CA3 place cells with size around 1225 cm2. The model proposed here can generate large place fields by simply having grid cells with large grid spacings as the input to modelled hippocampal cells.
In this part of the study, more realistic grid cells are used as the entorhinal input to the hippocampus. In addition, only grid cells with grid spacings in the fourth module are used, i.e., the grid spacing is sampled from the normal distribution with mean 98.4 cm and SD 8 cm, grid orientation is sampled from the distribution with mean 0° and SD 3°, and grid phases are randomly chosen from a uniform distribution. Similarly, 600 grid cells are used. The number of modelled hippocampal cells, Nh, is set to 20.
After learning, each modelled hippocampal cell pools a particular group of grid cells, similar to Figure 4A, 18 out of 20 modelled hippocampal cells satisfy the criteria of place cells defined Materials and Methods. Figure 10A shows the 18 learned place cells. Figure 10C shows that these place cells have radius from 18.71 to 21.22 cm (mean 19.68 cm and SD 0.75 cm). Therefore, the size of place fields ranges from 1099.76 to 1414.62 cm2. Figure 10B,D,E shows that these 18 place cells with large size cover the entire environment rather evenly in a hexagonal pattern.
Above all, the model can learn large place cells if the afferent grid cells have large grid spacings, consistent with experimental evidence that the sizes of grid cells and place cells increase along the dorsal-ventral axis (Fyhn et al., 2007; Kjelstrup et al., 2008) and with topographic entorhinal-hippocampal projections along the dorsal-ventral axis (Dolorfo and Amaral, 1998).
Weakly spatial cells in the EC are sufficient for hippocampal place cells to emerge
Recent experimental evidence shows that the emergence of hippocampal place cells happens earlier in development than grid cells (Langston et al., 2010; Wills et al., 2010). In addition, other experimental studies suggest that hippocampal place cells can still maintain their place fields even after grid cells are inactivated (Koenig et al., 2011; Brandon et al., 2014; Hales et al., 2014; Schlesiger et al., 2015). Although weakly spatial cells are more abundant than grid cells in the EC, how they contribute to the place fields of hippocampal place cells is still unclear. Here, we show that even weakly spatial cells can provide sufficient spatial information for the emergence of place cells that have an accurate representation of spatial locations. This suggests that place cells can emerge throughout the development of MEC grid cells, from the initial weakly-tuned spatial pattern to the fully developed hexagonal grid pattern.
In this section, 600 weakly spatial cells are used as the entorhinal input to the hippocampus and there are 100 modelled hippocampal cells. We use a smaller learning rate (η = 0.01) and more epochs (30,000) for the learning here. The firing fields of ten example weakly spatial cells are shown in Figure 11A.
An efficient hippocampal place map emerges
The firing fields of weakly spatial cells are very different from the periodic pattern of grid cells. Surprisingly, they can nevertheless provide sufficient spatial information such that the model based on sparse coding can decode entorhinal cell responses and give an accurate representation of the spatial location. Figure 11B shows the firing fields of learned place cells. Figure 11C–E shows that the centers of place cells evenly tile the entire spatial environment with an efficient hexagonal pattern.
Compared with Figures 3, 8A, using weakly spatial cells instead of grid cells results in learning a hippocampal place-map with less resolution. The mean radius of place fields using weakly spatial cells as the entorhinal input (mean 11.45 cm with SD 2.14 cm; Fig. 11B) is larger than Figure 3 (8.92 cm) and Figure 8A (8.75 cm). Furthermore, the nearest distance in Figure 11E (mean 11.50 cm and SD 0.94 cm) is also larger, compared with Figure 3 (mean 10.70 cm and SD 0.75 cm) and Figure 8A (mean 10.76 cm and SD 0.62 cm) when grid cells are used. The larger SD in Figure 11E suggests that the irregular fields of weakly spatial cells lead to the less even tiling of place cells. However, the learned place map still covers the entire environment well with small distance to place field (Fig. 11D) and efficiently with a hexagonal lattice (Fig. 11C).
The learned place map is robust to noise even when weakly spatial cells are used
Furthermore, the model is quite robust to noise and an efficient place map can still be learned, although a relatively strong noise is added to the modelled entorhinal cell responses in Equation 8:
Although there are places in the environment not covered by place cells, generally the modelled hippocampal cells learn place fields that efficiently tile the entire environment.
Above all, the model is consistent with the experimental evidence that place cells emerge earlier than grid cells during development and a possible explanation is that the neural system can learn a hippocampal map even when the hexagonal spatial field is not well developed, and place cells can maintain their place fields when grid cells are inactivated because weakly spatial cells in the EC can lead to the emergence of a hippocampal place map.
Sparse coding can learn hippocampal place cells although the input cells from the EC are weakly tuned to the spatial environment. Thus, input cells with stronger spatial selectivity can provide more spatial information so that unique place field can be decoded by sparse coding. Barry and Burgess (2007) used a learning model to learn place cells from responses of boundary vector cells that are selective to boundaries of the environment at particular angles and distances. Their result can be regarded as a special case of the results presented in this article, where boundary vector cells are simply entorhinal input cells with stronger tuning of the spatial environment.
These weakly spatial cells can arise from any form of sensory inputs, such as visual input and auditory input, that encode spatial information. For example, the visual input at different locations of the environment actually carries information about spatial locations and consequently the afferent visual information to the EC can lead to weakly spatial cells. Moreover, the principle of sparse coding can cause EC cells to generate a hippocampal place map. The conjecture proposed here can explain a recent experimental study that shows that place cell firings mainly reflect visual inputs (Chen et al., 2019) and another experimental study that suggests homing abilities of mice even in darkness may not need accurate grid cell firing (Chen et al., 2016).
The effect of environmental geometry on hippocampal cells may come from distorted entorhinal input
O’Keefe and Burgess (1996) found that place fields will be stretched following the environmental distortions. Later, Barry et al. (2007) found the same phenomenon for gird cells, namely that grid cells will be stretched along the distorted axis. Given the anatomic connection from the EC to hippocampus, it is natural to ask whether stretched place fields caused by the environment manipulation originate from stretched gird cells caused by the environment geometry. Our model shows that when grid fields are stretched along one axis of the environment, place fields will be stretched along that direction as well.
After the model is learned using 600 realistic grid cells as the entorhinal input to the hippocampus (Fig. 8A), the entorhinal-hippocampal connection (A) is kept fixed. Then the spatial environment is changed to 2 × 1 m, i.e., the environment is re-scaled along x-axis. In addition, gird fields are also stretched by a factor of 2 along x-axis. Next, we recover the firing fields of modelled hippocampal cells (described in Materials and Methods) with fixed connection and stretched grid cells. Figure 13 shows that modelled hippocampal cells are still selective to one location of the environment but their firing fields are stretched compared with the original place fields (Fig. 8A, left plot).
As grid cells are anchored to external landmarks (Hafting et al., 2005), grid fields are re-scaled by environmental changes (Barry et al., 2007). Our model suggests that the effect of environmental geometry on place cells may come from grid cells distorted by the environment manipulation. However, this does not rule out the possibility that the spatial selectivity of other entorhinal input to place cells is also altered by the environment, which causes the changes of place fields. If there is experimental data about the effect of environment geometry on other EC neurons that project to the hippocampus in the future, our model can be used to further explain what causes the change of hippocampal place fields.
Navigation trajectory versus random location
As described in Materials and Methods, the model is trained using random spatial locations uniformly sampled from the entire environment, so the spatial input to the model can uniformly cover the entire environment very quickly after some iterations. However, for a real navigation trajectory of a rat running freely in an environment, the route is continuous and there are spatial locations the rat has never been to. In this section, we used the same model as the one displayed in Figure 8A that has 600 realistic modelled grid cells as the entorhinal input to the model and 100 modelled hippocampal cells. Instead, the random spatial input to the model is replaced by a simulated running trajectory of a virtual rat. The trajectory is generated by a smoothed random walk using the method from D’Albis and Kempter (2017) with mean speed 0.25 m/s and 20 Hz of sampling positions. A simulated running trajectory of 3600 s is used to train the model. After learning, another simulated running trajectory of 1200 s is used to recover the firing fields of modelled hippocampal cells using the same method described in Equation 10.
Figure 14 shows the firing fields of 100 modelled hippocampal cells using the same realistic grid cells as Figure 8A but simulated running trajectory for training. Compared with Figure 8, firing fields trained and recovered by running trajectories are less circular because of non-uniform spatial locations used to train and recover the firing fields. Though the running trajectory of 3600 s contains 72,000 spatial locations (much >20,000 spatial locations that are uniformly sampled to train the model in Fig. 8), not all modelled hippocampal cells learn a place field. Positions along the running trajectory are continuous and are not sampled according to a uniform distribution, so the model has more data at some positions than others, causing the model to learn less place cells with less circular firing fields.
Discussion
Summary
In this article, we applied sparse coding with non-negative constraint to a hierarchical model of entorhinal-hippocampal network. Our results show that sparse coding can learn an efficient hippocampal place map that represents the entire environment when grid cells are diverse in grid spacing, orientation, and phase. However, lack of diversity in grid cells or fewer modelled hippocampal cells leads to the emergence of cells with multiple firing locations, like those cells found in the dentate gyrus. In addition, weakly spatial cells in the EC are sufficient for sparse coding to learn hippocampal place cells.
Comparison with other learning models
Our work differs significantly from previous studies on learning place cells from grid cell input (Rolls et al., 2006; Franzius et al., 2007b; Monaco and Abbott, 2011; Neher et al., 2017). Most importantly, our model investigates different types of entorhinal input: grid cells and weakly spatial cells. Also, among these learning models, we systematically investigate the influence of the diversity in grid cells using knowledge of grid modules (Stensola et al., 2012) on the formation for hippocampal cells. Moreover, we demonstrate that learned hippocampal place cells can represent the entire spatial environment efficiently using a hexagonal lattice, consistent with a recent work in the conceptual state spaces (Mok and Love, 2019). Furthermore, the same model can produce cells with one firing location, multiple firing locations and large place field size, which can account for the emergence of a range of different observed hippocampal cell types. In addition, we demonstrate that weakly spatial cells in the EC can also provide sufficient spatial information for the emergence of the hippocampal place map after learning and the model is very robust to noise. Most importantly, all the results presented in this article are generated by the same model, namely sparse coding with non-negative constraint.
Although the principle of independent component analysis used by Franzius et al. (2007a,b) to learn place cells from grid cells is similar to the principle of sparse coding used here, the place cell examples in their paper are mostly near the boundary of the environment. However, our model can learn place cells at different places of the entire environment, generate a hippocampal place map that tiles the entire environment efficiently, and automatically provide the needed inhibition by the dynamics of sparse coding. One possible reason is that place cells in a population are not necessarily independent because nearby cells do overlap, so independent component analysis might put a too strong assumption. Also, the non-negativity introduced in this article makes the model more similar to the real neural system, which might help the model uncover important biological properties.
Properties of grid cells that are necessary for the emergence of place cells
Our model shows that the principle of sparse coding can learn an efficient place map using input from the EC, grid cells or weakly spatial cells. Although the model can learn place cells when only weakly spatial cells are used, it does not imply that grid cells are not necessary for the formation of place cells. Fiete et al. (2008) proposed that grid cells with different spacings and phases altogether form a residual system that efficiently encodes the spatial location. In addition, the triangular lattice of the grid pattern is known to be the solution to the optimal circle packing problem (Thue, 1892) and the geometric scale of grid spacings can represent the spatial environment efficiently (Wei et al., 2015). Our results are consistent with the concept that grid cells provide efficient information that can be retrieved to form a hippocampal place map. In our results, when 600 more realistic grid cells are used as the entorhinal input to hippocampal place cells, the ratio between the average of square of grid cell responses and the average of square of place cell responses is 0.31. However, this ratio becomes 0.84 when 600 weakly spatial cells are used as the entorhinal input, suggesting that grid cells are much more efficient for providing spatial information to the hippocampus. Another important property of grid cells that might be necessary for the emergence of place cells is the temporal property-phase precession. The experimental study conducted by Schlesiger et al. (2015) suggests that the MEC input to the hippocampus is necessary for the temporal property of hippocampal place cells. Therefore, although weakly spatial cells can provide sufficient spatial information to learn an efficient place map, grid cells that have phase precession might be required to account for the phase precession of hippocampal place cells. This is left for the future work.
Non-grid cells in the EC
Although grid cells in the MEC have structured firing fields that lie on a hexagonal lattice, they only account for <1/3 of MEC cells and nearly 2/3 of MEC cells have spatial selectivity without well-characterized firing fields (Diehl et al., 2017). In the LEC, cells also display weak spatial specificity and convey nonspatial information (Hargreaves et al., 2005; Yoganarasimha et al., 2011). Our model explains how these upstream non-grid cells that contain weak spatial information can be used to form a hippocampal place map, which is consistent with increasingly more experimental evidence that suggests grid cells are not required to form the spatial firing field of place cells (Langston et al., 2010; Wills et al., 2010; Koenig et al., 2011; Brandon et al., 2014; Hales et al., 2014; Schlesiger et al., 2015).
Also, more recent studies have been investigating the effect of sensory input such as visual input on the firing of place cells. Chen and colleagues find that visual input largely determines place cells firing (Chen et al., 2013, 2016, 2019). In addition, a recent study shows that some neurons in V1 display location-specific firing (Haggerty and Ji, 2015). Therefore, we conjecture that neurons that respond to external stimuli in a spatial environment must contain spatial selectivity in any form and all these neurons contribute to the formation of a cognitive map. EC, the gateway between the hippocampus and the neocortex of the brain, provides abundant information for the hippocampus and plays a crucial role here. Our results show that the principle of sparse coding can be one underlying principle that efficiently retrieves spatial information from the upstream. Furthermore, the efficient hexagonal pattern of the learned place map (Figs. 3B, 8A, 10B, 11C) is consistent with the hexagonal clusters in a recent work in conceptual state spaces (Mok and Love, 2019), suggesting that the principle of sparse coding can be used to explain more functions of the hippocampus.
Non-negativity
In recent computational studies of the navigational system of the brain, the concept of non-negativity is used several times. Dordek et al. (2016) applied non-negative principal component analysis to extract grid cells from place cell responses and the introduction of non-negativity pushes the model to converge to a hexagonal lattice from a square lattice when there is no non-negativity. D’Albis and Kempter (2017) build a single-cell spiking model for the grid cell and the non-linearity brought by the non-negative weights is sufficient for the emergence of a hexagonal lattice. Similarly, Sorscher et al. (2019) build a theory for the emergence of grid cells and the non-negativity constraint on the firing rates brings a symmetry-breaking effect that leads to the hexagonal firing fields. Unlike these studies that impose non-negativity into the model of grid cells, we use non-negative sparse coding to learn the hippocampal place cells. Non-negativity is introduced to account for some biological aspects such as non-negative firing rates and excitatory connection between cortical areas. In the original paper of non-negative sparse coding (Hoyer, 2003), the model learns Gabor-like V1 simple cells when trained on natural images; in other words, the non-negativity introduced is to account for the biological aspects and it is the principle of sparse coding that enables the model to learn V1 simple cells. The non-negative sparse coding used in this article demonstrates that the principle of sparse coding can be used in the navigational system of the brain even when non-negativity constraint is incorporated. For our model, the non-negativity on neuronal responses is required to have a meaningful firing fields for the modelled hippocampal cells. However, when the non-negativity constraint on the connection A is removed, the modelled hippocampal cells still learn place fields that are similar to results demonstrated in this article. Also, it is important to note that the non-negativity constraint on the entorhinal-hippocampal connection A does not prohibited the role of inhibition in the model. Instead, inhibition in the sparse coding model is vital to provide the competition needed to achieve the sparseness of the model. However, a very detailed model of how sparse coding can be implemented is still not very clear, which is discussed in the next section.
Underlying neural circuits
Our study examines the extent to which sparse coding is as an underlying principle in the navigational system of the brain. However, the current model implies no specific neural circuits for the implementation of sparse coding, rather it is one of the principles that underlies the formation of the neural circuits. Neurophysiological and anatomic studies suggest that the EC and the hippocampus interact via a loop (Tamamaki and Nojyo, 1995; Tamamaki, 1997; Witter et al., 2014). Therefore, feedforward connection from the EC to the hippocampus, recurrent connection within the hippocampus, and feedback connection from the hippocampus to the EC all play an important role, although their specific contributions to the overall function of the network have not been fully uncovered yet. Rennó-Costa and Tort (2017) and Agmon and Burak (2020) investigated the coupling relationship between MEC grid cells and hippocampal place cells and showed that the proposed models can account for some experimental observations. However, how the underlying neural circuits can be implemented is still unclear. The proposed model based on sparse coding in this study does not rule out any of the network structures mentioned above, as sparse coding can be implemented in neural circuits either in a feedforward network with recurrent connection (Zylberberg et al., 2011) or a network with feedforward-feedback loops (Lian et al., 2019).
Future work
The current study does not propose a specific biological neural circuit for implementing sparse coding in the entorhinal-hippocampal region, which is the study of ongoing work. Such a model of these neural circuits would need to take into account the experimentally known networks in this area. Also, other properties of place cells such as phase precession (O’Keefe and Recce, 1993), multiple place fields in large environments (Park et al., 2011) and place map in 3D environments (Grieves et al., 2020) will be investigated in the future. In addition, the model here used prefixed grid cells. We did not attempt here to provide a description for how grid cells emerge, but rather the grid cells are assumed to provide an efficient representation of the environment. It would be interesting to also investigate the role of sparse coding in how grid cells themselves emerge. It is hoped that such future work, which incorporates these aspects of the development process of both grid cells and place cells, will provide further insights into how the navigational system of the brain works. Sparse coding represents just one of a number of possible mechanisms that shape network structures, and much remains to be explored to incorporate other mechanisms, such as those associated with the complexities of metabotropic receptor effects, as discussed by Hasselmo et al. (2021).
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements: We thank Kathrine Clarke and Dr. Ali Almasi and Dr. Catherine Davey for helpful comments on this manuscript.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by the Australian Government Grant AUSMURIB000001 associated with the ONR MURI Grant N00014-19-1-2571.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.