Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Commentary, History, Teaching, and Public Awareness

Say What Was Not Said

Viktor Jirsa
eNeuro 30 April 2021, 8 (2) ENEURO.0128-21.2021; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0128-21.2021
Viktor Jirsa
Aix Marseille University, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Institut de Neurosciences des Systèmes, Marseille 13005, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading
  • Blue Brain Project
  • Human Brain Project
  • In Silico
  • model
  • validation

About one year ago, I learnt of the existence of In Silico. It did not come as a surprise, as I had expected that toward the end of the Human Brain Project (HBP)’s 10-year funding period, there will be a rush of articles and generally increased media interest in covering the project’s progress, achievements, and failures, although In Silico is special, given its 10-year time frame. I joined the HBP in 2014 and lead today one of three science work packages. For this documentary, I expected to hear requests for interviews, but there was literally nothing of that sort. After I have finally watched In Silico, I understand why. The documentary is in fact not about HBP, despite claims to the contrary by a previous review (Abbott, 2020). In Silico director, Noah Hutton, rather focuses on his fascination with Henry Markram’s 10-year vision of building a brain from the bottom up, the Blue Brain Project (BBP). As will be described towards the end, the HBP follows a stringent and much broader concept of integrating neuroscience and medicine with technology. In a companion commentary (Destexhe, 2021), Alain Destexhe explains that only a small percentage of the actual efforts in HBP is devoted to Henry Markram’s activities in the project, I will thus not further dwell on it.

My second expectation was to find a discussion of science, shedding light on the scientific controversy regarding the endeavor of constructing an in silico brain. Disappointingly, this expectation was not fulfilled either, and it is, in my opinion, a missed opportunity. Noah Hutton thematizes several points such as bottom-up construction, consciousness, as well as noise and variability. But the discussion remains shallow, hardly reaching beyond snippets of statements by neuroscientists flagging key words and reiterating the complexity of these themes.

What comes across to some degree, though, is the concern regarding the challenge of validation of BBP’s bottom-up modeling effort. Although it is never explicitly said, it is somewhat lingering in between the lines. In that context, it is suggested that emerging traveling wave activity may be seen as a first indicator of self-organization phenomena in these simulations, which have not been directly programmed into the system (see also Yves Frégnac’s companion commentary; Frégnac, 2021). This is indeed true but far from surprising. Most theoretical neuroscientists are capable of predicting these patterns and characterizing their dynamic properties analytically (that is, with paper and pencil), including velocity and wave shape, deriving them from the architecture of the network. In fact, such analysis is representative of the diverse nature and scope of the multiscale approaches harbored in HBP, linking structural and functional properties across spatial and temporal scales. HBP clearly supports bottom-up approaches typically depicting neuronal systems on microscopic scales, but it also engages macroscopic top-down approaches rooted in the mathematics of dynamical systems theory, as well as statistical physics and mean field approaches that bridge across the various scales, at which brain dynamics unfold. The multiplicity of these approaches then offers alternate routes for validation through tests of consistency and explanatory power, in relation to data, that need not be solely restricted to bottom-up modeling.

The challenge of validation is two-fold. First, there is the matter of validation of the simulated activity, emerging from the bottom-up modeling of the brain. In In Silico, Sebastian Seung questions whether and how we will know if a simulation truly provides the right end result. The demand is a justified request for cross validation against empirical data. A second line of response to validation addresses the link of brain activity to behavior. Zachary Mainen points out that emergent patterns of activity may or may not be meaningful and need to be linked to function. Arguably, an issue to be raised here is that behavior, in its own right, is an elusive matter which neuroscientist ultimately seek to comprehend (Frégnac, 2017; Pillai and Jirsa, 2017; Jirsa et al., 2019). I will try to unpack these points in more detail, as their substance is essentially left untouched in the documentary.

First of all, the link between a mechanistic framework for brain simulation and the resultant brain activity can be useful to understand, even in absence of a systematic behavioral interrogation, especially when it leads way to categorical classification or functional quantification that can be of practical value for diagnostics or therapeutics. For instance, markers of brain activity can be used to diagnose disease and evaluate efficiency of curative interventions. Examples of this can be found in the HBP such as the perturbational complexity index (PCI) measuring different levels of consciousness in brain states (Renzo et al., 2019), or estimates of the extent of the epileptogenic zone in patients, who are candidates for epilepsy surgery (Proix et al., 2018). The latter HBP efforts have led to the large-scale clinical trial Epinov (https://www.3ds.com/fr/recits/living-brain/) using patient-specific brain models to predict and optimize individual surgery outcome.

In these applications, variability within and between subjects can pose a daunting challenge. Yves Frégnac named it as one of the key bottleneck issues to be solved, to make a better use of Big Data in Neuroscience (Frégnac, 2017): when considering in a given brain synaptic weights for a given cell-pair type, variability in their values should not be considered as biological noise, but could, on the contrary, reflect a differential distribution of information. For a simulation to be functionally meaningful, it would then be required to retrieve information from a large range of possible realizations stored in associative memories. Simulating only the mean and the variance (devoid of information) is thus not enough and can sometimes be even wrong (Marder and Goaillard, 2006).

The same principles extend across scales, including the full brain level. When considering several brains, as each differs from each other, they maintain full functionality within a certain range of variability, but then outside of this range, brain function is diminished or lost. The functional loss, however, occurs differently for each brain. The same structural change may be functionally dramatic in one brain, but inconsequential in another. This propensity for different system configurations to support the same or similar functions is called neurodegeneracy and is at the root of the argument of many critical voices regarding mechanistic modeling in general, and the BBP in particular. Neurodegeneracy causes model non-identifiability and dilutes or destroys the concept of an optimal parameter set (as there is none). Rather there is a degenerate range of co-dependent parameters, in which healthy brain operation is possible. This is not a showstopper for bottom-up modeling approaches as in BBP, instead, it is to be conceptually integrated into the model building process. In fact, neurodegeneracy is not only possible but even necessary in the models as it is crucial for capturing the robustness of the brain against injuries and pathologies (Jirsa, 2020). As such, this criticism, alluding to an apparent challenge, is practically highlighting a research avenue ripe for scientific exploration, and thus a potential strength that would only lead to the enhancement of the resulting models.

These concepts are difficult, but we can get a handle on them through the type of approaches advocated in BBP and HBP. The link with behavior is less tangible. In the documentary it has been presented as a potential strategy (again, superficially) to validate the simulations from in silico brains. Behavior remains one of the conundrums in neuroscience, which is the more surprising as it is actually its explanandum. Said differently, we study the explanans, the brain, but are incapable of properly defining that which we actually wish to explain, the emergent behavior. As long as we are confined to summary statistics as a place holder for human behavior, we will continue to fall short in our attempts to conceptually capture and characterize behavior which is at its core a complex dynamical phenomenon. Then, naturally, we will not be able to make meaningful statements about successfully modeling a brain demonstrating behavior, because, likely, we will not even be able to recognize it if it did, given that we lack the proper language, tools, and concepts to probe for it. I intentionally exaggerate here to get the following point across: without a proper definition and operationalization of behavior, we will not be able to generalize laboratory results to more complex behaviors, and thus completely lack ecological validity.

The call for a better understanding of behavior is a theme that is being revisited regularly in neuroscience. In the 1960s and 1970s, Gibson and Bernstein were a major source of inspiration for the ecological approach of perception and action, highlighting the coordination of movement and environment. Kelso and Turvey followed in the 1980s and 1990s, attempting to quantitatively formulate the phenomenological laws underlying behavior (Kelso and Tuller, 1984). Scott Kelso, in particular, applied this dynamics perspective to explain brain mechanisms. He just recently pointed out to me that it is amusing to see articles in leading journals in the 2020s demanding more ecologically relevant behavior in neuroscience research (Krakauer et al., 2017). I agree with him. As a consequence, though, the need for a better understanding of behavior disqualifies it from being an unambiguous solid reference to be used at this point in the validation of mechanistic models of brain function, except perhaps for simple invertebrate organisms with stereotyped reflex behavior. Validation, thus, remains a big issue for the human brain. It is not a challenge specific to BBP or HBP, it is a key challenge in neuroscience at large. And this is why we need visionary scientists such as Henry Markram who break in the path and have the guts of moving forward an entire field. Terry Sejnowski stated this nicely in the documentary, that the difference between pioneers and followers is that the pioneers are the guys with the arrows in the back. Well spoken.

So In Silico is more a narrative about leadership than about science. Noah Hutton’s initial fascination with Markram’s 10-year vision apparently fades away as time goes by, leaving him with space for frustration at the end. Still, I would have appreciated a little more, well, appreciation of the challenges that leadership takes on at such a large-scale. It is an underrated (at least in science) but foundational factor of successful leadership: the synergetic integration across diverse domains, such as theory, experiment, technology, management, politics, communication, society, and ethics. This credo of integration resonates with and guides European funding directions, and it is not uncommon for it to manifest itself in impact statements of ambitious grant submissions. Albeit the actual execution of such integration, within a project of the complexity of a European flagship, remains a deed accomplished only by a few. It is easy to critique, write down words of wisdom and propose a novel, more sophisticated, more promising, more ethical and “whatever-better” program (see, for instance, Mainen et al., 2016). There can and should be no limits for idealism and its imagining of perfect paths. Grounded progress and impact, on the other hand, specifically in highly complex multidisciplinary domains, can only occur through arduous ventures. It needs not only be deeply rooted in excellent science but also to be framed within a clear vision, enacted as an executable uninterrupted workflow that is large enough to build societal impact, convince funders and aggregate large interest within the science community (see also Christophe Bernard's editorial addressing this point; Bernard, 2021). The HBP is a good example thereof. Since the change of leadership in the HBP, Katrin Amunts has completely reshaped the project’s organization of science and neuroinformatics efforts through the introduction of innovative integration tools such as co-design projects, show cases and voucher calls (Amunts et al., 2019), leading to the creation of a European digital research infrastructure (http://www.ebrains.eu). It is the type of work that is ultimately required to create transformative impact and leave a mark in the history of neuroscience. Ploughing through with such an ambitious workplan, regardless, or despite, of others’ skepticism toward its vision or communication style, ultimately redraws the frontiers of neuroscience by breaking new grounds. It also induces the emergence of alternative complementary approaches to address the problems that could not be solved by the original approach in the first place. This is what should happen now, maybe is already happening.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgements: I wish to thank JA Scott Kelso, Yves Frégnac and Hiba Sheheitli who provided many useful comments on an early version of this text. Much of the work in my laboratory is funded through the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Framework Programme for Research and Innovation under the Specific Grant Agreement No. 945539 (Human Brain Project SGA3).

Footnotes

  • This paper is part of the Epistemological Lessons from the Blue and Human Brain Projects Collection. To view all of the papers in the Collection, please visit the Epistemological Lessons Collections page at eNeuro.org.

  • The author declares no competing financial interests.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Abbott A (2020) Documentary follows implosion of billion-euro brain project. Nature 588:215–216. doi:10.1038/d41586-020-03462-3 pmid:33288920
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Amunts K, Knoll AC, Lippert T, Pennartz CMA, Ryvlin P, Destexhe A, Jirsa VK, D’Angelo E, Bjaalie JG (2019) The Human Brain Project–Synergy between neuroscience, computing, informatics, and brain-inspired technologies. PLOS Biology 17:e3000344.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. ↵
    Bernard C (2021) The Beauty and the beast. eNeuro 8:e0142-21.
    OpenUrl
  4. ↵
    Destexhe A (2021) “In Silico”: computer simulations from neurons up to the whole brain. eNeuro
  5. ↵
    Frégnac Y (2017) Big data and the industrialization of neuroscience: a safe roadmap for understanding the brain? Science 358:470–477. doi:10.1126/science.aan8866 pmid:29074766
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Frégnac Y (2021) How blue is the sky? eNeuro 8:e0130-21.
  7. ↵
    Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR, Huys R (2019) Grand unified theories of the brain need better understanding of behavior: the two-tiered emergence of function. Ecol Psychol 31:152–165. doi:10.1080/10407413.2019.1615207
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  8. ↵
    Jirsa V (2020) Structured flows on manifolds as guiding concepts in brain science. In Viol K, Schöller H, Aichhorn W (eds), Selbstorganisation – ein Paradigma für die Humanwissenschaften. Springer, Wiesbaden.
  9. ↵
    Kelso JAS, Tuller B (1984) A dynamical basis for action systems. In: Handbook of cognitive neuroscience (Gazzaniga MS, ed), pp 321–356. New York: Plenum.
  10. ↵
    Krakauer JW, Ghazanfar AA, Gomez-Marin A, MacIver MA, Poeppel D (2017) Neuroscience needs behavior: correcting a reductionist bias. Neuron 93:480–490. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.041 pmid:28182904
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Mainen ZF, Häusser M, Pouget A (2016) A better way to crack the brain. Nature 539:159–161. doi:10.1038/539159a pmid:27830818
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Marder E, Goaillard GM (2006) Variability, compensation and homeostasis in neuron and network function. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:563–574.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Pillai AS, Jirsa VK (2017) Symmetry breaking in space-time hierarchies shapes brain dynamics and behavior. Neuron 94:1010–1026. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.013 pmid:28595045
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Proix T, Jirsa VK, Bartolomei F, Guye M, Truccolo W (2018) Predicting the spatiotemporal diversity of seizure propagation and termination in human focal epilepsy. Nature Commun 9:1088.
  15. ↵
    Renzo C, Pigorini A, Casarotto S, Fecchio M, Faria G, Sarasso S, Rosanova M, Gosseries O, Boly M, Bodart O, Ledoux D, Brichant JF, Nobili L, Laureys S, Tononi G, Massimini M, Casali AG (2019) A fast and general method to empirically estimate the complexity of brain responses to transcranial and intracranial stimulations. Brain Stimul 12:1280–1289. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2019.05.013 pmid:31133480
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Christophe Bernard, INSERM & Institut de Neurosciences des Systèmes

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: NONE.

NA

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 8 (2)
eNeuro
Vol. 8, Issue 2
March/April 2021
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Say What Was Not Said
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Say What Was Not Said
Viktor Jirsa
eNeuro 30 April 2021, 8 (2) ENEURO.0128-21.2021; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0128-21.2021

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Say What Was Not Said
Viktor Jirsa
eNeuro 30 April 2021, 8 (2) ENEURO.0128-21.2021; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0128-21.2021
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • Blue Brain Project
  • Human Brain Project
  • In Silico
  • model
  • validation

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Commentary

  • Some Tips for Writing Science
  • COVID-19 Deterred Career Path of Our Undergraduate Neuroscience Students: Educators’ Perspective
  • Remembering Hirsh Cohen and His Role in Developing Computational Neuroscience
Show more Commentary

History, Teaching, and Public Awareness

  • Some Tips for Writing Science
  • COVID-19 Deterred Career Path of Our Undergraduate Neuroscience Students: Educators’ Perspective
  • Remembering Hirsh Cohen and His Role in Developing Computational Neuroscience
Show more History, Teaching, and Public Awareness

Subjects

  • Epistemological Lessons from the Blue and Human Brain Projects
  • History, Teaching, and Public Awareness
  • Commentaries

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.