Abstract
The organization of region-to-region functional connectivity has major implications for understanding information transfer and transformation between brain regions. We extended connective field mapping methodology to 3-D anatomic space to derive estimates of corticocortical functional organization. Using multiple publicly available human (both male and female) resting-state fMRI data samples for model testing and replication analysis, we have three main findings. First, we found that the functional connectivity between early visual regions maintained a topographic relationship along the anterior-posterior dimension, which corroborates previous research. Higher order visual regions showed a pattern of connectivity that supports convergence and biased sampling, which has implications for their receptive field properties. Second, we demonstrated that topographic organization is a fundamental aspect of functional connectivity across the entire cortex, with higher topographic connectivity between regions within a functional network than across networks. The principle gradient of topographic connectivity across the cortex resembled whole-brain gradients found in previous work. Last but not least, we showed that the organization of higher order regions such as the lateral prefrontal cortex demonstrate functional gradients of topographic connectivity and convergence. These organizational features of the lateral prefrontal cortex predict task-based activation patterns, particularly visual specialization and higher order rules. In sum, these findings suggest that topographic input is a fundamental motif of functional connectivity between cortical regions for information processing and transfer, with maintenance of topography potentially important for preserving the integrity of information from one region to another.
Significance Statement
Quantifying spatial patterns of region-to-region functional connectivity provides an avenue for testing theories of corticocortical information transformation and organization. This work demonstrates that this quantification is feasible not only in early visual cortex, but even in highly multimodal regions where spatial topography is less clear. Overall, we show that topographic relationships as a common motif functional connectivity across the cortex between regions within the same functional network and that analyzing the lateral prefrontal cortex in terms of topographic connectivity reveals organizational features that voxel-wise connectivity analysis misses.
Introduction
One primary goal of systems neuroscience is to understand how integration occurs across cortical areas in order to support complex behavior (Cavada and Goldman-Rakic, 1991; Hoover and Strick, 1993; Haber et al., 2000). As different brain regions display spatially specific patterns of long range and short range connections to their target regions (Van Essen and Gallant, 1994; Kravitz et al., 2011, 2013), it is believed that the particular properties of brain circuitry must provide a foundation for brain function and information processing, and that connectivity profiles from one region to another allows for the potential understanding of information transfer/transformation. Topographic, convergent, and divergent connectivity have been postulated to support different computational needs of a given neural network (Thivierge and Marcus, 2007). For example, systematic convergent connectivity in the visual hierarchy has been shown to be responsible for object processing, from lines (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962) to complex objects (Tanaka, 1997), while divergent connectivity has been theorized in motor systems for complex feedback mechanisms, such as motor efference copies (Wolpert and Flanagan, 2001).
Neuroimaging studies of brain connectivity, while indirect, have the benefit of their exhaustive sampling across the entire anatomic space, which complement the interpretations from the sparse sampling of rigorous anatomic tracing in animals. This is evident in functional connectivity studies using resting state fMRI data, from which a low dimensional structure of large scale networks has been consistently observed as a key organizational feature of the cortex (Fox et al., 2005; Biswal et al., 2010; Yeo et al., 2011; Di et al., 2013). Indeed, decomposing the whole brain into its fundamental gradients have revealed a structure for the organization of large scale networks, with the primary gradient trending from unimodal networks to multimodal networks (Margulies et al., 2016). However, despite the overwhelming focus on large scale brain organization, detailing the particular patterns of connectivity from one region to another remains a challenge.
Region-to-region topographic connectivity has been observed between early visual areas, which show retinotopic organization (Dumoulin and Wandell, 2008; Wang et al., 2015). By systematically seeding functional connectivity from known functional gradients, it has been shown that functional connectivity within a visual region, and between visual regions seem to follow along known eccentricity organization, along the anterior-posterior axis (Arcaro et al., 2015). In other words, certain parts of a visual region that represent certain eccentricities are more likely to connect to the specific parts of other visual regions that represent similar eccentricities. While this type of systematic seeding is possible in areas with a known and measurable sensory topography, a more general model, such as connective field mapping (Haak et al., 2013), is useful for studying the connection topography of higher order regions. The previous connective field mapping technique fits 2-D surface based population receptive fields (pRFs) using cortical signal from one region as the input to another region (Haak et al., 2013). This method has been used to model early visual areas, and demonstrate maintenance of retinotopic organization across these visual regions (Haak et al., 2013; Gravel et al., 2014).
It is unclear whether the linear pattern of connectivity observed in early visual cortex is dependent on a shared sensory topographic organization, or whether it is present in any highly connected network. In particular, for multimodal regions such as the prefrontal cortex, while it is known that various subdivisions are closely connected, the exact functional organization remains controversial (Ungerleider et al., 1998; Goldman-Rakic, 1999; Petrides, 2005). In theoretical models, spatial topography in connectivity is postulated to serve the function of ensuring the fidelity of information transfer across a network (Thivierge and Marcus, 2007). Topographic organization of connectivity also has the potential to generate complex abstract representations by virtue of topographic overlap (Tinsley, 2009). This suggests that abstract computations can be performed by biases in spatial connectivity and filtering between regions of a large scale network, in the same way that has been suggested in the visual system (Van Essen and Gallant, 1994; Tinsley, 2009).
In this study, we have modified and extended the connective field model, allowing for the estimation of topography of functional connectivity between regions using resting-state fMRI data in order to infer potential fidelity of information transfer between regions within and across unimodal and multimodal networks. This method fits a 3-D isotropic Gaussian to the connectivity pattern in the mapping region from individual voxels in the seed region (Fig. 1A). This fitting procedure produced four parameters, three location parameters describing the preferred locus of connectivity and a spread parameter, and the fitted Gaussian acts like an encoding model for time courses of BOLD activity (Fig. 1B). We further estimated the degree of topographic mapping from one region to another by performing Procrustes analysis. The degree to which the rank ordered parameters can be fit to the rank ordered coordinates using a linear transformation provides the estimation of the topographic mapping between regions. We first examined the degree of topography connectivity in the visual system, as the literature is relatively established, and then examined whether it is simply a specific case of a general motif of brain functional connectivity across various networks, and finally used this approach to examine prefrontal cortex functional organization.
3-D connective field modeling. A, Gaussian fitting procedure. Each seed voxel’s time course (dotted bracket inside the matrix on the left represents the time course of one voxel of the seed region) is correlated to every voxel’s time course in the mapping region and then Fisher’s z transformed. For each seed voxel, the corresponding Fisher’s Z matrix is then fitted to the 3-D Gaussian model, by minimizing the correlation distance between the Fisher’s Z matrix and the Gaussian distribution. The fitted Gaussian parameters, location, and standard deviation (SD) parameters, for each seed voxel are used in subsequent quantitative tests, such as topography and convergence. Validation of the statistical models used to assess topography are provided in Extended Data Figures 1-1, 1-2. These two metrics, topographic connectivity and convergence, have a negative nonlinear relationship (Extended Data Fig. 1-3). B, Time-series prediction model. For each time point, the overlap between the actual activation in the mapping region [A(x,y,z,t)] and the fitted Gaussian distribution in the mapping region [gv(x,y,z)] for that particular seed voxel produce the expected level of activity in the corresponding seed voxel. The first arrow points to a period of low overlap, and thus a period of low expected level of activity, while the second arrow points to a period of high overlap, and thus a period of high expected level of activity. This procedure was used to evaluate the performance of the 3-D connective field model.
Extended Data Figure 1-1
Simulations results of rank linearity and probabilistic model of rank linearity. A, Rank linearity approximates the probability of a given rank value. Model parameters represent best fit values obtained by nonlinear least squares fitting in MATLAB’s curve fitting toolbox. B, Effect of data smoothness on rank linearity is related to the estimated number of resels in a region; 95% confidence intervals of the simulation and model are displayed as error bars on each point. Simulation values in green dots and model values in black dashed lines. Download Figure 1-1, TIF file.
Extended Data Figure 1-2
Simulations (green) deviate from model data (black) at low number of resels as shown by two examples: a region of 729 voxels (A) and a region of 125 voxels (B). We reasoned that if this was a result of number of total resels in a region, then this deviance should occur at a lower smoothness value in regions with a lower number of voxels. This is what we observed comparing A, B, As a rule of thumb, we tag all calculations of rank linearity when the number of resels are below 10. While no observations in the main manuscript were tagged, theoretically one could perform Monte Carlo simulations to determine appropriate error rates in those cases. Download Figure 1-2, TIF file.
Extended Data Figure 1-3
Relationship between linearity and convergence factor. A, The node by node linearity and convergence matrices. An inverse relationship between these metrics can be seen in the pattern of node by node relationships. B, A quantitative comparison of all unique pairs of regions. There is a negative non-linear relationship between linearity and convergence, although this relationship only accounts for a minority of variance (37.06%) across the two metrics. Download Figure 1-3, TIF file.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
We used three publicly available datasets: one sample of healthy controls with well validated resting state fMRI data, which is the Cambridge Buckner subset of the 1000 functional connectomes project (Biswal et al., 2010), and two smaller samples more well suited for test-retest reliability studies. The first small sample being the intrinsic brain activity test retest (IBATRT) dataset (Zuo et al., 2014), and the second being the midnight scan club (MSC) dataset (Gordon et al., 2017).
For the Cambridge Buckner data sample there were a total of 198 subjects (123 female), ages 18–30 (M = 21.03, SD = 2.31), with all subjects included in the final analysis. For the IBATRT data sample, there were a total of 36 subjects (18 female) ages 19–48 (M = 27.33, SD = 7.86) with two sessions and each with two runs. Four IBATRT subjects were excluded due to excessive motion in at least one of the four runs, leaving 32 subjects with data in the two runs of session 1 (16 female; ages 19–48, M = 26.03, SD = 7.24). For the MSC dataset, there were a total of 10 subjects (five female), ages 24–34 (M = 29.1, SD = 3.3), and all included in the final analysis.
fMRI parameters
Cambridge Buckner data (Siemens 3T Trim Trio): T1-weighted images were collected with MPRAGE with the following image parameters: slices = 192, matrix size = 144 × 192, voxel resolution = 1.20 × 1.00 × 1.33 mm3. Resting state fMRI data were T2*-weighted images acquired using EPI with the following parameters: 47 interleaved axial slices, TR = 3000 ms, voxel resolution = 3.0 × 3.0 × 3.0 mm3 (119 volumes).
IBATRT data (Siemens 3T Trim Trio): T1 weighted images were collected with MPRAGE with the following image parameters: slices = 176, matrix size = 256 × 256, voxel resolution = 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm3. Resting state fMRI data were T2*-weighted images acquired using EPI with the following parameters: 29 ascending axial slices, slice gap = 0.36 mm, TR = 1750 ms, voxel resolution = 3.4× 3.4 × 3.6 mm3 (343 volumes each run). While there were up to four runs across two sessions for subjects, we only used the two runs in the first session.
MSC Data (Siemens 3T Trim Trio): Four T1 weighted images were collected: slices = 224, voxel resolution = 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 mm3. Four T2 weighted images: 224 slices, voxel resolution 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 mm3. Resting state fMRI were T2*-weighted images acquired using EPI: 36 interleaved axial slices, TR = 2200 ms, voxel resolution = 4 × 4 × 4 mm3 (818 volumes each session). A gradient echo field map was collected with the same parameters as the BOLD EPI images for each session. There were 10 sessions for each subject, all used in the analysis.
Image preprocessing
For each individual in the Cambridge Buckner and IBATRT data samples, preprocessing was performed using SPM12 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/). The functional images were first corrected for slice timing, and then realigned to the middle volume according to a six-parameter rigid body transformation. Structural images were coregistered with the mean functional image, segmented, and then normalized to the MNI template using both linear and nonlinear transformations. Functional images were normalized using the same parameters as the structural normalization.
Further preprocessing was performed following the standard procedures of resting-state fMRI analysis either using CONN (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012) or custom MATLAB (2015b) scripts. A nuisance regression was constructed with the following confounding variables: six motion parameters up to their second derivatives, scans with evidence of excessive motion [framewise displacement (FD) > 0.5 or DVARS > 5], session onset, estimated physiological signal generated through aCompCor (a temporal PCA of the white matter and CSF voxels with the number of components included determined individually on the basis of a Monte Carlo null model; Behzadi et al., 2007), and a linear drift component. For the Cambridge Buckner data the residuals from the nuisance regression were filtered using a bandpass between the frequencies of 0.008 and 0.09 Hz, while for the IBATRT data, the bandpass filtering and nuisance regression were done simultaneously (Hallquist et al., 2013). Finally, the resultant data were despiked using a tangent squashing function.
For the MSC data, we used their release of preprocessed data which includes slice intensity correction, mode 1000 intensity normalization, realignment, transformation into Talairach space and FSL’s field distortion correction (Smith et al., 2004). These data also came with additional preprocessing for resting-state analysis. Censored volumes were determined by an FD threshold of 0.2 mm. The data were first demeaned and detrended, then a nuisance regression was performed removing the following factors, with censored volumes ignored: global signal, white matter mean signal, CSF mean signal, and motion regressors with the full Volterra expansion. The data were then interpolated across the censored volumes using least squares spectral estimation followed by bandpass filtering between 0.009 and 0.08 Hz. The censored volumes were removed in the final resultant data samples for analysis (for full description of processing pipeline, see Gordon et al., 2017).
Regions of interest (ROIs)
The initial analyses of 3-D connective field mapping were conducted using the visual areas as ROIs. For the early visual areas, we used the probabilistic visual atlas (Wang et al., 2015), selecting all voxels with any probability of either dorsal or ventral visual cortex, with no overlap between the two ROIs (Fig. 2A). To determine whether the finding from the overall visual cortex mask was a general property of early visual areas, we also defined more selective masks of the dorsal and ventral portions of V1, V2, and V3, each with probability >30% to reduce overlap between the masks. We then applied the same connective field mapping analyses across each visual region’s dorsal and ventral portions.
Group results of the 3-D connective field modeling in dorsal and ventral visual cortex using the Cambridge Buckner resting-state fMRI dataset. A, Examples of the resulting Gaussian model are shown for two seed voxels (red dots), plotted in red among other voxels (black circles) in dorsal visual cortex (red masked area in the right hemisphere), and their associated group level Gaussian fit (color scale ranged from blue to yellow) in ventral visual cortex (green masked area). For full visualization, each two column shows two plots of the seed voxel’s fitted map rotated by 180° from each other. B, Preferred location of connectivity of each seed voxel is plotted as a vector with the origin in the seed location, and the vector direction showing the fitted x0, y0, and z0 parameters. The vectors are colored coded with blue to red representing low to high Euclidean norm of the vector. While unclear in the current data, in higher resolution data, these vector fields are slightly deformed by the cortical surface (Extended Data Fig. 2-1). Extended Data Figure 2-2 shows the vector fields of dorsal and ventral visual cortex subregions. Further, we demonstrate the reliability of this result in Extended Data Figures 2-3, 2-4, 2-5. C, Group average of the Gaussian model’s SD parameter. Left, Each voxel within the seed region, dorsal visual cortex, is plotted with the data point’s color scaled by the SD parameter of their connective field model. Cooler to warmer colors represent smaller to larger SDs. Right, This plot shows the correlation between the anterior-posterior extent of the seed voxel and their associated SD parameter, suggesting that SD increases along more anterior voxels.
Extended Data Figure 2-1
A, Depiction of a single subject’s vector field from V1d to V1v plottted on that subject’s normalized anatomy. B, Zoomed in version of the same subject’s vector field shown in A. C, An average of 300 subject’s vector fields, accentuation the features seen in the vector field in B. D, Comparison of a voxels across and within sulcus walls. While both X and Z parameters are confounded by space, the Y parameter comparison is not, demonstrating a difference between voxels along a sulcus wall and voxels between sulcus walls. Download Figure 2-1, TIF file.
Extended Data Figure 2-2
The linear organization pattern of region-to-region functional connectivity is similar across early visual areas. More constrained ROI masks were selected to demonstrate that the connectivity patterns from dorsal to ventral portions of V1, V2, and V3 are constrained by the anterior-posterior axis, likely along the topographic eccentricity representation. The vector fields are displayed similarly as described in Figure 2B, with the red-blue color scale representing the Euclidean norm of each seed voxel’s vector. Download Figure 2-2, TIF file.
Extended Data Figure 2-3
Evaluation of the prediction of each seed voxel’s time course from the 3-D connective field model. Correlation of the actual time courses of voxels in the seed region (dorsal visual cortex) with their corresponding predicted time courses was compared to their correlation and all other predicted time courses. The distribution matched pairs of correlations are much tighter and shifted positively in comparison to that of the mismatched correlations. Download Figure 2-3, TIF file.
Extended Data Figure 2-4
Test-retest reliability examined using the IBATRT and MSC datasets. A, Correlation of each subject’s parameters for the V1d seed voxels in the first and second session. B, Correlation between the mean parameters for each of the sessions, the black dashed line is the least squares fit, while the light green dashed line is the identity line. C, The correlation between each MSC subject’s parameters for V1d seed voxels while incorporating different amount of data into each test-retest set, maxing out at 150 min of data within each set. With 150 min of data, the median individual subject test-retest correlation is 0.81. Download Figure 2-4, TIF file.
Extended Data Figure 2-5
Vector fields from individual subjects in the MSC dataset. The anterior posterior topographic organization of the vector fields is detectable in every subject, as denoted by the r value in each subject’s vector field plots showing the correlation between the y-location parameters and the y-space. Download Figure 2-5, TIF file.
In an additional analysis, we created slightly more lenient V1 mask selecting any voxels that have a >15% probability of being V1. We used this V1 ROI as a mapping region for the right fusiform face area (FFA) and the right parahippocampal place area (PPA), both FFA and PPA were derived using Neurosynth meta-analyses, with “faces” and “place” as keywords, respectively (Yarkoni et al., 2011).
For the whole-brain topographic connectivity analysis, we made use of a previously published whole-brain parcellation atlas of 400 regions. The parcels were generated by maximizing correlations within a parcel while minimizing local changes in correlations within a parcel (Schaefer et al., 2018). For time and computational feasibility, we only used the 200 parcels of the right hemisphere.
3-D connective field mapping and model parameters
The 3-D connective field mapping analysis has been released in the form of a MATLAB toolbox, with a simple GUI wrapper to help guide analysis (www.nitrc.org/projects/r2r_prf/).
For both the “seed” and “mapping” regions, the mean time course of the ROI was subtracted from each voxel’s time course, so as to reduce the influence of correlation at the mean signal level, although in practice the results are similar with or without removing the means. Then, for each voxel within the seed region, we calculated its correlation with every voxel within the mapping region and transformed the resulting correlation values using Fisher’s Z transformation. Each distribution of Fisher’s z values (for each seed voxel) was fitted with a 3-D isotropic Gaussian distribution (Fig. 1A):
Initial values for the x0, y0, and z0 parameters during the optimization procedure were the x-, y-, and z-locations of the maximum Fisher’s z value, and an arbitrarily small number (floating-point epsilon) for the SD. The optimization procedure had lower bounds of the smallest location parameters within the mapping region and the floating-point epsilon for the SD, while the upper bounds were the maximum location parameters and the smallest range of the three dimensions for the SD. The parameters of the Gaussian were estimated by minimizing the correlation distance between the Fisher’s z values and the Gaussian probability density function using the interior-point algorithm (Byrd et al., 2000), implemented by MATLAB’s fmincon function (MathWorks Inc, 1995–2019). Predicted time series for each voxel v in the seed region were estimated as the degree of overlap between the estimated Gaussian function and the activation pattern of the mapping region for each time point t (Fig. 1B):
Both the predicted time series and the actual data time series were z-scored to remove any arbitrary scaling factors. Fit was assessed by comparing the distribution of correlations between each voxel’s fitted model and the true data to the distribution of correlation between each voxel’s true data and every other voxel’s fitted model.
Metrics of region-to-region organizational patterns
Topographic organization of functional connectivity between the seed and mapping region was estimated as rank linearity: estimated using the Procrustes method by fitting the three rank-ordered space parameters of the Gaussian model to the three rank-ordered space parameters of the seed locations (Kendall, 1989). The 3-D estimated parameters from the 3-D connective field model where first rank ordered, and then a transformation matrix with translation, rotation, and scale components was estimated that minimizes the normalized sums of squares error (SSE) between the rank ordered parameters and their spatial origin. The complement of the normalized SSE between the transformed parameters and the actual seed locations was used as a measure of the degree of topographic connectivity between two regions.
In order to determine whether the topographic arrangement of a region’s functional connectivity is above and beyond the base topography expected due to chance (given BOLD data characteristics), we formulated a way to examine what it means for a region to be topographically organized. Given the probability of a voxel (in a volume of randomly distributed data) having a data rank order equal to its spatial rank [p(r)] is the inverse of the volume (V) of the seed:
Then, the expected rank linearity of a volume is the average probability of this occurrence across the volume, which is simply the inverse of the volume of the seed again:
then, understanding the effect of smoothing on this expected value is understanding the extent to which smoothing reduces the number of independent observations in the spatial field, as defined by the number of resels in the volume:
where FWHM is the estimated smoothness of the volume, and D is the dimensionality of the data (three in the case of fMRI data). Statistical inferences can be made by evaluating the 95% confidence intervals of this probability on a binomial distribution given the number of voxels in the volume.
To examine whether the process described reflects the behavior of our rank linearity estimations, we simulated volumes of randomly distributed data while manipulating both the number of voxels in the volume and the smoothness. First, we showed that the expected value of rank linearity is approximately equal to the probability of a rank, which is the inverse of the voxels in a volume (Extended Data Fig. 1-1A). Second, we found that the effect of smoothing follows from the approximate number of independent observations (i.e., number of resels) within the volume (with the 95% confidence intervals well approximated by evaluation on the binomial; Extended Data Fig. 1-1B). However, we noted that there seemed to be some divergence of the model from the simulated data at higher smoothing values. We then conducted more simulations to test whether the deviance was related to the total number of independent observations in the volume, by manipulating the number of voxels in the volume. If the deviance is due to the total number of independent observations (resels) then this data should diverge from the model with a lower smoothing FWHM in simulations with fewer voxels. We found that this was roughly the case [Extended Data Fig. 1-2A(volume = 729 voxels) vs Extended Data Fig. 1-2B(volume = 125 voxels)]. We selected a rule of thumb to flag inferences that may be inappropriate, where the number of resels in a ROI is less than or equal to 10.
In addition to topographic connectivity, we also estimated convergence of functional connectivity. Convergence is the logical counterpart to topographic connectivity, and was calculated using a 3-D form of the Kolmogorov–Smirnov (KS) test (Fasano and Franceschini, 1987). The convergence metric was defined as the deviance of the Gaussian parameters from uniform distribution across the voxels in the mapping region. This provides a relatively distribution insensitive measure of convergence (Fasano and Franceschini, 1987). To correct for regional differences in spatial correlations and number of voxels, we used the KS value corrected for the KS value at the 5% α level. The relationship between convergence factor and rank linearity is shown in Extended Data Figure 1-3.
Further, vector fields were constructed to visualize the connectivity organization between two regions, with each vector originating at each seed voxel, and the orientation and length of the vector determined by the centered Gaussian parameters, its preferred location in the mapping region in relation to the center of mass of the mapping region (Fig. 2B).
However, corticocortical relationships are likely to be obfuscated in 3-D space due to cortical folding. While previous attempts to examine this have aligned data to the cortical surface to avoid this problem, we did not use cortical sheets for several reasons. First and foremost, since the data are acquired in volumetric space, most operations aligning to a cortical surface model results in additional smoothing to the data (e.g., through averaging). In addition, unfolding the cortical sheet is an arbitrary process, with distortions added in different forms depending how it is done. Therefore, constructing objective 2-D coordinates is intractable. Our measures of topography provided novel information in the original space, but are likely influenced by the different folding patterns of the cortex to some extent. Therefore, we also visualize the relationship between regions by constructing a vector field. The vector field allows the researcher to observe when spatial relationships from one cortical region conform to the cortical surface, as the vector field should bend along sulci and gyri (for an example in a single subject, and group vector field on how vectors are distorted when constrained to the cortical sheet, see Extended Data Fig. 2-1).
We assessed test-retest reliability using the IBATRT and MSC datasets. For the IBATRT dataset, we examined the spatial correlation between the patterns of parameters in run 1 and run 2, within individual subjects and across session means. For the MSC dataset, we examined potential effects of added scan time to the stability of the 3-D. Gaussian parameters for individual subjects. We split each subjects’ sessions into two groups, a test and a retest group. We varied the number of sessions in each group, with a maximum of 5 session per group, and calculated the test-retest reliability for each subject. We also performed 3-D connective field mapping for each subject with all their sessions to demonstrate the stability of the group effect at the level of individual subjects.
Examining connectivity profiles in higher order visual regions
To study how the patterns of connectivity from early visual cortex to higher order visual regions, such as FFA and PPA (for ROI selection, see above, Regions of interest), may predict their complex response properties, we fit 3-D connective field models seeding from these areas, mapping into primary visual cortex. We compared convergence and topographic connectivity between PPA/FFA and V1, and compared the model parameters to those of the intrinsic convergence and topographic connectivity within V1.
We expected both regions to have higher convergence than the relationships between dorsal and ventral V1. Given the observed retinotopy in PPA (Arcaro et al., 2009), and the relatively weaker observed retinotopy in FFA (Saygin and Sereno, 2008), we also expected PPA to demonstrate a higher amount of topographic connectivity and a lower amount of convergence with V1 relative to FFA connectivity relationships with V1.
Whole-brain characterization of topographic functional connectivity
We performed 3-D connective field mapping on each unique pair between all possible pairs out of the 200 parcels in the right hemisphere of the Shaefer atlas for each subject in the MSC data. For each fitted pair, we calculated the rank linearity of connectivity using Procrustes analysis. We selected several seed regions to test whether topography is maintained through specific networks or generally across the brain. We selected several a priori networks and examined whether their topographic characteristics follow network like behavior, namely that their within-network topographic connectivity is stronger than between network topographic connectivity.
As a higher order summary, and to compare with previous whole-brain gradient decompositions (Margulies et al., 2016; Murray et al., 2018), we performed nonmetric multidimensional scaling on the rank linearity matrices from all parcel pairs to estimate the gradients of topographic connectivity across the right hemisphere. The far ends of these gradients were considered to represent which regions maintain linear information across the brain. We produced a two mode solution, on the basis of an elbow in the plot of the rank correlation of the produced distance and the observed distance matrices (Extended Data Fig. 6-1A), and visualized the gradients across the brain.
Lateral prefrontal cortex analysis
We selected the subset of parcels (see above, Regions of interest) within the lateral prefrontal cortex, and performed a similar gradient decomposition analysis specifically on prefrontal functional connectivity topography. We estimated two gradients (for rational, see Extended Data Fig. 6-1B) of the lateral prefrontal cortex and then examined whether the discovered organization reflects principles of prefrontal cortex organization described in the literature (Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Koechlin et al., 2003). For comparison, we further examined the gradients resulting voxel-wise functional connectivity (Extended Data Fig. 7-1).
Each participant of the MSC data sample also went through three tasks in the scanner, an incidental memory task in which participants judged various characteristics of faces, scenes, and words, a motor task in which participants were instructed to move various parts of their body, and a mixed block task design in which participants had to either discriminate between nouns and verbs, or coherently and incoherently moving dots, with a cue on the onset and offset of each block (Gordon et al., 2017). We used two contrasts to examine sensory specialization (memory: face – scene, mixed: visual – semantic discrimination) and one to examine abstract rules [mixed: discrimination (visual and semantic) – cue effects].
In order to examine how the organizational features of the lateral prefrontal cortex observed in the resting state scans relate to spatial patterns of task activation, we constructed three multiple regressions, one for each contrast, predicting task activation from each topographic gradient and average convergence.
Results
Mapping functional connectivity in the visual cortex
To replicate previous findings from 2-D modeling on the surface (Haak et al., 2013; Gravel et al., 2014), we first used 3-D connective field modeling to show that dorsal and ventral portions of the visual cortex have a topographic organization in their functional connectivity. Figure 2A illustrates the group average 3-D connective field maps in the ventral visual cortex for two selected seed voxels in the dorsal visual cortex using the Cambridge Buckner dataset. Figure 2B summarizes these results as a vector field in the seed region, with a vector originated from each seed voxel displaying the location of its preferred connectivity as the relative displacement from the center of the mapping region (Fig. 2B). This vector field of the dorsal visual cortex revealed a strong connectivity mapping to the ventral visual cortex along the predicted anterior-posterior organization (Arcaro et al., 2015), which was also supported quantitatively by the high correlation between the seed’s y-location and the mean modeled y-location of the Gaussian fit in the mapping region, with r(394) = 0.71, p = 1.63 × 10−62 (r = 0.83, with outliers removed). The spread or SD parameter of the Gaussian model also increased along the seed region’s y-axis (Fig. 2C, left), which was supported quantitatively by the correlation between the seed’s y-location and the modeled SD parameter, with r(394) = 0.61, p = 5.45 × 10−42 (Fig. 2C, right). This relationship was similarly evident between the dorsal and ventral portions of V1, V1, and V3, using a set of more restricted masks (Extended Data Fig. 2-2). These results together suggested that the inter-regional connectivity in early visual areas mirror the organization of eccentricity representation, with region-to-region connectivity ordered spatially along similar eccentricity locations, and the spread of that connectivity potentially following the increase in receptive field sizes toward more anterior visual cortex. While the group averaged data can provide qualitative aspects of the organization of regions, the averaging procedure can introduce various biases (and average out individual differences to produce an optimistically simple output).
To quantify the organization at an individual subject level, we first quantified whether or not these measures are stable at the individual subject level data, and further we moved to a data sample that has much longer BOLD collections to ensure high-quality estimations (MSC). The median test-retest reliability within subjects increases with more scan time per subject, with the 150 min in each split leading to a test-retest reliability of 0.81 (Extended Data Fig. 2-4C). A vector field of dorsal-ventral connectivity mapping for the visual cortex was generated for each individual and each session, and the anterior-posterior relationship was observed in every subject (Extended Data Fig. 2-5). Quantitatively, the correlation between y-location modeled Gaussian parameter and seed y-location was high: ranging from 0.5 to 0.72 across the 10 individuals. In addition, topographic connectivity estimates for the entire dorsal and ventral visual cortex for each MSC individual ranged between 13.77% and 18.27% (all pFWE’s < 0.05). Similarly, topographic connectivity estimates of V1d→V1v were highly replicated in each MSC subject, ranging from 24.62% to 34.71% (only one session of MSC02 and one session of MSC08 > αFWE).
We also evaluated the model validity and stability in several ways. First, we examined the distribution of correlations between the predicted time courses from the model and the actual BOLD time courses (Fig. 1B) for each voxel in the large dorsal visual cortex mask (Extended Data Fig. 2-3, left). The distribution of r values across all subjects and all their correspondent time courses when using the matching prediction was strictly positive, with a mean of 0.55, whereas the distribution of correlations produced by nonmatched pairs is much wider with a mean of close to zero (Extended Data Fig. 2-3, right). Second, also evaluated the stability of the 3-D connective field model within individual subjects and across mean results for the dorsal and ventral visual cortex, using the IBATRT dataset. For each subject, the estimated y-location parameters from the first and second runs were correlated across voxels (Extended Data Fig. 2-4A). The resulting distribution of r values had a median of 0.60, demonstrating moderate to good reliability at the individual subject level. At the group level, the mean parameters for each run correlated across voxels strongly, with r = 0.98 (Extended Data Fig. 2-4B).
Connectivity bias in higher-order visual areas
Higher order visual regions have been characterized as complex filters of early visual region information (Van Essen and Gallant, 1994), with this type of model shown to predict FFA activation during a task (Kay and Yeatman, 2017). We thus examined to what extent the pattern of region-to-region functional connectivity reflect this filtering process. Using the MSC data, we applied the 3-D connective field mapping method to model the pattern of right FFA connectivity with V1 and compared its connectivity profile with that of PPA, a region which is selective to stimuli that have more peripherally distinctive features. Because of initial poor r2 of the Gaussian fits, we combined the sessions into two distinct sessions, allowing for better estimation across each. In comparison to the PPA, the FFA showed more connectivity with posterior and lateral V1 (t(9) = −7.04, p = 6.04 × 10−5 and t(9) = 3.36, p = 0.008, respectively; Fig. 3A). This is consistent the FFA receiving information from V1 areas with more foveal receptive fields. For visualization, we plotted group averaged vectors from each seed region to V1. From these vector fields, FFA seems to share more convergent and less topographic connectivity with V1 (Fig. 3B). We quantified this in individual subjects and compared across regions, with V1d to V1v connectivity for reference. When compared with PPA, the FFA has both significantly higher convergence and significantly lower topographic connectivity with V1 (t(9) = 5.25, p = 5.27 × 10−4 and t(9) = −4.33, p < 0.002, respectively; Fig. 3C). PPA, when compared with measures within V1, in turn, shows lower linearity and higher convergence.
Pattern of functional connectivity between higher order visual regions and V1 corresponds well with their visual response properties from previous studies. A, Compared with the PPA, FFA showed a more posteriorly located and narrower range of connectivity with V1. Thin lines represent kernel density plots of individual subjects of the MSC dataset, while thick lines represent the kernel density plot of the group average data. B, Another representation of the biases of FFA to V1 connectivity (left) in comparison to PPA to V1 connectivity (right). The black points represent voxels within the seed region and the green points represent voxels within the mapping region, with the vectors connecting them represent the seed voxels’ preferred connectivity location along the mapping region. The mean vector fields provide information about quantitative features of the connectivity (such as the relatively more focal and mixing connectivity seen in FFA vs PPA) but to quantify these results requires computation on individual subject vector fields. C, Estimations of topographic connectivity show higher topography with V1 maintained in PPA, and higher convergence with V1 in FFA. Both of these metrics are shown next to dorsal and ventral V1 (within V1) estimates for reference.
Topographic connectivity organization across the cortex
While the early visual system demonstrated large topographic relationships in functional connectivity between regions, a fundamental question left unanswered is whether this is a special property of the early visual cortex or it is a general motif of corticocortical functional connectivity. To address this issue, we calculated the linearity of connectivity between all unique pairs of the 200 right hemisphere parcels in the Schaefer parcellation. We used the MSC dataset for this analysis to ensure stable parameter estimates, given the long scan time for each session. For each subject of the MSC dataset, the average percentage of connections that surpassed α = 0.05 threshold across all sessions and all pairs of parcels was 20.60% (range across subjects: 13.30 − 25.69%). Note that the subject with the lowest number of significant topographic relationships, MSC08 (with 13.30% of connections surpassing 0.05 α), was identified by the original investigators to have a significant proportion of time in a drowsy state, affecting this individual’s BOLD data (Gordon et al., 2017). Therefore, on average, each brain region maintains a detectable topographic relationship with ∼20% of the rest of the brain, at a liberal threshold for detection. Eliminating parcels which likely do not follow the assumptions of our model (due to low number of resels) increases this average percentage (to 23.54%, ranging from 14.25% to 29.64%).
Figure 4 illustrates the linear organization for three selected seed regions’ connectivity with other regions across the entire right hemisphere. The flatmaps show a clear segregation of linearity values within and across the different networks, exemplified by seeding from anterior and posterior visual cortex demonstrating stronger linearity with other visual areas, middle frontal gyrus demonstrating stronger linearity with lateral prefrontal and parietal regions, and lower linearity between regions from different networks. In Extended Data Figure 4-1, we present MSC01’s session by session seed rank linearity maps, presented with -log(p) values to demonstrate the stability of the statistical inference.
Topographic connectivity as a general rule of functional connectivity between cortical regions. Topographic connectivity estimates are shown from three seed regions (shown as asterisk in each map), posterior visual cortex, anterior visual cortex, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The color scale from darker to brighter color represents lower to higher topographic connectivity for the seed regions’ connective field in the other brain regions. This whole-brain illustration demonstrates that while topographic connectivity is a general phenomenon for region-to-region connectivity, it is maintained within specific networks, suggesting function in maintaining integrity of information transfer across regions within a network. Extended Data Figure 4-1 shows each of these seeded maps in a single subject of the MSC data.
Extended Data Figure 4-1
MSC subject 01’s seeded topographic maps thresholded at p < 0.001 for each session. Color maps are scaled on the basis of -log(p) values. Topographic maps of functional connectivity seeded from anterior visual cortex (top), posterior visual cortex (middle), and DLPFC (bottom) all demonstrate consistent patterns of linearity within each of their networks across each session. Maps are overlaid on a flatmap aligned with the HCP fslr32k atlas. Download Figure 4-1, TIF file.
To visualize and quantify the network basis of topographic organization of region-to-region connectivity, we constructed a graph representation of our rank linearity maps (Fig. 5A). We then quantified within and between network linearity for each of the 10 MSC subjects across three different networks (VisCent, VentralAttenB, DorsalAttenB). For each of the six possible comparisons, the within network rank linearity was significantly higher than the between network rank linearity (t(9) = 9.52, t(9) = 3.80, t(9) = 9.26, t(9) = 3.19, t(9) = 9.75, and t(9) = 6.75; p = 5.38 × 10−6, p = 4.00 × 10−3, p = 6.76 × 10−6, p = 1.10 × 10−2, p = 4.40 × 10−6, and p = 8.33 × 10−5; Fig. 5B).
Network analysis. A, Graph description of our topographic network analysis using the 17 network labels from the Schaefer atlas. Node position was determined by performing classical multidimensional scaling on the full linearity matrix, and then an edge was drawn if it surpassed a linearity threshold of 20%. Node size was scaled linearity in relation to the node’s degree in the thresholded network. B, Quantification of the network like organization of linearity with three example networks, visual (central), ventral attention B, and dorsal attention B. The within network linearity is universally higher than the between network linearity in these three networks, as can be seen in A by the relative closeness of nodes within a network. Note: Schaefer atlas used for network labels (for more details, see Schaefer et al., 2018).
To examine whether there are any higher order (supranetwork) organizational features of topographic connectivity, nonmetric multidimensional scaling was applied to produce two dimensions that maximally reproduce the nonlinearity matrix (as a distance matrix) the 200 parcels in the right hemisphere (Extended Data Fig. 6-1; Fig. 6). The resulting primary mode was similar to large scale whole-brain decompositions observed in previous studies of normal functional connectivity and genetic expression data (Margulies et al., 2016; Murray et al., 2018). This mode described a segregation lower order unimodal regions and higher order multimodal regions, with the negative end exemplified by visual and somatomotor networks and the positive end exemplified by default mode network. The second mode seemed to be a fractionation of the primary mode, potentially representing communication of information across these lower order/higher order boundaries.
Gradient decomposition of whole-brain linearity (for rational for number of estimated gradients, see Extended Data Fig. 6-1). Whole-brain region-to-region linearity matrix was decomposed using nonmetric multidimensional scaling in order to examine organizational patterns of topographic connectivity across the brain. The gradients are color scaled from red to blue, with red-yellow colors representing one side of the gradient, and blue-green colors representing the other side of the gradient. Top, First whole-brain linearity gradient, demonstrates a segregation of unimodal sensory areas and multimodal association areas. Bottom, Second whole-brain linearity gradient, the fractionated nature suggests potential integration of information across unimodal sensory areas and multimodal association areas.
Extended Data Figure 6-1
Determination of the number of gradients in the linearity decomposition for the whole brain (A) and the lateral frontal cortex (B). We examined the rank correlation between the distance matrix and the MDS estimated distance matrix, and visually identified an elbow by examining where the largest spike lies in the derivative of the correlations with respect to component number. In both cases, this ended up being two components. Download Figure 6-1, TIF file.
Connectivity organization of the lateral prefrontal cortex
After illustrating the ubiquity of topographic organization of functional connectivity across visual cortex and large networks, we specifically examined how gradients of linearity express in a highly multimodal structure such as the lateral prefrontal cortex. We found two gradients (Extended Data Fig. 6-1B). Gradient 1 demonstrates a rostral-caudal axis that segregates posterior middle frontal gyrus/inferior precentral sulcus and anterior inferior frontal gyrus/frontal pole (Fig. 7A, top left). Gradient 2 demonstrates a diagonal axis, segregating posterior superior frontal gyrus and anterior inferior frontal gyrus (Fig. 7A, top right). These two topographic connectivity gradients closely resemble the two prominent frameworks described in the literature to simplify the overall functional organization of the frontal cortex. One is often referred to as the domain specialization framework, suggesting that the dorsal and ventral lateral prefrontal cortices are preferentially organized to process spatial and nonspatial (e.g., objects) information, respectively, due to their differential connectivity with the dorsal and ventral visual streams (Goldman-Rakic, 1987). The second is an increase in abstraction along the rostral-caudal axis, with the more rostral regions linked to more abstract facets of behavioral control (e.g., sensory perception → context → rule; Koechlin et al., 2003). In addition, we performed nonmentric MDS on the standard voxel-wise resting-state functional connectivity of the lateral prefrontal cortex, and found two gradient decompositions that mainly segregate middle frontal gyrus and inferior frontal gyrus from the rest of the frontal cortex (Extended Data Fig. 7-1).
Patterns of functional connectivity within lateral prefrontal cortex. A, The decomposition of the bivariate rank-linearity of the lateral prefrontal cortex revealed two gradients. Gradient 1 demonstrates an organization from posterior middle frontal gyrus/inferior precentral sulcus to anterior inferior frontal gyrus/frontopolar cortex, while gradient 2 demonstrates an organization from posterior superior frontal cortex to anterior inferior frontal gyrus. The average convergence factor demonstrates a peak along the frontal pole (for similar gradient decomposition of voxel-wise functional connectivity, see Extended Data Fig. 7-1). B, Three different contrasts were examined to correlate to frontal organizational features: face-scene to examine visual specialization, visual-semantic discrimination to examine sensory specialization, and discrimination-rule to examine levels of abstraction. C, First-order correlation demonstrating the most highly associated organizational feature with each contrast (for full regression analysis results, see Table 1).
Extended Data Figure 7-1
Lateral frontal gradients estimated from voxel-wise resting-state fMRI data. Gradient 1 segregates middle frontal gyrus from the rest of lateral frontal cortex, while gradient 2 segregates inferior frontal gyrus from the rest of the lateral frontal cortex. These derived gradients are qualitatively different than the gradients derived from the topographic gradient estimates, demonstrating that these metrics provide unique information beyond the typical resting-state analyses. Download Figure 7-1, TIF file.
We examined whether the average task activation patterns for sensory specialization and level of abstraction from the MSC data sample (Fig. 7B) can be predicted by these organizational features. We found that a linear model with each topographic gradient and convergence predicts each of the activation patterns from each of the contrasts (face-scene: R 2 = 0.37, p = 0.003 0.01; visual-semantic: R 2 = 0.24, p = 0.046; discrimination-rule: R 2 = 0.41, p = 0.002). The strongest predictor of face-scene contrast was the gradient 2 value (β = 0.55, p = 8.74 × 10−4), while the strongest predictor for the discrimination-rule contrast was the gradient 1 value (β = −0.56, p = 6.34 × 10−4; Fig. 7C; Table 1). This supports the above interpretation of the two gradients of topographic connectivity as relating to domain specialization and abstraction, respectively.
Organizational features of lateral frontal cortex relate to task activation
Discussion
Using a 3-D Gaussian modeling approach with whole-brain resting-state fMRI data, we found that closely connected cortical regions exhibit a relatively strong topographic organization of their functional connectivity. This topographic organization of region-to-region functional connectivity was highly reliable in early visual regions, following an order along the anterior-posterior axis for eccentricity representation, as predicted by the literature (Haak et al., 2013; Gravel et al., 2014; Arcaro et al., 2015). In higher order visual regions, such as the FFA and PPA, the estimated connectivity profile seemed to reflect complex filtering of information, such that FFA connectivity to V1 produced the hypothesized patterns of bias towards the part of V1 that has more foveal representations. Further, the PPA shows a more maintained topography with V1 in comparison to PPA. These results are consistent with previous attempts to compare these two higher order regions using a different multivariate method to estimate voxel level patterns of connectivity from one region to another (Baldassano et al., 2012), where it was found that FFA shows connectivity bias to more foveal resources, while PPA shows a connectivity bias to more eccentric resources. In the whole brain, topographic connectivity again emerged as a primary organizational feature for functional connectivity among regions within large-scale networks, including the default mode and frontoparietal networks, while such relationships were much weaker between regions across different networks. Lastly, we found that the topographic and convergence patterns of connectivity in the lateral prefrontal cortex supports the two prevailing frameworks of prefrontal cortex functional organization, domain specialization and abstraction. Our overall findings thus suggest a ubiquitous nature of topography in spatial organization of functional connectivity among closely related functional regions in the human brain.
Eccentricity-dependent functional connectivity in early visual cortex as a special case of linear communication
We found that the early visual cortex demonstrates orderly region-to-region functional connectivity that maintains the organization of eccentricity representation across regions. This result replicates previous work that used resting state fMRI data and phase-locking fMRI task data in studies of retinotopic organization of the visual system (Haak et al., 2013; Gravel et al., 2014; Arcaro et al., 2015). The orderly functional connectivity likely contributes to the maintenance of retinotopic organization across the posterior cortical regions for multiple levels of visual processing. We also demonstrated that this motif breaks down for connectivity between early and higher order visual cortices (Fig. 4). The pattern of FFA connectivity demonstrates predicted qualities given its receptive field properties, that is, its convergence and selection of foveal resources (Kanwisher et al., 1997). These findings on region-to-region connectivity patterns provide a potential structure for understanding what information is maintained across regions, which would be useful for future modeling of information transformation and transfer from one region to another region (Thivierge and Marcus, 2007; Basti et al., 2018).
The topographic organization of functional connectivity observed in the early visual cortices seems to be a special case of a larger phenomenon. Our findings suggest that such a spatial layout of functional connectivity is a probable general mode of communication across the brain (Figs. 5, 6), likely at least partially for the purposes of high-fidelity information transmission across regions within a functional network (Thivierge and Marcus, 2007). In the visual cortex, coincidental activity that drives plasticity provides a potential mechanism for the generation and maintenance a spatially linear organization of communication between nearby regions (Schoen et al., 1990; Elliott and Shadbolt, 1996; Catalano et al., 1997). However, activity dependent synaptic pruning is not the only mechanism that drives organization in early visual cortex. Recent studies have suggested a form of proto-organization driven by genetic expression and their products (e.g., morphogens) in animal models of development and in humans (Cheng et al., 1995; Crowley and Katz, 2000; Arcaro and Livingstone, 2017). This mechanism of organization developing from molecular gradients is more widely observed, pertaining to many brain systems. For example, molecular signaling has been shown to be responsible for organization of thalamocortical connections in and ordered spatial gradient, with Emx2 and Pax2 regulating the anterior-posterior organization of cortical fields and their thalamic afferents (Bishop et al., 2000). Using 3-D connective field modeling, we found a strong linear organization in lateral prefrontal areas despite a lack of clear or consistent retinotopy in these areas (Hagler and Sereno, 2006; Kastner et al., 2007). Strong linearity in the absence of retinotopy suggests a potential link with proto-organization. Indeed, our gradient decomposition of linearity resembles gradients of genetic expression (Burt et al., 2018; Murray et al., 2018) providing potential convergent evidence for this interpretation.
Topographic connectivity and computation within and across large-scale brain networks
Spatially linear organization of input-output relationships is thought to maintain information across levels of processing, allowing for precise action on continuous sensory information (Thivierge and Marcus, 2007). Topographic organization and projections from topographic space has been postulated to produce ordered abstract space as shown in connectionist models (Tinsley, 2009). With a continuous space of abstract relations, complex information transformation in abstract space can be achieved by the same general mechanism as filtering of visual information. For example, this is the proposed mechanism underlying saliency map models, where information from many independent channels are combined topographically to represent the overall visual salience of an image (Itti and Koch, 2000; Roggeman et al., 2010). These saliency map models have been successful in predicting eye movements to complex visual stimuli, attentional load responses in parietal cortex, and even working memory capacity (Foulsham and Underwood, 2008; Knops et al., 2014).
With the simple assumption information is spatially segregated, a tractable form of general computation in the brain is through spatial biases in connectivity, which would subsume the above description of topographic connectivity. Spatial biases can, however, have vastly different scopes. With distinct information converging onto a neuron giving rise to many potential combinations of that information, it has been shown that the exact combination is at least partially determined by spatial biases in convergence along the dendritic compartment (Taylor et al., 2000; London and Häusser, 2005). Higher order spatial biases, from cortical column to cortical column, may be responsible for higher order computation. Previous work has examined information transformation more directly, estimating linear transformations that map the representational dissimilarity matrices from one region to another (Basti et al., 2018). Our 3-D connective field model provides an appropriate description of the proposed mechanisms for these transformations, allowing for critical tests of this theory in observational fMRI data.
Organization of the lateral prefrontal cortex using spatial connectivity metrics
Gradient decomposition of topographic connectivity of the prefrontal functional connectivity demonstrated evidence for two gradients of functional organization. Gradient 2, particularly, seemed to predict task based activational differences between face and scene perception. Previous studies have consistently found differential projections from dorsal and ventral visual stream nodes to dorsal and ventral prefrontal cortex, respectively (Kawamura and Naito, 1984; Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Cavada and Goldman-Rakic, 1989). Indeed, scene processing, while involving specialized regions of the ventral visual pathway, does seem to also involve the dorsal stream (Aminoff and Tarr, 2015). This segregation of dorsal and ventral pathway input into the prefrontal cortex has been supported both structurally and functionally in humans (Takahashi et al., 2013). This dorsal-ventral segregation of the prefrontal cortex has been shown to be differentially responsible for spatial and object working memory (Constantinidis and Qi, 2018). Indeed, inactivation of dorsal prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory, but not object working memory (Chafee and Goldman-Rakic, 2000; Suzuki and Gottlieb, 2013; Clark et al., 2014). Thus, the differential input from dorsal and ventral streams has functional consequences, leading to the segregation of downstream dependence of higher order functions depending on the type of information being operated on.
While a prominent perspective of prefrontal organization, the domain specialization hypothesis of lateral prefrontal functional organization is not the only studied organizational principle. Accumulating evidence shows another prominent gradient of prefrontal functional organization along the anterior-posterior axis related to the level of abstraction, from the posterior representation of sensorimotor response selection to the more abstract representation of task goals/rules in the anterior portions of the prefrontal cortex (Koechlin et al., 2003; Kouneiher et al., 2009; Azuar et al., 2014). By varying the rules and contexts of various tasks, it was found that areas related to the stimulus response mapping were located more caudally, while areas related to contexts and rules were more rostral (Koechlin et al., 2003; Badre and D'Esposito, 2007; Azuar et al., 2014). While we found that convergence follows a posterior to anterior gradient, there was no evidence that it related to the rule contrast in the mixed task. Patterns of convergence in the lateral prefrontal cortex were, however, associated with different sensory information, likely relating to differential processing demands required. Topographic gradient 2, on the other hand, did strongly related to the rule contrast in the mixed task, suggesting that topographic connectivity within the lateral prefrontal cortex delineates multiple pathways of organization.
In sum, on the basis of these new metrics of the spatial profile of functional connectivity, our data support both frameworks of functional organization for the lateral prefrontal cortex. However, based on these results it may be the case that these two functionally defined organizations are a result of the same underlying neural organization, driven by the differential topographic input within the lateral frontal cortex. This is consistent with the previous literature examining functional connectivity which has found evidence for both dorsal-ventral and rostral-caudal organizations in the lateral prefrontal cortex (Blumenfeld et al., 2013; Schumacher et al., 2019).
Limitations and future directions
Our estimations of topographic organization of connectivity between across regions were likely subjected to parcel selection, which is a limitation for this type of analysis. Given the multitude of parcellations and the multitude of methods used to obtain parcellations, the results could look slightly different if other justified parcellations were used. It is likely that our estimations of linearity would be even stronger with “true” parcels, given that more specification in early visual cortex actually boosted the estimated linearity (from ∼40% to ∼60%). In addition, the 3-D. Gaussian model itself also has limitations and makes certain assumptions. For one, it assumes a single focal point of connectivity from one region to another, which is probably overly simplified. However, the final R 2 of the optimization procedure provides a marker for the fit of the model to the data; if these values are too low in the analysis, it would be inappropriate to make any conclusions about the fitted values. As the R 2 of the model tended to increase with more scan time, a large proportion of the misfit is likely due to noise. With the 30-min sessions of the MSC data, the modal R 2 tended around 0.30–0.60, indicating a reasonable fit. Gaussian mixture models can be developed in the future to account some conditions of misfit, such as regions with multiple maxima.
It is important to note that the 3-D connective field method has a high dependence on high signal-to-noise ratio in single voxels, as opposed to the more conventional analyses that either use signal averaging within ROIs or smoothing to increase the signal-to-noise ratio. Because of this, higher resolution datasets may suffer from less stability due to the increase of proportion of thermal noise in each voxel (Edelstein et al., 1986), although the impact on signal-to-noise ratio can potentially be counterbalanced by a reduction in physiological noise (Bodurka et al., 2007). Further studies are needed to test our approach on higher resolution data. Optimally, the goal is to achieve a reliable connective field mapping at the size of functional columns of the ROIs, which may be feasible in the frontal cortex, where columns may be as large as 800–900 μm (Hirata and Sawaguchi, 2008; Masse et al., 2017).
Conclusions
By applying 3-D connective field modeling on resting-state fMRI data, we demonstrated that topographic organization of functional connectivity is likely common mode of communication across the cerebral cortex. This pattern of connectivity organization is most evident within the same functional network module, with the whole brain illustrating a primary gradient of linearity of functional connectivity comparable to two different data sources: resting-state functional connectivity and genetic expression. By the same principles, the topographic connectivity of the lateral prefrontal cortex seemed to be organized around two axes: posterior superior frontal gyrus → anterior inferior frontal gyrus and posterior middle frontal gyrus → anterior inferior frontal gyrus/frontopolar cortex. Both these axes segregated spatial patterns of activation relating to domain specialization (spatial→object) and abstraction (S-R→rule). These findings show that deriving finer scale (voxel level) spatial organization of region-to-region connectivity in volumetric space seems to be useful in resting-state fMRI data. Vector field representations of region-to-region connectivity has the potential to be an informative visualization for information transfer and transformation from one region to another, with topographic connectivity and convergence of connectivity as the quantitative metrics, especially if the voxel size can be reduced to the size of a cortical column in the ROIs.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.