Abstract
The preBötzinger complex (preBötC) generates the rhythm and rudimentary motor pattern for inspiratory breathing movements. Here, we test “burstlet” theory (Kam et al., 2013a), which posits that low amplitude burstlets, subthreshold from the standpoint of inspiratory bursts, reflect the fundamental oscillator of the preBötC. In turn, a discrete suprathreshold process transforms burstlets into full amplitude inspiratory bursts that drive motor output, measurable via hypoglossal nerve (XII) discharge in vitro. We recap observations by Kam and Feldman in neonatal mouse slice preparations: field recordings from preBötC demonstrate bursts and concurrent XII motor output intermingled with lower amplitude burstlets that do not produce XII motor output. Manipulations of excitability affect the relative prevalence of bursts and burstlets and modulate their frequency. Whole-cell and photonic recordings of preBötC neurons suggest that burstlets involve inconstant subsets of rhythmogenic interneurons. We conclude that discrete rhythm- and pattern-generating mechanisms coexist in the preBötC and that burstlets reflect its fundamental rhythmogenic nature.
Significance Statement
Breathing movements depend on a neural rhythm and rudimentary motor pattern. Microcircuits of the brainstem preBötzinger complex (preBötC) produce both, but by unknown mechanisms that prove refractory to canonical explanations. Inspired by an unconventional proposal that rhythm and motor pattern are separable processes (Kam et al., 2013a), we replicated their findings that rhythmicity in local preBötC microcircuits can occur independently without obligatory neural bursts that generate motor output. The rhythm is voltage dependent and the constituent interneurons change from cycle to cycle. These results suggest that breathing rhythm is attributable to recurrent excitation among interneurons that discretely trigger neural bursts and motor output. The preBötC, previously considered uniquely rhythmogenic, contains rhythm- and pattern-generating microcircuits.
Introduction
Breathing is a vital rhythmic behavior. Inspiration, the inexorable active phase of the breathing cycle, originates from the preBötzinger complex (preBötC) in the lower medulla (Smith et al., 1991; Del Negro et al., 2018). preBötC interneurons generate rhythmic activity and project to cranial and spinal premotor and motor neurons that drive inspiratory muscles. However, the neural mechanisms that give rise to inspiratory rhythm and motor pattern are unclear. These mechanisms can be studied in reduced preparations that isolate the preBötC and cranial hypoglossal (XII) inspiratory motor circuits, and thus provide an experimentally advantageous minimal breathing-related microcircuit in vitro. Here, we disentangle neural mechanisms intrinsic to the preBötC that engender both inspiratory rhythm and fundamental aspects of the motor pattern.
We stipulate: preBötC bursts propel inspiratory activity to premotor and motor neurons thus they are important for motor pattern. However, are preBötC bursts rhythmogenic? The following observations cast doubt on that premise. The magnitude of inspiratory bursts (in the preBötC and motor output) can be diminished while only minimally affecting their frequency (Johnson et al., 2001; Del Negro et al., 2002; Peña et al., 2004; Pace et al., 2007). Also, manipulating network excitability affects the frequency, but not the magnitude of preBötC inspiratory bursts and motor output (Del Negro et al., 2009; Sun et al., 2019). It thus appears that two discrete phenomena emanate from the preBötC: a fundamental rhythm (whose frequency is adjustable) and a rudimentary pattern consisting of bursts that drive motor output. These appear to be separable processes as codified by Kam and Feldman in their burstlet hypothesis (Kam et al., 2013a; Feldman and Kam, 2015). To describe one cycle, preBötC neurons experience a ramp-like depolarization lasting 100–400 ms, which reflects recurrent excitatory synaptic activity among constituent rhythmogenic neurons (Smith et al., 1990; Rekling et al., 1996; Kam et al., 2013b). This is called the preinspiratory phase because it precedes, and ordinarily leads to, the inspiratory burst. However, Kam and Feldman showed that preinspiratory activity can be divorced from inspiratory bursts by lowering the neural excitability. What often remains is preBötC network activity matching that of the preinspiratory phase but absent the burst; they dubbed these events burstlets (Kam et al., 2013a).
Here, we test the burstlet hypothesis of inspiratory rhythm and pattern generation by explicitly or conceptually repeating Kam et al's (2013a) experiments. We, too, detected preBötC burstlets absent XII output, which were distinct from larger amplitude preBötC bursts accompanied by XII output. Manipulations that lower neural excitability increase the prevalence of burstlets relative to bursts. Composite preBötC rhythm depends on cellular excitability because manipulations of external K+ concentration control its frequency. Intracellular recordings and photonic imaging of preBötC inspiratory neurons demonstrate that the burstlets occur in subsets of preBötC neurons, not the entire rhythmogenic population that participates in bursts. Our results also support the fundamental tenet of burstlet theory that preinspiratory activity and burstlets reflect a common rhythmogenic mechanism, and that a threshold process causes burstlets (i.e., preinspiratory activity) to trigger bursts and subsequent motor output. These results imply that pattern generation, although a distinct process from rhythm generation, starts from the preBötC core microcircuit.
Materials and Methods
The Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at William & Mary approved these protocols, which conform to the policies of the Office of Laboratory Animal Welfare (National Institutes of Health) and the guidelines of the National Research Council (National Research Council, 2011). Mice were housed in colony cages on a 14/10 h light/dark cycle with controlled humidity and temperature at 23°C and were fed ad libitum on a standard commercial mouse diet (Teklad Global Diets, Envigo) with free access to water.
Mice
preBötC field recording experiments employed CD-1 mice (Charles River). Whole-cell recordings employed CD-1 mice as well as mice with Cre-dependent expression of fluorescent Ca2+ indicator GCaMP6f dubbed Ai148 by the Allen Institute (RRID:IMSR_JAX:030328; Daigle et al., 2018). We crossed homozygous Dbx1Cre (Bielle et al., 2005) females with Ai148 males. We refer to their offspring as Dbx1;Ai148 mice. Newborn Dbx1;Ai148 pups express GCaMP6f in neurons derived from progenitors that express the embryonic transcription factor developing brain homeobox 1 (Dbx1).
Slice preparations
Neonatal mice (P0–P4) of both sexes were anesthetized by hypothermia and killed by thoracic transection. Brainstems were removed in cold artificial CSF (ACSF) containing the following: 124 mM NaCl, 3 mM KCl, 1.5 mM CaCl2, 1 mM MgSO4, 25 mM NaHCO3, 0.5 mM NaH2PO4, and 30 mM dextrose, which we aerated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2. Brainstems were then glued to an agar block with the rostral side up. We cut a single 450- to 500-µm-thick transverse medullary slice with the preBötC at the rostral surface. The position of the preBötC was benchmarked according to neonatal mouse preBötC atlases (Ruangkittisakul et al., 2011, 2014).
Electrophysiology
Slices were held in place and perfused with ACSF (∼28° C) at 2–4 ml min−1 in a recording chamber on a fixed-stage upright microscope. The external K+ concentration, i.e., [K+]o, in the ACSF was initially raised to 9 mM, which facilitates robust rhythm and motor output in slices (Funk and Greer, 2013).
Population activity from preBötC interneurons and XII motor neurons was recorded using suction electrodes fabricated from borosilicate glass pipettes (OD: 1.2 mm, ID: 0.68 mm). preBötC field recordings were obtained by placing the suction electrode over the rostral face of the preBötC at the surface of the slice. XII motor output was recorded from XII nerve rootlets, which are retained in slices. Signals were amplified by 20,000, band pass filtered at 0.3–1 kHz, and then RMS smoothed using a differential amplifier (Dagan Instruments). Smoothed signals were used for display and quantitative analyses.
We used an EPC-10 patch-clamp amplifier (HEKA Instruments) for whole-cell current-clamp recordings. Patch pipettes were fabricated from borosilicate glass (OD: 1.5 mm, ID: 0.86 mm) to have tip resistance of 4–6 MΩ. The patch pipette solution contained the following: 140 mM K-gluconate, 5 mM NaCl, 0.1 mM EGTA, 10 mM HEPES, 2 mM Mg-ATP, and 0.3 mM Na3-GTP. We added 50 µM Alexa Fluor 488 hydrazide dye (A10436, Life Technologies) for visualization after whole-cell dialysis. Whole-cell recordings were made from preBötC inspiratory neurons selected visually based on rhythmic fluorescence changes in Dbx1;Ai148 mice. In CD-1 mice, we only collected whole-cell data from preBötC inspiratory neurons that were active in sync with XII motor output. Membrane potential trajectories were low-pass filtered at 1 kHz and digitally recorded at 4 kHz using a PowerLab data acquisition system, which includes a 16-bit analog-to-digital converter and LabChart v7 software (ADInstruments).
We modified [K+]o in the ACSF from 9 to 3 mM to modulate the network excitability. Each trial consisted of a sequence of non-contiguous [K+]o levels selected randomly in descending order.
Low-amplitude activity in preBötC field recordings was classified as a burstlet if it met these two criteria: the peak of preBötC activity exceeded the mean of the distribution of baseline noise by 2*SD, and there was negligible concurrent activity in the XII root recording. The mean and SD of baseline noise were obtained by sampling every data point during a sliding 120-s window, constructing a histogram of baseline noise, and fitting that distribution with a Gaussian function to obtain the mean and SD.
A sigh burst in the preBötC field recording was distinguished from an inspiratory burst if it met these three criteria: the area of the putative sigh burst exceeded the mean area of all inspiratory bursts by one SD; the cycle period of the putative sigh bursts measured 1–4 min and not outside this range; and the putative sigh burst was followed by a prolonged inter-event interval >1.3 times the average inspiratory cycle time for six consecutive cycles preceding a putative sigh burst (Lieske et al., 2000; Ruangkittisakul et al., 2008; Borrus et al., 2019).
For field recordings, we measured the amplitude (amp) of the preBötC population activity and XII motor output, as well as inspiratory frequency (f; or cycle time). We also measured the rise time, decay time, and duration of burstlets in field recordings. For whole-cell recordings, inspiratory bursts refer to depolarizations with concomitant spiking in preBötC neurons that occur in sync with XII motor output. We measured the amplitude of inspiratory bursts after smoothing to eliminate spikes but preserve the envelope of depolarization. To identify burstlet-like activity in whole-cell recordings, we performed simultaneous field recordings from the contralateral preBötC as well as XII motor output.
We wrote algorithms in MATLAB (RRID:SCR_001622) to calculate the mean frequency as well as the amplitude of bursts and burstlets. The coefficient of variation (CV) of preBötC or XII motor output frequency was calculated as the ratio of SD to the mean frequency.
For all intracellular and two-photon recordings, a neuron that participates in an inspiratory burst is referred as a burst-active neuron and a neuron that participates in a burstlet, is referred as a burstlet-active neuron.
Cycle-triggered averages were calculated and plotted in IgorPro (v.8, RRID:SCR_000325) using the onset of XII output as the event trigger for averaging preBötC inspiratory bursts; the onset of the burstlet itself served as the event trigger for averaging burstlets. We obtained the depolarization rate (V/s) of the event-triggered averages as the quotient of event amplitude and the elapsed time for that event to reach its peak amplitude. For preinspiratory activity, event amplitude was calculated as the absolute difference between the baseline level of activity and the level of activity in the field recording at the onset of XII motor output. For bursts, event amplitude was calculated as the difference between peak amplitude and field amplitude at the onset of XII motor output (that procedure omits the amplitude portion attributable to the preinspiratory phase). For burstlets, event amplitude was calculated as the difference between peak amplitude and baseline at the onset of the burstlet.
Two-photon imaging
We imaged intracellular Ca2+ in neurons contained in slices from Dbx1;Ai148 mice using a multi-photon laser-scanning microscope (Thorlabs) equipped with a water immersion 20x, 1.0 numerical aperture objective. Illumination was provided by an ultrafast tunable laser with a power output of 1050 mW at 970 nm, 80-MHz pulse frequency, and 100-fs pulse duration (Coherent Chameleon Discovery). We scanned Dbx1;Ai148 mouse slices over the preBötC and collected time series images at 32 Hz. Each frame reflects one-way raster scans with a resolution of 256 × 256 pixels (116 × 116 µm). Fluorescence data were collected using Thorlabs LS software and then analyzed using Fiji (Schindelin et al., 2012; Schneider et al., 2012), MATLAB, and IgorPro.
Regions of interest (ROIs) representing individual Dbx1-derived preBötC neurons were detected using MATLAB. First we found the set of collective inspiratory bursts in the time series from each Dbx1;Ai148 slice by averaging the fluorescence intensity of all pixels for each frame; fluorescence peaks are easily detectable periodic events. The collection of cycle periods is normally distributed; the 95% confidence intervals (CIs) are defined by the mean cycle period ± 2*SD.
Next, we down-sampled the planar resolution of our stack of images by 2n (n ≥ 1). The mean fluorescence of the constituent pixels was assigned to each composite pixel. We performed temporal fast Fourier transforms (FFTs) on the composite pixels. The maximum FFT value within the 95% CIs for slice frequency (determined above) was then mapped to a corresponding position in a new processed two-dimensional image. This method quantifies how strongly a composite pixel changes fluorescence at frequencies that correspond to inspiratory rhythm. After having created the complete processed image, we computed the mean and SD for FFT values associated with all composite pixels. Any composite pixel with intensity less than mean + 2*SD was set to zero. Any contiguous remaining pixel sets (whose diameter exceeds 6 µm) were retained as ROIs.
We then applied the set of ROIs to analyze Ca2+ transients in the original fluorescence imaging stack using the equation (Fi – F0)/F0, i.e., ΔF/F0, where Fi is the instantaneous average fluorescence intensity of all the pixels in a given ROI and F0 is the average fluorescence intensity of all the pixels within the same ROI averaged over the entire time series. Finally, we smoothed the ΔF/F0 time series with a forward moving average with a window of four time points.
Statistics
All the null hypothesis statistical tests were calculated using Prism (v.8, RRID:SCR_002798). Changes in the frequency and amplitude of preBötC field activity and XII output as a function of [K+]o were evaluated using linear regression. Changes in the rise time, decay time, and duration of burstlets were evaluated using linear regression. We compared group means using either Student’s paired t test or repeated measures one-way ANOVA, applying Holm–Sidak’s multiple comparison test post hoc. We compared the variability of frequency of preBötC events and XII motor output using a Kruskal–Wallis test, applying Dunn’s multiple comparison test post hoc. We compared the frequency of preBötC events using Welch’s t test.
Results
preBötC generates bursts at high levels of excitability but burstlets appear as excitability decreases
We manipulated excitability by varying [K+]o by integer units between 9 and 3 mM. At high [K+]o (e.g., 9 or 7 mM in Fig. 1A), we observed mostly burst events, defined by peaks of activity in the preBötC field recording with coincident XII output. Nevertheless, we also observed events in the preBötC field recording whose amplitudes measured 15–65% of the bursts and occurred without coincident XII discharge (Fig. 1A).
We detected and then studied low-amplitude preBötC events whose peaks exceeded the 95% CIs of baseline noise (Fig. 1A, insets). That detection process ensures that low-amplitude events are unlikely (with probability <0.05) to be ordinary uncoordinated fluctuations of neural activity. The alternative is that these low-amplitude preBötC events reflect coherent network activity.
If the low-amplitude events reflect burstlets as defined by Kam and Feldman (Kam et al., 2013a), then they should be more abundant at low levels of excitability where the collective activity of rhythmogenic neurons may not reach the threshold for burst generation. Visual inspection of the traces in Figure 1A shows that to be the case, i.e., low-amplitude events devoid of XII output are more abundant at 5 and 3 mM [K+]o compared to 9 and 7 mM [K+]o.
At 3 mM [K+]o, 70 ± 3% (n = 12 slices) of detected preBötC events occurred without concomitant XII output. We quantified the relative abundance of low-amplitude versus burst events for the entire data set (Fig. 1B, n = 19 slices). At incrementally higher [K+]o levels, the relative fraction of low-amplitude events decreases in a sigmoidal fashion such that they comprise only 5.2 ± 6.3% (n = 19 slices) of the preBötC events at 9 mM. We conclude that low-amplitude preBötC events, absent motor output, reflect burstlets as defined previously (Kam et al., 2013a).
Burst-burstlet and sigh rhythmic frequencies vary as a function of network excitability
We measured the frequency of preBötC rhythm (fpreBötC, which we refer to as composite rhythm because the measured events consist of either bursts or burstlets) and XII motor output (fXII) at different [K+]o levels (Fig. 2A) similar to Kam et al. (2013a). They measured the fpreBötC and fXII at three discrete [K+]o levels (3, 6, and 9 mM) and reported significantly lower fXII at 3 mM compared to either 6 or 9 mM. Regarding fpreBötC, it was lower at 3 mM, yet there was no difference between the fpreBötC measured at 6 vs 9 mM (Kam et al., 2013a, see their Fig. 1A and Table 1). Those data do not resolve the relationship between composite rhythm and network excitability so we measured rhythmic activity at all integer [K+]o levels between 3 and 9 mM. fpreBötC and fXII increased linearly as the excitability increased (in this report: Fig. 2A, Table 1, which reports linear regression tests). Additionally, fpreBötC and fXII differed significantly at both 3 and 4 mM [K+]o (Welch’s t test, p = 0.009 and p = 0.031, respectively) which maps to the portion of the curve in Figure 1B, where the relative fraction of burstlets plateaus at 70 ± 30% (n = 12 slices) and thus explains the relative sparsity of XII events compared to preBötC burstlets. Manipulations of network excitability influence the frequency of composite rhythm and the relative fraction of burstlets that comprise it.
Changes in the network excitability affect the periodic variability of XII motor output (Del Negro et al., 2009). We reexamined that principle and further tested the variability of the composite preBötC rhythm (Fig. 2B). The variability of fXII, quantified by CV, peaked at ∼1.1 when [K+]o was 6 mM (Fig. 2C; Table 3). In contrast, the CV of fpreBötC remained between 0.9 and 0.7 over low to medium levels of excitability (3–6 mM [K+]o) without peaking at 6 mM [K+]o (Fig. 2B; Table 3). These data suggest that peak CV of fXII at 6 mM [K+]o, an intermediate level of excitability, is not attributable to instability in the preBötC rhythm. It rather reflects the equal probability of evoking either burstlets (absent XII output) and bursts with XII output (Fig. 1B or Kam et al., 2013a, their Fig. 2), which makes the periodic XII output more variable than preBötC activity.
The variability of fpreBötC and fXII are lowest at 9 mM [K+]o (Fig. 2B,C; Table 3). ACSF containing 9 mM [K+]o represents the empirically determined ideal conditions for rhythmically active slices (Funk and Greer, 2013; Smith et al., 1991) where the likelihood of burstlets is minimal (Fig. 1B; Kam et al., 2013a, their Table 1).
We also examined the frequency of the sigh rhythm (fsigh), which increased linearly as the network excitability increased by means of [K+]o (Fig. 2D; Table 1).
We also examined the amplitude of bursts, burstlets, and XII output. The amplitude of preBötC bursts and XII output were invariable over all [K+]o levels (Fig. 3A,C), confirmed using linear regression tests (Table 1). However, the burstlet amplitude increased from 1.2 ± 0.6 mV at 3 mM [K+]o (n = 12 slices) to 2.3 ± 1.0 mV (n = 13 slices) at 9 mM K+ (Fig. 3B), which linear regression showed was unlikely to occur by chance if the slope of burstlet amplitude versus [K+]o was actually zero (p = 0.001; Table 1). These results show that burstlet amplitude depends on network excitability whereas preBötC burst and XII motor output amplitudes do not.
We measured the rise time, decay time, and duration of burstlets, but there were no trends across [K+]o levels, confirmed using linear regression (Table 2).
Burstlets evolve bilaterally and underlie the pre-inspiratory phase of preBötC bursts
If burstlets reflect coherent preBötC rhythmicity, then they should be bilaterally synchronous. To test that prediction, we recorded preBötC activity from both sides of slices along with XII motor output (Fig. 4A); 97 ± 4% and 92 ± 7% of preBötC burstlets were bilaterally synchronous at 6 and 3 mM [K+]o, respectively (n = 4 slices). The bilateral preBötC bursts commence ∼400 ms before the onset of XII motor output (Fig. 4A, inset), which is considered the preinspiratory phase and the hallmark of rhythm generation (Smith et al., 1990; Rekling et al., 1996; Kam et al., 2013a; Feldman and Kam, 2015; Del Negro et al., 2018).
If burstlets reflect the preinspiratory component of inspiratory bursts, as proposed by Kam and Feldman, then their trajectories should look alike when superimposed. At 6 mM [K+]o, the rising phase of the burstlet resembles the preinspiratory phase of the inspiratory burst (Fig. 4B). We compared the depolarization rates of the rising phase of burstlets, the preinspiratory phase of inspiratory bursts, and the rising phase of inspiratory bursts. The rising phase of burstlets is comparable to the rising phase of the preinspiratory activity, but the rising phase of both burstlets and preinspiratory activity are incommensurate with the rising phase of inspiratory bursts (one-way ANOVA, F(2,12) = 33.76, p = 1.2E-5; burstlet vs preinspiratory, p = 0.577; burstlet vs inspiratory, p = 2.5E-5; and preinspiratory vs inspiratory, p = 3.7E-5; n = 7 slices; Fig. 4C). These data suggest that burstlets and the preinspiratory phase of bursts reflect the same underlying process, which is distinct from the process underlying full inspiratory bursts.
Burstlets are the summation of EPSPs in preBötC neurons
We examined how individual preBötC inspiratory neurons contribute to collective events detected in field recordings (bursts and burstlets) via whole-cell recordings in CD-1 (n = 7) and Dbx1;Ai148 mouse (n = 3) slices. Dbx1;Ai148 mouse pups express genetically encoded Ca2+ reporter GCaMP6f in Dbx1-derived preBötC neurons obligatory for breathing rhythmogenesis (Bouvier et al., 2010; Gray et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2014; Cui et al., 2016; Vann et al., 2016, 2018; Baertsch et al., 2018).
In control conditions (9 mM [K+]o), inspiratory drive potentials synchronized with XII motor output during almost all cycles (96 ± 7%, n = 16 preBötC neurons recorded in slices from 10 different animals). We considered those events bursts. We then modified the excitability by changing [K+]o to either 6 mM (n = 11 neurons in eight slices) or 7 mM (n = 6 neurons in three slices). We recorded drive potentials of 6- to 10-mV amplitude that were not accompanied by XII motor output. We considered those events burstlets (Fig. 5A).
During bursts, preBötC neurons, from a baseline membrane potential of –60 mV (maintained below the activation threshold of persistent Na+ current via bias current), exhibit inspiratory drive potentials exceeding 20 mV and intraburst spiking of 2–17 spikes/burst (∼6–60 Hz). During burstlets, these same neurons exhibit EPSPs that summate during the burstlets (Fig. 5A, insets) as well as spikes (Fig. 5B, cycle-triggered average from a whole-cell recording). We never observed a preBötC neuron (n = 0/17 neurons in 10 slices) that was active during burstlets but not bursts.
Burstlets resemble the preinspiratory phase of bursts. This applies to field recordings (Fig. 4B) and whole-cell recordings at both [K+]o levels: 7 mM (n = 4 out of six neurons) and 6 mM (n = 6 out of 11 neurons; Fig. 5B). These are the first intracellular recordings to show that burstlets reflect the temporal summation of EPSPs, often crossing threshold to generate repetitive spiking, in preBötC neurons.
preBötC inspiratory neurons do not participate in every burstlet
Kam et al. (2013a) showed that 89% of the inspiratory preBötC neurons take part in burstlets. We retested that notion by comparing fpreBötC and fXII monitored during separate or simultaneous whole-cell and field recordings (Fig. 6A,B). We predicted that if 89% of inspiratory neurons participate in burstlets, then the frequency of composite rhythm obtained in whole-cell recordings should be comparable to that obtained in field recordings. We found no difference in fpreBötC measured via whole-cell and field recordings at 7 or 9 mM [K+]o. However, at 6 mM [K+]o, fpreBötC was significantly lower in whole-cell recordings compared to field recordings (Fig. 6A,B; Table 4). Additionally, at all [K+]o levels, the fXII calculated in whole-cell recordings and field recordings remain the same. These data imply that relatively fewer inspiratory preBötC neurons are burstlet-active at 6 mM [K+]o compared to 7 or 9 mM. Simultaneous triple recordings of Dbx1-derived preBötC rhythmogenic neurons, preBötC population activity, and XII motor output demonstrate that individual neurons do not participate in every population burstlet (Fig. 6C).
Burstlets occur in subsets of inspiratory preBötC neurons
To investigate how many and which neurons participate in burstlets, we recorded inspiratory Dbx1-derived preBötC neurons in Dbx1;Ai148 slices while simultaneously monitoring XII motor output (Figs. 7, 8). We recorded three to nine imaging planes per preBötC with 12 ± 7 active neurons per plane (range 3–27) for an average of 62 ± 20 inspiratory neurons recorded per Dbx1;Ai148 slice. Then, we manipulated [K+]o to examine burst (9 mM; n = 6 slices) and burstlet (7 mM, n = 6 slices; 6 mM, n = 2 slices) rhythms.
At 9 mM [K+]o, 20 ± 9% of the Dbx1-derived inspiratory neurons were active during burstlets (Fig. 7). Additionally, a neuron that was active during one burstlet was not always active during other burstlets (Fig. 8A, neurons 2, 3, 9, 14). On changing to 7 mM [K+]o, 17 ± 15% of the Dbx1-derived inspiratory neurons were active during burstlets (Fig. 7). Again, a neuron that participated in one burstlet did not always participate in the next burstlet (Fig. 8B, neurons 2, 3, 9). On changing to 6 mM [K+]o, 23% of Dbx1-derived neurons were active during burstlets (Fig. 7). These data show that the subset of Dbx1-derived preBötC neurons that participates in burstlets constitutes 17–23% of the population active during inspiratory bursts and that the composition of the burstlet-active subset varies from cycle to cycle.
Discussion
Inspiratory breathing movements emanate from neural activity in the preBötC but its rhythmogenic mechanisms remain incompletely understood and, some might argue, misunderstood. There appears to be a dichotomy between the mechanisms underlying rhythmogenesis and those governing motor pattern. Here, we investigate this rhythm-pattern dichotomy to unravel the neural mechanisms of preBötC functionality.
Defunct theories of inspiratory rhythmogenesis and why burstlets are viable explanation
Theories of rhythmogenesis fall into three camps. The first posits a ring of mutually inhibitory neurons that generates sequential phases of the breathing cycle including preBötC inspiratory bursts (Richter, 1982; Smith et al., 2007, 2013; Ausborn et al., 2018). The second theoretical framework emphasizes bursting-pacemaker neurons; the synchronization of pacemakers serves as a template for network activity (Feldman and Cleland, 1982; Johnson et al., 1994; Rekling and Feldman, 1998; Butera et al., 1999a,b; Ramirez et al., 2004). The third theory, dubbed a group pacemaker, posits that recurrent synaptic activity triggers mixed-cationic conductances to produce inspiratory bursts (Rekling et al., 1996; Rekling and Feldman, 1998; Del Negro and Hayes, 2008; Rubin et al., 2009).
Disinhibition of the preBötC (Shao and Feldman, 1997; Brockhaus and Ballanyi, 1998; Janczewski et al., 2013; Sherman et al., 2015; Marchenko et al., 2016; Cregg et al., 2017; Baertsch et al., 2018), as well as attenuation of pacemaker conductances (Del Negro et al., 2002, 2005; Peña et al., 2004; Pace et al., 2007; Koizumi and Smith, 2008) and mixed-cationic conductances (Koizumi et al., 2018; Picardo et al., 2019) neither perturbs the frequency in the predicted manner nor stops breathing in vivo or inspiratory rhythms in vitro, which falsifies all three rhythmogenic mechanisms. Nevertheless, the key to understanding rhythmogenesis may be found in what these theories get wrong: inextricable neural bursts that culminate the inspiratory phase of the cycle.
To consider the iconoclastic notion of rhythmogenesis in the absence of bursts we, like Kam and Feldman (Kam et al., 2013a; Feldman and Kam, 2015), focus on the preinspiratory phase that ordinarily leads to bursts and motor output. The preinspiratory phase is a hallmark of rhythmogenesis, marking early-activating rhythmogenic interneurons (Onimaru et al., 1987, 1988; Smith et al., 1990; Rekling et al., 1996; Carroll and Ramirez, 2013; Carroll et al., 2013). Concurrent excitation of four to nine preBötC interneurons in vitro, by photolytic glutamate uncaging (Smith et al., 1990; Rekling et al., 1996; Kam et al., 2013b; Sun et al., 2019), can effectively trigger a preBötC network burst after a latency of 100–400 ms, similar to the duration of the preinspiratory phase. There are two important take-aways: first, small numbers (<10) of coactive neurons can trigger a burst; second, the burst occurs after sufficient time for percolation of network interactions to reach threshold. Kam and Feldman (Kam et al., 2013a) divorced preinspiratory activity from bursts, showing that rhythmic burstlets remained in their absence, and argued that burstlets represent the rhythmogenic substrate.
We also observed preBötC field activity like burstlets absent XII output at all [K+]o levels. Manipulating network excitability detaches the preinspiratory and inspiratory components of preBötC burst and affects their prevalence. Field and whole-cell recordings from preBötC neurons showed that burstlet rise time, duration, and amplitude match preinspiratory activity. These data affirm the hypothesis that both burstlets and preinspiratory activity share a common rhythmogenic mechanism.
How many constituent neurons activate during bursts and burstlets?
The frequency of preBötC composite rhythm differed significantly between field recordings and whole-cell recordings at 6 mM [K+]o. Field recordings reflect activity among many preBötC neurons, whereas whole-cell recordings reflect just one constituent preBötC neuron. Two things change as excitability decreases. First, fewer neurons participate in burstlets but field recordings still detect the collective events. Second, any neuron singled out for whole-cell recording is less likely to be part of the burstlet-active subpopulation. The initial burstlet report showed that ∼89% of preBötC neurons active during bursts also participate in burstlets. Here, using photonics to monitor ∼62 Dbx1-derived inspiratory burst-active neurons, we found ∼20% participate in burstlets. We conclude using our whole-cell recordings and photonic recordings that the subset of burstlet-active neurons is inconstant and appears lower than 89%.
The estimated size of the rhythmogenic population is 560–650 preBötC neurons (Hayes et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2014) so the burstlet-active subpopulation numbers between 112–130 (20%) and 500–580 (89%). That seemingly large range can explain why burstlet amplitude varies with [K+]o: increasing excitability can recruit potentially hundreds of additional constituent preBötC neurons to the burstlet-active subpopulation. Whether or not the fraction of burstlet-active preBötC neurons is closer to 20% or 89%, very few (<10) coactive preBötC neurons can trigger full bursts and motor output (Kam et al., 2013b; Sun et al., 2019) so the relative fraction of burstlet-active neurons may not be a critical parameter governing network activity.
Burstlet mechanism: network oscillator that depends on recurrent synaptic excitation
Here, fpreBötC increased monotonically with [K+]o. Because [K+]o modulates network excitability via direct influence on baseline membrane potential in preBötC constituent neurons, we conclude that preBötC composite rhythm is voltage dependent. In contrast, the initial burstlet report showed no statistically significant disparity between fpreBötC at 6 vs 9 mM [K+]o, yet there was a disparity for 3 mM versus either 6 or 9 mM. This left the question open as to whether burstlet rhythm might be some form of synchronized voltage-independent biochemical oscillator in constituent neurons. Monotonically increasing fpreBötC as a function of [K+]o rules out that possibility. A significant difference in fpreBötC and fXII, at 3 and 4 mM [K+]o, suggests that burstlets maintain the preBötC rhythm. These data imply that burstlets are rhythmogenic in nature, but that concept of rhythmogenicity has not been proven and awaits definitive testing.
So, what mechanism does give rise to burstlets? Our whole-cell recordings show temporal summation of EPSPs during burstlets. We held membrane potential at –60 mV, which imposes steady-state deactivation of the persistent Na+ current (Del Negro et al., 2002; Ptak et al., 2005; Yamanishi et al., 2018). Therefore, burstlets do not reflect voltage-dependent bursting properties and do appear to reflect recurrent synaptic excitation.
In general, network oscillators (distinct from pacemaker or inhibition-based models) rely on recurrent excitation among constituent rhythmogenic neurons (Grillner, 2006; Grillner and El Manira, 2020). Modifying the neuronal excitability influences the relative fraction of spontaneously active neurons in the network, and, for silent neurons, the proximity of baseline membrane potential to spike threshold. Increasing excitability therefore magnifies the number of neurons interacting, facilitates synaptic drive summation, and accelerates the process of recurrent excitation to directly influence frequency. We conclude that burstlets are not only rhythmogenic, but also follow dynamics of recurrent excitation, i.e., a network oscillator model of rhythmogenesis.
Frequency control differs for sighs compared to burstlets and bursts
The preBötC can generate burstlets, inspiratory bursts, and sigh-related bursts (Lieske et al., 2000; Ruangkittisakul et al., 2008). Here, fsigh increased as the [K+]o level increased, which at face value suggests voltage dependence (akin to fpreBötC and fXII as argued above). However, fsigh is an order of magnitude lower than fpreBötC and fXII. Further, the [K+]o-dependent increase in fsigh is 20 times less steep than that of fpreBötC or fXII. These observations suggest that the mechanism for frequency control of the composite preBötC rhythm and the XII motor output do not similarly apply to sigh rhythm. We propose that sigh frequency control is not voltage dependent like burstlets and inspiratory bursts, which implicates a biochemical oscillator for sigh rhythms that interacts with the network oscillator underlying burstlets and inspiratory bursts to bring about the less-steep fsigh versus [K+]o curve. We cannot yet specify how the sigh rhythm is generated; it is beyond the scope of this paper.
Pattern generation
Burstlet amplitude depends on the excitability too. Above, we inferred that additional constituent neurons (perhaps ∼100 s) are recruited as excitability increases. In contrast, the amplitude of preBötC bursts and XII motor output are not voltage dependent across [K+]o levels. Although burstlet amplitude changes with excitability, the rise time, fall time, and duration of burstlet events do not vary with [K+]o levels. First, we conclude that the network dynamics that recruit burstlet-active neurons, and govern the temporal evolution of burstlets, are largely the same at different levels of excitability. Second, we conclude that preBötC activity, once passing threshold, triggers a cascade that activates all (or nearly all) preBötC neurons, and also activates premotor and motor neurons. That cascade probably depends on synaptic connectivity among pattern-related preBötC neurons and premotor neurons outside of the preBötC, but not preBötC excitability per se.
We cannot yet specify how activity during the preinspiratory phase reaches a threshold for burst generation. It may have to do with a quorum: a certain number of rhythmogenic interneurons must be active (i.e., spiking) to trigger an irreversible cascade that ostensibly activates all (or nearly all) preBötC neurons. Or, it may have to do with synchrony: a certain number of rhythmogenic interneurons must be spiking in sync to trigger that cascade (Ashhad and Feldman, 2019). The former focuses on mass action of constituent interneurons wherein temporal precision is inconsequential. The latter focuses on phasic precision rather than mass action. Our present data cannot distinguish which mechanism is at work but given that burstlet amplitude is voltage-dependent and the number of constituent neurons participating in burstlets may vary at any given level of excitability, the quorum model seems less feasible.
There is an existing framework for understanding both rhythm and pattern generation of the preBötC. Dbx1-derived preBötC neurons are inspiratory rhythmogenic (Bouvier et al., 2010; Gray et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2014; Cui et al., 2016; Koizumi et al., 2016; Vann et al., 2016, 2018; Baertsch et al., 2018), playing key preinspiratory and burst-generating roles (Picardo et al., 2013; Cui et al., 2016), and some serving exclusively premotor function (Revill et al., 2015). It may be possible to identify subsets of predominantly rhythmogenic versus predominantly premotor or pattern-related Dbx1-derived preBötC neurons based on intrinsic membrane properties (Picardo et al., 2013) or neuropeptide somatostatin expression (Cui et al., 2016). Those two classification schemes are not mutually exclusive because many somatostatin- and somatostatin receptor-expressing preBötC neurons are Dbx1-derived (Gray et al., 2010). It is also possible that particular ion channels serve in a pattern-related capacity within core rhythmogenic neurons. For example, transient receptor potential (Trp) channels in Dbx1-derived preBötC neurons whose activation amplifies inspiratory burst magnitude (Koizumi et al., 2018; Picardo et al., 2019) also maintain the tidal volume of inspiratory breaths in unanesthetized adult mice (Picardo et al., 2019).
Our results affirm the ideas presented in the burstlet hypothesis (Kam et al., 2013a) that rhythm and pattern generation are discrete processes, which nevertheless both begin in the preBötC. Burstlets, subthreshold from the standpoint of motor discharge, appear to reflect the core rhythmogenic mechanism involving recurrent synaptic excitation.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health Grant R01 HL104127 (to C.A.D.N.).
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.