Abstract
Action video gaming can promote neural plasticity. Short-term monocular patching drives neural plasticity in the visual system of human adults. For instance, short-term monocular patching of 0.5–5 h briefly enhances the patched eye’s contribution in binocular vision (i.e., short-term ocular dominance plasticity). In this study, we investigate whether action video gaming can influence this plasticity in adults with normal vision. We measured participants’ eye dominance using a binocular phase combination task before and after 2.5 h of monocular patching. Participants were asked to play action video games, watch action video game movies, or play non-action video games during the period of monocular patching. We found that participants’ change of ocular dominance after monocular patching was not significantly different either for playing action video games versus watching action video game movies (Comparison 1) or for playing action video games versus playing non-action video games (Comparison 2). These results suggest that action video gaming does not either boost or eliminate short-term ocular dominance plasticity, and that the neural site for this type of plasticity might be in the early visual pathway.
Significance Statement
Recent studies have shown that short-term (0.5–5 h) monocular patching induces a new form of short-term ocular dominance plasticity in human adults, in which the patched eye rather than the unpatched eye gets stronger, and the effect is transient. On the other hand, there is evidence that action video gaming has potential in enhancing perceptual learning induced visual plasticity in adulthood. In this study, we found that action video gaming did not impact short-term ocular dominance plasticity in visually normal adults. Our psychophysical evidence suggests that the neural site of this plasticity should be local and early in the cortical pathway.
Introduction
Action video gaming has been popular in the general public and for research (Green and Bavelier, 2003, 2006, 2007, 2012; Dye et al., 2009; Buckley et al., 2010; Jeon et al., 2012; Appelbaum et al., 2013; Latham et al., 2013; Morin-Moncet et al., 2016; Franceschini et al., 2017; Bediou et al., 2018; Föcker et al., 2018). It is fast-paced and perceptually demanding, requiring the players to provide quick motor responses and oversee objects in surroundings (Dale and Green, 2017; Bediou et al., 2018; Wong and Chang, 2018; Bavelier and Green, 2019). In both observational (Green and Bavelier, 2003; Blacker and Curby, 2013; Wilms et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2017) and training (Green and Bavelier, 2003; Boot et al., 2008; Oei and Patterson, 2013; Bisoglio et al., 2014) studies, it has been shown to enhance our cognition, perception and attention on task-relevant and irrelevant visual stimuli (Green and Bavelier, 2003, 2006; Castel et al., 2005; Boot et al., 2008; Dye et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2016; Föcker et al., 2018; Bavelier and Green, 2019). Action video gaming also improves visual functions of adults. For instance, after 30–50 h of action video game training, the adults exhibited enhanced spatial resolution (Green and Bavelier, 2007), improved contrast sensitivity (Li et al., 2009), and better performance in a visual counting task (Li et al., 2011). Jeon et al. (2012) later confirmed these visual improvements by measuring visual acuity, stereopsis, global motion, and configural face processing. Electrophysiological evidence shows that professional gamers have faster detection and responses to visual stimuli (Latham et al., 2013). Taken together, these studies suggest that action video gaming has potential in enhancing visual plasticity in adulthood.
Recently, a new form of neural plasticity has been reported in adults. Patching one eye (i.e., monocular patching) for a short period (0.5–5 h) of time increases the contribution of the patched eye in binocular vision (Lunghi et al., 2011; Zhou et al., 2013a,b; Bai et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2017; Min et al., 2018, 2019; Ramamurthy and Blaser, 2018). The change, which is referred to as short-term ocular dominance plasticity (Lunghi et al., 2015a), is linked to the primary visual cortex (Zhou et al., 2017). The plasticity is quite different from that observed during the critical period, where the unpatched eye improves (Hubel and Wiesel, 1970; Berardi et al., 2000). Lunghi et al. (2011) first reported the phenomenon using binocular rivalry (i.e., binocular competition); the change lasted for up to 90 min. Other investigators subsequently confirmed this finding via binocular rivalry or binocular combination (Zhou et al., 2013a,b; Bai et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2017; Başgöze et al., 2018; Ramamurthy and Blaser, 2018). The neural basis of this short-term ocular dominance plasticity is thought to occur in the early visual pathway (Lunghi et al., 2015a,b; Zhou et al., 2015; Chadnova et al., 2017; Binda et al., 2018). Despite these numerous studies, the neural mechanisms of this plasticity and factors that could enhance it are still unknown.
Recent studies (Lunghi et al., 2016; Sauvan et al., 2019) suggest that neuroplastic changes induced by both short-term and long-term patching are tightly connected. With a presumably similar neural mechanism, one form of plasticity might be able to predict or enhance the other. Given the potential of action video gaming in enhancing the long-term visual plasticity as we mentioned above (e.g., perceptual learning; Li et al., 2009, 2011; Jeon et al., 2012), we thought it would be worthwhile to see whether action video gaming could influence short-term ocular dominance plasticity. In particular, we asked our participants to complete three different tasks (playing action video games, watching action video game movies and playing non-action video games) during 2.5 h of monocular patching, and compared their pre-patching and post-patching ocular dominance. We hypothesized that action video gaming could strengthen this form of plasticity, and, therefore, we expected ocular dominance to be modulated significantly more with action video gaming, compared with the other two tasks. We found that for all three conditions, monocular patching induced significant changes in ocular dominance. However, our results showed no evidence for a strengthened effect with action video gaming.
Materials and Methods
Participants
We recruited twelve normal adults (age: 23.00 ± 2.05 years old; seven females) for this study. According to their reported playing habits and the ranks given by their games, four of the participants were considered “expert” whereas the other eight were considered “less experienced”. We did not recruit “novices” (with no experience of the action video games that we used) in this study. This was because the gaming tasks could be difficult for novices to finish due to the complexity of this game genre (e.g., the complicated environment and the manipulation of items and/or spells in these games). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity (no worse than 0.0 logMAR) in both eyes, with spherical equivalent no more than 1.00D and astigmatism no more than 0.75D. No participants had ophthalmic diseases including but not limited to strabismus, amblyopia and nystagmus, or had history of visual training, occlusion therapy or ophthalmic surgeries including recently-performed refractive surgeries. During the experiments, participants were required to wear their normal refractive correction if needed.
A written informed consent was obtained from each participant before the beginning of the experiments. This study was in line with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of Wenzhou Medical University.
Apparatus
In the ocular dominance measures, all stimuli were generated on a MacBook Pro (13-inch, 2017, Apple Inc) running MATLAB R2016b (The MathWorks Inc) with Psychtoolbox 3.0.14 extensions. We used a head-mounted display, GOOVIS (AMOLED display, NED Optics), to achieve dichoptic viewing. The refresh rate of the display was 60 Hz, and the resolution was 1920 × 1080. Gamma correction was applied to ensure a linear output in the test. We used a custom-made chinrest to prevent movements of participants’ heads during the measurement sessions.
We included two desktop games, the League of Legends (Riot Games) and the PlayerUnknown’s Battlegrounds (PUBG Corporation), and their similar mobile versions (Tencent Games; for details, see https://pvp.qq.com, https://pubgm.qq.com, and https://pg.qq.com) as action video games in this study. For non-action video games, we included Minesweeper (http://minesweeperonline.com/). Participants used their own devices (either laptops or mobile devices) for all tasks.
Design
Each participant completed three experiment sessions. In a typical session (Fig. 1), initial ocular dominance was obtained at baseline (T0), after which the participant received 2.5-h patching of their dominant eye with a translucent patch. During this stage, one of the three tasks (i.e., playing action video games, watching action video game movies and playing non-action video games), was assigned to each participant. Participants completed the tasks at a comfortable distance depending on the platform of the games (whether desktop or mobile) under normal indoor illuminance. Participants were allowed to take a restroom break as needed; during most of the time, however, participants were instructed to focus on the assigned tasks, under supervision of the experimenter. Subsequently, ocular dominance was measured at 0, 3, 6 and 9 min (T1, T2, T3, T4) and 30 min (T5) after the patch was removed. These post-patching results were then compared with the initial baseline, and any changes in ocular dominance would indicate the strength of ocular dominance plasticity.
These three experiment sessions (i.e., playing action video games, watching action video game movies and playing non-action video games) were conducted on separate days in a random order. To compare the impact of pure visual stimulation versus that of complex integrated stimulation (e.g., visual stimulation with auditory inputs and attentional engaging), we turned off the sound while participants were watching action video game movies; in this way, action video game movies should be interpreted as movies that provided the same visual inputs as when participants were playing the games. The former condition would enable us to quantify the pure visual plasticity, while the latter would enable us to quantify the additional benefits of playing action video games.
Procedures
Measurement of ocular dominance was completed by a binocular phase combination paradigm (Ding and Sperling, 2006). In each trial, participants were first asked to finish an eye alignment (fusion) task and then a binocular phase combination task, where two horizontal gratings with the same spatial frequency (0.46 cycles per degree, c/d) and opposite phase shifts (−22.5° and +22.5°) were dichoptically presented to the two eyes of our participants (Fig. 2). Participants would perceive the two stimuli as one fused horizontal grating, of which the perceived phase was determined by the relative strength of the two eyes’ contributions to the binocular viewing. Stimulus contrast was set as 100% for the non-dominant eye and δ × 100% (0 ≤ δ ≤ 1) for the other eye. δ is the interocular contrast ratio close to individuals’ balance point (i.e., at which the binocular perceived phase was close to zero degrees), which was selected based on their performance from practice trials. Participants were asked to move a flanking reference line to the middle of the central dark stripe of the fused grating. This position of the reference line was then converted into the perceived phase for each participant. To avoid a potential positional bias, two configurations were given: in configuration 1, the phase was set as −22.5° for the dominant eye and +22.5° for the non-dominant eye; in configuration 2, the phase was set reversely. This was repeated eight times in a typical test session, which would last ∼3 min. After all the sixteen trials (i.e., two configurations × eight repetitions) were performed, an average perceived phase (i.e., [phase in configuration 1 – phase in configuration 2]/2) was calculated to indicate ocular dominance. A negative change after monocular patching in the perceived phase would indicate that the dominant eye (i.e., the patched eye) became stronger, while a positive change in the perceived phase would indicate that the unpatched eye became stronger. More details of the paradigm (Fig. 2) are described in a previous study (Zhou et al., 2013b).
Data analysis
We grouped the task of playing action video games and that of watching action video game playing into Comparison 1, and the task of playing action video games and that of playing non-action video games into Comparison 2. The ocular dominance changes at different time sessions after monocular patching were compared by Kruskal–Wallis H tests. The results of different tasks in Comparison 1 and Comparison 2, respectively, were compared by repeated-measures ANOVA. To further investigate the magnitude of the effect over time, we calculated the areal measures [area under curve (AUC)] from 0 min to 9 min (i.e., T1 to T4 in Fig. 1) and performed paired samples t tests for further analysis. The level of significance was set as p < 0.05. All statistical analysis was completed in SPSS 23.0 (IBM Corporation).
Results
Comparison 1: playing action video games versus watching action video game movies
In Comparison 1, we compare the change in ocular dominance of action video game play during monocular patching with that of action video game movie watching. As shown in Figure 3A, participants’ change of ocular dominance (i.e., perceived phase change from the pre-patching baseline) was negative after monocular patching, indicating that the patched eye became stronger in both conditions. Kruskal–Wallis H tests also showed that the perceived phase changes were significantly different between different time sessions for both playing action video games (H(5) = 39.498, p < 0.001) and watching action video game movies (H(5) = 42.798, p < 0.001) conditions. A repeated-measures ANOVA further showed that the perceived phase was not significantly different between the two viewing conditions (F(1,11) = 1.122, p = 0.312). To better show the difference between the two viewing conditions in different individuals, we also calculated the areal measures (AUC) within the first 10 min (i.e., T1 to T4 in Fig. 1) and plotted the results in Figure 3B. The average effect (i.e., AUC) for the two conditions were 63.34 667 ± 38.71 312 (playing action video games, mean ± SD) and 73.47083 ± 31.04102 (watching action video game movies, mean ± SD). Overall there was no significant difference between the two viewing conditions (t(11) = −0.813, p = 0.433; two-tailed paired samples t test).
Through a power analysis, we found that the effect size of Comparison 1 was 0.288542, and the sample size needed for power = 80% and significance level = 0.05 would be at least 97. Therefore, we conclude that the difference between the two tasks, if any, would be very small.
Comparison 2: playing action video games versus playing non-action video games
In Comparison 2, we compare the change in ocular dominance of participants playing action video games during monocular patching with that of playing non-action video games. As shown in Figure 4A, participants’ perceived phase change from baseline was negative after monocular patching, indicating that the patched eye became stronger in both conditions. Kruskal–Wallis H tests also showed that the perceived phase changes were significantly different between different time sessions for both playing action video games (H(5) = 39.498, p < 0.001) and playing non-action video games (H(5) = 37.250, p < 0.001) conditions. ANOVA further showed that the perceived phase was not significantly different between the two viewing conditions (F(1,11) = 0.004, p = 0.951). To better show the difference between the two viewing conditions in different individuals, we calculated the areal measures (AUC) within the first 10 min (i.e., T1 to T4 in Fig. 1) and plotted the results in Figure 4B. The average effect (i.e., AUC) for the two conditions were 63.34667 ± 38.71312 (playing action video games, mean ± SD) and 62.50167 ± 35.82488 (playing non-action video game movies, mean ± SD). There was no significant difference between the two viewing conditions (t(11) = 0.092, p = 0.928; two-tailed paired samples t test).
Through a power analysis, we found that the effect size of Comparison 2 was 0.022656, and the sample size needed for power = 80% and significance level = 0.05 would be at least 15,294. Therefore, we conclude that there was no difference between these two tasks.
Does gender play a role in the results?
One interesting finding in the literature is that male participants might have better performance than females in spatial cognition, while females instead showed larger improvements on the same tasks after action video game training (Feng et al., 2007). To clarify the concern, we classified our participants into two subgroups according to their gender (i.e., male vs female; Fig. 5), and analyzed the AUC ratio between the two tasks in both Comparison 1 and Comparison 2. We found no significant difference between the two subgroups [Comparison 1: t(10) = −0.074, p = 0.942 (Fig. 5A); Comparison 2: t(10) = 0.405, p = 0.694 (Fig. 5B)]. These results suggest that the factor of gender had no role in our experiments.
Discussion
In this study, we investigated whether action video gaming during monocular patching could influence ocular dominance plasticity in visually normal adults. In Comparison 1, we assessed whether there would be a difference in short-term monocular patching induced visual plasticity between two conditions: during monocular patching subjects were either playing (i.e., active attendance) or watching (i.e., passive attendance) action video games. Since the visual stimuli in these two conditions were the same, any difference in short-term monocular patching induced visual plasticity would be due to the outcome of playing action video games. In Comparison 2, we investigated whether there would be a difference in short-term monocular patching induced visual plasticity between when observers were playing either action or non-action video games. A typical action video game is more perceptually demanding and difficult to perform than a non-action video game. Because of this difference, we had hypothesized that action video gaming would exert a larger influence on visual plasticity. However, we found that patching with playing action video games did not enhance or eliminate the magnitude of ocular dominance change in either Comparison 1 or Comparison 2. Eye fatigue was not monitored in this experiment; however, some participants did report eye fatigue after playing action video games while others did not.
As a novel form of visual plasticity, short-term ocular dominance plasticity, with its effect on binocular balance and potential for amblyopic treatment (Zhou et al., 2013b, 2019; Lunghi et al., 2019), has drawn the attention of many scientists in the field of vision science. However, the change is transient, as opposed to the permanence of the neuroplastic changes that occur from long-term monocular deprivation during the critical period (Daw, 2014). Recent investigators have postulated that that these two forms of neural plasticity in the visual system are related. For instance, Lunghi et al. (2016) argued that the plasticity induced by short-term monocular patching could predict that induced by long-term patching, thus suggesting a similar neural mechanism for the two types of plasticity. Sauvan et al. (2019) reported that short-term monocular patching could enhance the effect of long-term plasticity, albeit not significantly. In addition, action video gaming has been reported to improve perceptual performance on visual tasks after a few weeks or months of visual training (Levi and Li, 2009; Li et al., 2013; Vedamurthy et al., 2015; Gambacorta et al., 2018). This form of improvement is called perceptual learning. If action video gaming could enhance changes in visual plasticity, it could be employed in concert with monocular patching and monocular training to improve the visual acuity as well as binocular balance in patients with poor vision and other visual disorders such as amblyopia (Gambacorta et al., 2018).
The finding is interesting that there is no significant difference between action video game play and non-action video game play in our participants in terms of short-term ocular dominance plasticity. It is worth noting that in a previous study, Li and colleagues demonstrated a larger improvement in contrast sensitivity with action video game training than with non-action video game training in visually normal adults (Li et al., 2009). It is likely that such an improvement reflects a change in monocular sensitivity in the early cortical pathway (e.g., V1) and is relevant to perceptual learning. Therefore, the inconsistency between our study and Li and colleagues’ might be due to different neural mechanisms being responsible for perceptual learning and monocular patching-induced plasticity. Perceptual learning relies on repeated intensively visual training and is thought to involve the properties (i.e., peak tuning and signal/noise) of individual cortical neurons before binocular summation (Hua et al., 2010; Ren et al., 2016), whereas monocular patching-induced plasticity relies on short-term visual deprivation and involves the interactions between neurons receiving left and right eye inputs (Binda et al., 2018; Chadnova et al., 2017; Zhou et al., 2015).
We had expected to see an enhancement of ocular dominance change by action video game play for two reasons. First, during patching, playing action video games via the unpatched eye could recruit additional attentive processes involving top-down feedback from higher visual areas (Gilbert and Li, 2013). If such attentional feedback modulated changes in ocular dominance, a low-level phenomenon, the change in ocular dominance would have increased. However, this was not the case in our findings from Comparison 2. Hence, ocular dominance plasticity seems to be determined by local low-level, feedforward interactions in the primary visual cortex. Second, cross-modal inputs have been shown to affect visual plasticity (Iurilli et al., 2012; Ibrahim et al., 2016; Teichert et al., 2018, 2019) by suppressing the early visual cortical activity in animals (Iurilli et al., 2012; Ibrahim et al., 2016). Also, recent studies have revealed that non-visual sensory deprivation can cross-modally restore plasticity in the visual cortex in matured mice (Teichert et al., 2018, 2019). Nevertheless, our results in the two comparisons show that complex integrated stimulation (e.g., auditory inputs and attentional engaging) seems to exert no additional effect, compared with visual stimulation alone (i.e., watching without hearing or playing), on short-term ocular dominance plasticity in human adults. Therefore, the neural mechanisms responsible for cross-modal influences may not be involved in the neural plasticity induced by short-term monocular patching. Another explanation, however, is that the attentional engagement and the visuo-auditory integration might have opposite effects on short-term ocular dominance plasticity, which could lead to a null effect as observed in our experiment. Future studies may need to determine the separate effects of these factors.
In fact, our finding that the complex integrated stimulation may not play a significant role in short-term ocular dominance plasticity is consistent with the ones from other studies which demonstrate that only low-level areas are locally involved in short-term ocular dominance plasticity in human adults (Lunghi et al., 2015a,b; Chadnova et al., 2017; Binda et al., 2018). To illustrate, an electrophysiological study reports a change in the amplitude of the C1 component following patching, a phenomenon that has been confirmed to be closely related to the activity of V1 (Lunghi et al., 2015a). There is also evidence that reduced GABA concentration in V1 is highly correlated with the perceptual boost of the patched eye (Lunghi et al., 2015b). Furthermore, a recent fMRI study suggests a significant impact on the neural coding at the level of V1 after 2 h of monocular contrast deprivation (Binda et al., 2018). Our results, together with these previous reports, suggest that the neural site of the ocular dominance plasticity from monocular patching is local and resides within the early cortical pathway.
In short, we found that action video gaming does not impact short-term ocular dominance plasticity in visually normal adults more than watching action video game movies or playing non-action video games. Thus, complex integrated stimulation, in contrast to visual stimulation alone, may not play a significant role in this plasticity in human adults. Our findings suggest the neural mechanism responsible for short-term ocular dominance plasticity might occur early in the cortical pathway.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements: We thank all our subjects for participating in the experiments.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China Grants 31970975 and 81500754 and the Wenzhou Medical University Grant QTJ16005 (to J.Z.) and by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research Grant CCI-125686, the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Grant 228103, and the ERA-NET NEURON Grant JTC2015 (to R.F.H.).
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.