Abstract
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is often used to understand the function of individual brain regions, but this ignores the fact that TMS may affect network-level rather than nodal-level processes. We examine the effects of a double perturbation to two frontoparietal network nodes, compared with the effects of single lesions to either node. We hypothesized that Bayesian evidence for the absence of effects that build upon one another indicates that a single perturbation is consequential to network-level processes. Twenty-three humans performed pro-saccades (look toward) and anti-saccades (look away) after receiving continuous theta-burst stimulation (cTBS) to right frontal eye fields (FEFs), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), or somatosensory cortex (S1; the control region). On a subset of trials, a TMS pulse was applied to right posterior parietal cortex (PPC). FEF, DLPFC, and PPC are important frontoparietal network nodes for generating anti-saccades. Bayesian t tests were used to test hypotheses for enhanced double perturbation effects (cTBS plus TMS pulse) on saccade behaviors, against the alternative hypothesis that double perturbation effects to a network are not greater than single perturbation effects. In one case, we observed strong evidence [Bayes factor (BF10) = 325] that PPC TMS following DLPFC cTBS enhanced impairments in ipsilateral anti-saccade amplitudes over DLPFC cTBS alone, and not over the effect of the PPC pulse alone (BF10 = 0.75), suggesting that double perturbation effects do not augment one another. Rather, this suggests that computations are distributed across the network, and in some cases there can be compensation for cTBS perturbations.
Significance Statement
We explore whether a frontoparietal network important to executive control, attentional processing, and saccadic gaze behaviors operates in a distributed fashion, compared with what would be predicted from combining contributions from individual brain regions. This is important as lesions or perturbations to these regions individually can produce behavioral deficits. We apply inhibitory transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to a frontal cortical region, followed by a second TMS perturbation to a parietal region. The point is that this second perturbation could, in principle, build on the effects of the first perturbation. We tested different hypotheses regarding the effects of such double perturbations and conclude that the effects do not build on one another, suggesting that a single perturbation affects a network-level process.
Introduction
It is well known that the effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) extend beyond the site of stimulation (Ilmoniemi et al., 1997; Paus et al., 1997; Ruff et al., 2006; Ko et al., 2008; Morishima et al., 2009). In some instances, distal effects may reflect compensatory responses to the TMS perturbation (Sack et al., 2005; O’Shea et al., 2007; Hartwigsen et al., 2013), suggesting “homeostatic metaplasticity” (Müller-Dahlhaus and Ziemann, 2015) at the level of network nodes. Here we assess another functionally relevant possibility: whether behavioral consequences of a spatially localized perturbation from TMS are driven by the distributed nature of computations throughout a circuit (Price and Friston, 2002). This would have consequences as to whether nodal effects build on one another.
The saccadic eye-movement system provides a tractable testing ground for assessing circuit-level consequences of TMS (Leigh and Kennard, 2004; Munoz et al., 2007). Roles of three cortical nodes, frontal eye fields (FEFs), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and posterior parietal cortex (PPC) have been well described (Munoz and Everling, 2004; Johnston and Everling, 2011; Paré and Dorris, 2011). In the anti-saccade task (where subjects must look away from a peripheral visual stimulus; Hallett, 1978), DLPFC is thought to be critical to establishing the appropriate task set and preventing an automatic saccade to the stimulus; FEF is thought to be critical to voluntary saccade programming and to “preparatory set”; and FEF along with PPC are thought to be critical to the visuo-motor transformations to develop a saccade “vector” (Connolly et al., 2002; Leigh and Kennard, 2004; Munoz and Everling, 2004).
Evidence shows how DLPFC, FEF, and PPC interact as part of a distributed system, as follows: TMS to either DLPFC or FEFs (or supplementary eye fields) during saccade programming prolonged reaction times, suggesting preparatory set is distributed among all three nodes (Nagel et al., 2008). Magnetoencephalography (MEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) showed that FEF and PPC are both involved in the attentional aspects of the anti-saccade vector (Medendorp et al., 2005; Moon et al., 2007), and TMS to FEF or PPC produces hypometric anti-saccades (Nyffeler et al., 2008b; Jaun-Frutiger et al., 2013; Cameron et al., 2015). However, it is not possible to distinguish a difference in timing (even with MEG) between when an anti-saccade program is developed in the PPC compared with FEF (Moon et al., 2007), implying a distributed process.
We build on this knowledge to study the effects on behavior after a “double perturbation” to this network in the right hemisphere. Shortly after applying continuous theta-burst stimulation (cTBS; Huang et al., 2005) to either right FEF (r-FEF) or right DLPFC (r-DLPFC), we measure the consequences of a second time-resolved perturbation to the circuit, in the form of a single TMS pulse to right PPC (r-PPC). This approach arbitrates among five hypotheses regarding the consequences of the double perturbation. In hypothesis A, “Augmented,” the double perturbation could produce an augmented effect by concurrently impairing spatially separate nodes that provide critical, but computationally distinct, functions, resulting in behavioral perturbations that are greater than the effect of either perturbation alone (Fig. 1A). Alternatively, hypothesis B, “Distributed,” pertains to the case where computations are performed by a distributed system at the network level, so a single perturbation to either node should perturb behavior as much as the double perturbation (Price et al., 2017; Fig. 1B). In hypothesis C, “Compensatory,” distal nodes could compensate for the perturbation, which would predict greater effects from the double perturbation compared with the cTBS perturbation alone (Fig. 1C), because the second perturbation impairs a region that has become more important functionally, because of the first (cTBS) perturbation. In hypothesis D, “Spreading,” the effects from cTBS spread trans-synaptically to other portions of the network (Ko et al., 2008), predicting greater effects from the double perturbation than from the single-pulse perturbation alone (Fig. 1D). Finally, in hypothesis E, “Boosting,” additional regions throughout the network could provide homeostatic compensation, which would manifest as a perplexing boost to performance following cTBS (alone) and could reduce or prevent the impairment from additional TMS perturbations (Fig. 1E). (The difference between this and the Compensatory hypothesis is that there is the perplexing boost to performance after cTBS.)
To discriminate among those hypotheses, we used fMRI to localize right DLPFC, FEF, and PPC in individual subjects performing an anti-saccade task. These regions were then used for targeting subject-specific TMS interventions while participants performed the same task outside the scanner. Performance (percentage correct direction), reaction times, and saccade amplitudes were assessed using Bayesian t tests to provide statistical evidence in favor or against greater effects from double-TMS, compared with single-TMS, perturbations.
Materials and Methods
Participants
The study was approved by the local ethics committee (Commissie Mensgebonden Onderzoek, Arnhem-Nijmegen), and written informed consent was obtained from the participants in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. A total of 27 healthy, right-handed, young-adult, human subjects was recruited for four sessions ∼1 week apart. Three subjects were excluded for failure to provide usable eye-tracking data on all TMS sessions, and one subject had error rates on anti-saccade trials exceeding 90% (>3× the SD) and so was excluded, resulting in a sample size of 24 participants (mean ± SE age, 23 ± 2 years; 11 males).
Detailed procedure
Session 1
Participants were screened for contraindications related to fMRI and to single-pulse TMS and cTBS according to common safety guidelines (Rossi et al., 2009; Oberman et al., 2011). Resting motor thresholds (RMTs) and active motor thresholds (AMTs) were established for the first dorsal interosseous muscle of the subject’s right hand using electromyography. TMS was applied using a hand-held biphasic figure-eight coil with a 75 mm outer winding diameter (MagVenture), connected to a MagProX100 System (MagVenture). Coil orientation was chosen to induce a posterior–anterior electrical field in the brain (45° from the mid-sagittal axis).
Subjects performed five runs of an interleaved pro-saccade (look toward)/anti-saccade (look away) task to identify the cortical regions of interest (ROIs; Fig. 2B). An interleaved task was used as evidence suggests an important role for DLPFC (Everling and DeSouza, 2005; Johnston et al., 2014) as well as for FEF (DeSouza and Everling, 2010) in task or preparatory set and thus could not simply default to an anti-saccade task set on each trial. Two target positions (13° or 9°) in the left and right directions were included so that subjects would have to rely on spatial information to calculate the saccade vector. In this way, we could be sure that the paradigm required DLPFC, FEF, and PPC processes.
Detailed fMRI procedure
fMRI scans were obtained with a 3 tesla MRI scanner (Skyra, Siemens Medical Systems) using a 32-channel head coil. The functional images were acquired with multiband sequence [acceleration factor = 3, repetition time (TR) = 1000 ms, echo time (TE) = 30 ms, flip angle = 60°). Each volume consisted of 33 slices, with a distance of 17% and a thickness of 3 mm. The voxel resolution was 3.5 × 3.5 × 3.0 mm, the FOV in the read direction was 224 mm, and the FOV in the phase direction was 100%. Two volumes were discarded from each functional run to account for scanner steady-state equilibrium, leading to a total of 339 volumes per run. The anatomic images were acquired with an MPRAGE sequence (TR = 2300 ms, TE = 3.9 ms, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm). In total, 192 images were obtained for each participant. During the scan, participants lay in a supine position and their heads were stabilized using soft cushions.
Imaging data were analyzed with SPM8 (Wellcome Trust Center for Cognitive Neuroimaging, London, UK). At the single-subject level, the data were realigned to the first volume of each run using six rigid body transformations (three translations and three rotations). The images were then coregistered to the individual structural T1, and spatial smoothing was performed by means of an 8 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel. A first-level analysis was performed by specifying a general linear model with regressors for each condition (fixation trials were not modeled, however). Motion parameters (three translations, three rotations) were included as nuisance regressors.
A contrast of anti-saccade trials against baseline was computed to define 5 mm ROIs centered on locations of peak activation on each subject anatomic scan, using a t contrast at p < 0.001 (uncorrected). Table 1 provides the Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI) coordinates of these ROIs, and their distances to the scalp as derived from Localite TMS Navigation software 2.2. Figure 2A illustrates the coordinates on a canonical T1 scan. r-DLPFC was defined as peak fMRI anti-saccade activity surrounding the middle frontal gyrus, anterior to the ventricles. Right FEF was defined as peak activity in the precentral sulcus (selecting medial peaks if lateral peaks were also present, to relate more to anti-saccade processes; Neggers et al., 2012). r-PPC was defined as peak activity in the intraparietal sulcus, selecting peaks in more medial clusters if more than one was present. Finally, right S1 (r-S1; the control region) was localized anatomically for each participant, as the most superior extent of the postcentral gyrus, located on average 9 ± 2 mm lateral to the longitudinal fissure to avoid lateral proprioceptive eye-position signals ( Zhang et al., 2008; Balslev et al., 2011; Table 1, Fig. 2A).
Session 2–4
cTBS was applied to r-DLPFC, r-FEF, or r-S1 before performing the task on three separate sessions, counterbalanced for order. cTBS was applied to FEF or to DLPFC because we wished to assess double perturbation effects across two nodes, which are both linked to PPC, but where one (FEF) is thought to have a more direct link in visuo-motor processes (Leigh and Kennard, 2004; Munoz and Everling, 2004) and in network interactions described in the resting state (Corbetta et al., 2005; He et al., 2007; Vossel et al., 2014). cTBS was delivered with a posterior–anterior direction of the electric field induced in the brain, with the handle pointed backward at ∼30° to the sagittal plane. In this way, the outer windings of the TMS coil did not overlap the other ROIs. TMS coil alignment was achieved using Localite and a subject-specific anatomic scan.
The parameters for cTBS were identical to those described by Huang et al. (2005) consisting of 50 Hz triplets repeated at 5 Hz over a period of 40 s. Stimulation intensity for cTBS was defined as 80% of the AMT (mean = 41% ± 9% maximum stimulator output), defined as peak-to-peak motor-evoked potential (MEP) amplitudes exceeding 200 μV on 5 of 10 trials, while subjects maintained voluntary contraction of ∼10%. Stimulation intensity for single-pulse TMS to PPC was set at 110% of the RMT (mean = 43% ± 8% maximum stimulator output), defined as peak-to-peak MEP amplitudes of 50 μV on 5 of 10 trials. Forty seconds of cTBS (at 80% of AMT) has effects lasting ∼50 min (Wischnewski and Schutter, 2015), providing sufficient time to test the influence of the PPC pulse.
Eye tracking and task
The position of the right eye was recorded using an infrared Eyelink 1000 eye tracker (SR Research) with a 1000 Hz sampling rate. A 9-point calibration was conducted, and a drift correction point was used as the intertrial fixation point. Saccades were identified by a horizontal deflection (3× SDs of the baseline velocity) and a duration between 15 and 150 ms. The camera was positioned under the stimulus screen, ∼60 cm away from the eyes of the participant, who sat precisely at 70 cm from a wide-angle LCD screen (with central presentation zone set at 4:3, and 1024 × 768 resolution).
Subjects performed the same task (Fig. 2B) as in the fMRI. Representative eye traces from a single subject are shown in Figure 2C. In each run, there were 72 trials, of which 48 contained a TMS pulse presented to PPC at a random interval between 30 and 300 ms after the onset of the peripheral stimulus (described in Data analysis). The first run commenced 10 min after cTBS and was analyzed up to 50 min after cTBS to capture the same cTBS effects on each session. Subjects were asked to perform five runs, each taking ∼8 min, including drift corrections and breaks, meaning that for each condition of interest (task, direction) there were 30 trials without the single pulse (“pulse absent”), and 60 trials containing the single pulse.
Data analysis
Data were analyzed in MATLAB version 11 (MathWorks). Valid trials consisting of correct and incorrect directions were separated from invalid trials, consisting of saccade reaction times (SRTs) < 90 ms (anticipatory errors), slower than 1000 ms, and trials where the TMS pulse to PPC occurred after saccade onset. The following three behavioral parameters of interest were analyzed: amplitude of the primary saccade, percentage correct direction, and SRT.
We first set a division between an “Early” and “Late” pulse time bin as follows: using the pulse absent trials, we collected the SRTs across subjects for correctly performed anti-saccades and for direction errors on anti-saccades for each cTBS session separately, and plotted these data in 10 ms bin histograms (Fig. 3). A binomial test revealed the first bin (Fig. 3, black arrows) where the two trial types were no longer significantly different from chance (50%); these bins occurred at 150 ms for the S1 cTBS and DLPFC cTBS sessions, and at 160 ms for the FEF-cTBS session. This method approximates the division between visually triggered “express” pro-saccades and voluntary saccades (Munoz and Everling, 2004), and is important to approximate when the PPC pulse would have greater influences during visual processing rather than motor programming components of an anti-saccade, which are in different directions.
We performed a repeated-measures ANOVA in SPSS (IBM) using pulse absent trials to determine whether there were significant interactions between the site of cTBS and stimulus eccentricity for amplitudes. However, no interactions with cTBS site and eccentricity were significant (F(2,44) < 1.75, p > 0.19), so we collapsed across eccentricity. Next, we performed a multivariate repeated-measures ANOVA using pulse absent trials, split into the first and second half of testing time, to examine whether there were any significant interactions involving Half and cTBS Site across the three parameters of interest (a potential concern being that cTBS effects wore off): however, no interactions with cTBS Site and Half reached significance (Pillai’s trace values < 0.19, F(6,86) < 1.54, p > 0.18).
Statistics
To directly assess our five network hypotheses regarding the combined effects from cTBS and the PPC pulse (Fig. 1), we performed Bayesian paired-sample t tests in JASP (JASP Team, 2017; Figs. 4-Figs. 7, brackets). A Bayes factor (BF10) indicates the evidence for the alternative hypothesis relative to the null hypothesis given the data. Our tests were focused first on situations where the double perturbation produces impairments that were greater than the single perturbations; thus, BF10 here indicates whether the combined effects were greater than the individual effects from cTBS alone, or from PPC TMS alone. For amplitude and percentage correct, lower values are indicative of greater impairments: therefore, the alternative hypothesis for BF10 is that the difference of the combined effect minus the single perturbation effect was <0, and the null hypothesis would be that this difference is not less than zero. For reaction times, higher values are indicative of an impairment (slower latency), so the alternative hypothesis is that the combined effect minus the single perturbation effect is greater than zero (and the null hypothesis is that it is not greater than zero). Note, however, that strong evidence from these tests for the null hypothesis (not less than zero) could be driven by a difference in the opposite direction. When such “strong” evidence was found (BF10 < 0.1; Jeffreys, 1961; Wetzels et al., 2011), we subsequently performed tests in the opposite direction to determine whether the effect of the single perturbation was greater than that of the double perturbation.
We report evidence for behavioral impairments that meet or exceed “substantial” (BF10 > 3; Jeffreys, 1961; Wetzels et al., 2011). Between 0.33 and 3, the evidence is considered only “anecdotal,” and in relation to p values, it was shown that ∼70% of “positive” results from 855 tests falling in the interval between p < 0.01 to p < 0.05 corresponded to only anecdotal evidence (Wetzels et al., 2011). Therefore, our boundary criteria of substantial are conservative in relation to typical p values.
Tests for each individual trial type compared with the control condition (S1 cTBS, PPC pulse absent) were also conducted using Bayesian one-sample t tests in JASP to confirm whether the individual perturbations themselves caused impairments. Here, BF10 indicates the relative likelihood that cTBS or single-pulse TMS impaired behavior compared with the null hypothesis that the behaviors were not impaired relative to the control condition. The values for these tests are seen in Tables 3-Tables 6 and are seen as asterisks in Figures 4-Figures 7 when substantial.
Table 2 (statistics table) lists all BF values from the Bayesian t tests along with their corresponding effect sizes as the medians of the posterior distributions, with 95% confidence intervals.
Results
FEF versus control cTBS conditions: anti-saccades
Saccade amplitude
There was substantial evidence that FEF cTBS caused impairments in leftward anti-saccade amplitudes for conditions also involving PPC pulses, and for rightward anti-saccades for conditions involving the late PPC pulse (Table 3, Amplitude, BF10 > 3). There was not substantial evidence that the PPC pulse on its own produced an impairment, and there was also not substantial evidence (Fig. 4A, brackets, all BF10 ≤ 2.91) to indicate greater impairments from the double perturbation condition compared with either single perturbation condition.
Percentage correct direction
There was not substantial evidence that anti-saccades were impaired by either form of TMS; in fact, strong evidence toward the null hypothesis was found for conditions with the PPC pulse (Table 3, Percentage correct, BF10 < 0.1). [Bayesian t tests performed in the opposite direction revealed substantial or greater evidence (BF10 > 3) for a performance benefit from the PPC pulses.] Similarly, there was strong evidence that there were not greater impairments from the double perturbation compared with either single perturbation (Fig. 4B).
Saccade reaction times
For SRTs, “decisive” (Wetzels et al., 2011) evidence for impairments were observed for conditions with the late PPC pulse alone, but not for those following FEF cTBS (Table 3, Saccade reaction time). Strong evidence was found that FEF cTBS plus a late PPC pulse resulted in greater impairments relative to FEF cTBS alone (and substantial evidence was found for a greater impairment for the early PPC pulse for leftward anti-saccades; Fig. 4C). However, strong evidence was found that impairments for leftward anti-saccades were not greater when the late PPC pulse followed FEF cTBS compared with when it was alone (Fig. 4C, BF = 0.08, italicized): when tested in the reverse direction, there was substantial evidence that the impairment after the late PPC pulse alone was greater than after FEF cTBS, BF10 = 4.12.
DLPFC versus control cTBS conditions: anti-saccades
Saccade amplitude
There was substantial evidence for impairments to anti-saccades after DLPFC cTBS in conditions involving the late PPC pulse, and for DLPFC cTBS alone for leftward anti-saccades (Table 4, Amplitude). Strong evidence was found for a greater impairment from the combined perturbation effects for rightward anti-saccades after the late pulse relative to the DLPFC cTBS alone (BF10 = 325.22), but this was not found compared with the effects of the late PPC pulse alone (BF10 = 0.75; Fig. 5A).
Percentage correct direction
There was no evidence that anti-saccades were impaired by DLPFC cTBS, with, or without, the PPC pulse (Table 4, Percentage correct). (Bayesian t tests revealed strong evidence for anti-saccade benefits to performance following DLPFC cTBS and late PPC pulses.) There was also no evidence for greater impairment from a double perturbation compared with single perturbation (Fig. 5B).
Saccade reaction times
There was strong evidence for impaired reaction times at the late pulse time following DLPFC cTBS for right anti-saccades (Table 4, Saccade reaction time), and there was strong evidence that the combined effects of DLPFC cTBS and a late PPC pulse resulted in greater impairments relative to DLPFC cTBS alone (Fig. 5C), but there was no evidence for greater impairment compared with the PPC pulse.
FEF versus control cTBS conditions: pro-saccades
Saccade amplitude
Table 5, Amplitude, and Figure 6A show that there was not substantial evidence for the effects of either TMS condition on pro-saccade amplitudes.
Percentage correct direction
Substantial impairments were found for rightward pro-saccades following FEF cTBS during trials with the addition of a late PPC pulse (Table 5, Percentage correct, BF10 = 4.53). There was also substantial evidence that the impairments to leftward pro-saccades were greater following FEF cTBS when there was a late PPC pulse (Fig. 6B, BF10 = 3.74) compared with FEF cTBS alone. There was not substantial evidence for other impairments.
Saccade reaction times
Substantial or greater evidence for pro-saccade reaction time impairments was observed for all PPC pulse conditions (Table 5, Saccade reaction time). There was also strong evidence that the combined effects of FEF cTBS and PPC pulses resulted in greater impairments relative to FEF cTBS alone (Fig. 6C); however, there was no evidence for a greater impairment over the PPC pulse effects alone.
DLPFC versus control cTBS conditions: pro-saccades
Saccade amplitude
There was not substantial evidence for any effects to pro-saccade amplitudes (Table 6, Amplitude, Fig. 7A).
Percentage correct direction
There was substantial evidence that the impairments to rightward pro-saccades were greater following DLPFC cTBS when the late PPC pulse was present (Fig. 7B; BF10 = 3.60), but no other evidence for impairments was substantial (Table 6, Percentage correct).
Saccade reaction times
There was decisive evidence for reaction time impairments at the late PPC pulse time following DLPFC cTBS, and substantial evidence for impairments at the early PPC pulse time for leftward anti-saccades (Table 6, Saccade reaction time). Also, there was substantial evidence that the combined effects of DLPFC cTBS and PPC pulses resulted in greater impairments relative to DLPFC cTBS alone (Fig. 7C).
Discussion
We found Bayesian evidence for impaired FEF and DLPFC anti-saccade amplitudes following a cTBS perturbation, and that compensation by PPC was possible after DLPFC cTBS perturbed ipsilateral anti-saccades. There was no evidence that cTBS impaired anti-saccade reaction times or correct directions, and we note that the impairments to anti-saccade amplitudes were not found in every condition following cTBS alone. Interestingly, however, we did not find any Bayesian evidence for an augmented effect, whereby the two TMS perturbations built on one another, suggesting instead the effects are generated at the network rather than nodal/regional level only.
Performance of pro-saccades and anti-saccades involves cortical and subcortical regions including FEF, PPC, DLPFC, supplementary eye fields, anterior cingulate cortex, visual cortex, basal ganglia, cerebellum, superior colliculus, and brainstem reticular formation (Moschovakis et al., 1996; Munoz and Everling, 2004; Munoz and Schall, 2004; Everling and DeSouza, 2005; Ford et al., 2005; Medendorp et al., 2006; Schall, 2009). A frontoparietal, precuneus, and parietal-medio-temporal network has also been identified as being involved in anti-saccade generation by independent company analysis-based fMRI, in addition to an eye-field network involved in both pro-saccades and anti-saccades (Domagalik et al., 2012). This highlights the wide-ranging involvement of several brain networks with the implication that one may not always observe deficits after a TMS perturbation or lesion, given the potential for redundancy or “degeneracy” (Price and Friston, 2002). Nevertheless, key neurophysiological processes related to voluntary saccade programming, reflexive saccade inhibition, and attentional reorienting processes point to important nodal roles for FEF, PPC, and DLPFC, explaining why deficits can result from single lesions or perturbations.
The frontal eye fields
In FEF, “saccade” and “fixation” neurons could provide two substrates for saccade programming and saccade inhibition. First, some of the saccade neurons code for the motor goal of saccades, while others process visual and visuomotor information (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985; Schlag-Rey et al., 1992; Schall, 2002; Sato and Schall, 2003; Schall et al., 2011). Reversible FEF lesions by cooling probe in monkeys were shown to produce hypometria (Keating and Gooley, 1988; Peel et al., 2014), and patients with FEF lesions have shown reduced contralateral saccade amplitudes (Rivaud et al., 1994; Ploner et al., 1999), though not always (Terao et al., 2016). Second, FEF saccade neurons show decreased activity during the preparatory phase of anti-saccade compared with pro-saccade trials (Everling and Munoz, 2000). FEF fixation neurons, on the other hand, show increased activity during fixation (even in the absence of a stimulus; Hanes et al., 1998; Izawa et al., 2009), implying that they are substrates for stopping reflexive saccades (Munoz and Everling, 2004; Boucher et al., 2007; Schall and Godlove, 2012). Indeed, some patients with lesions encompassing FEF have shown difficulty in suppressing reflexive saccades (Guitton et al., 1985; Van der Stigchel et al., 2012; Terao et al., 2016), and increased voluntary saccade latencies (Terao et al., 2016). However, one patient with a highly circumscribed left FEF lesion showed no deficits in inhibiting reflexive saccades, but did have hypometria (Gaymard et al., 1999). Together, this shows that FEF is important to voluntary saccade programming, but task, or lesion, specifics may dictate whether its role is critical given the potential for the contributions from other network regions with neuronal populations that can carry similar information. Evidence shows, for instance, that deficits following an FEF lesion become more severe if the superior colliculus is also lesioned (Schiller et al., 1979; Keating and Gooley, 1988).
TMS perturbations to FEF have largely produced similar effects. Like lesions, TMS perturbations lack the specificity to affect saccade neurons uniquely from fixation neurons, meaning that caution should be taken in attempts to interpret the effects on particular neuronal populations. A single TMS pulse to FEF increased the latency for ipsilateral anti-saccade trials, but did not increase pro-saccade errors (Müri et al., 1991; Olk et al., 2006). However, in another study, a single TMS pulse to FEF at 100 ms post-stimulus onset, increased anti-saccade latency and increased the frequency of contralateral pro-saccade errors (Terao et al., 1998). (This distinction may be due to the fact that single pulses during anti-saccade generation would perturb an ongoing process whereby anti-saccade processes are in competition with more automatic pro-saccade signals, an effect that can explain our findings regarding pro-saccade and anti-saccade reaction times.) Another study showed increases latency in both pro-saccade as well as anti-saccade trials, particularly late during preparation (at 200 ms; Nagel et al., 2008). In a few studies, cTBS to FEF was shown to increase reaction times (Nyffeler et al., 2006a,b; Liu et al., 2011), but in other cases cTBS was reported to affect saccade amplitudes instead (Jaun-Frutiger et al., 2013; Cameron et al., 2015).
The posterior parietal cortex
In monkeys, the generation of anti-saccades recruits lateral intraparietal area (LIP) neurons (the region of the primate PPC most associated with attention and eye movements; Gottlieb and Goldberg, 1999; Zhang and Barash, 2000; Bisley and Goldberg, 2010). LIP has been described as a “priority” map for attentional orienting, either overtly (a gaze change) or covertly (Bisley and Goldberg, 2010), integrating bottom-up visual information with top-down goal-directed information. Some LIP neurons signaling a visual stimulus then show activity during the motor component of vector inversion, which could be representing a remapped visual response (Zhang and Barash, 2000). In humans, PPC bilaterally (along with FEF) is shown to signal the vector inversion process (Medendorp et al., 2005; Moon et al., 2007; Collins et al., 2008). Patients with lesions to PPC have demonstrated saccade hypometria (Duhamel et al., 1992; Ptak and Müri, 2013), and those exhibiting neglect lesions often display erroneous saccades to ipsilesional “distractor” stimuli (Ptak and Müri, 2013) or deficits in remapping a saccade goal if the target changes position (Duhamel et al., 1992). Some patients display longer latencies on reaction times for reflexive, visually guided saccades (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 1991; Terao et al., 2016), fitting with evidence that the PPC may have a role in triggering express saccades (Hamm et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2013). Altogether, this highlights an important role of PPC in the visuo-motor aspects of saccade generation. Disruptive effects from TMS on these visuo-motor aspects is also consistent with these following observations: a TMS pulse to PPC shortly after stimulus onset (100 ms) produces hypometric anti-saccades to the ipsilateral (to TMS) direction, which then reverses to affect the motor vector in the opposite direction when applied later (>333 ms; Nyffeler et al., 2008b). Contralateral neglect is also reported from cTBS to right PPC (Nyffeler et al., 2008a).
The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
DLPFC is well known to be involved in cognitive control (Gazzaley and D’Esposito, 2007) and is therefore highly likely to be an important region in a network controlling voluntary saccades. Human and monkey studies have indeed found “preparatory” signals during pro-saccade or anti-saccade instruction periods in DLPFC (Everling and Munoz, 2000; Connolly et al., 2002; DeSouza et al., 2003; Everling and DeSouza, 2005; Ford et al., 2005; Brown et al., 2007), and SC neurons have been demonstrated to receive task-related signals from DLPFC (Johnston and Everling, 2006). There are also spatial signals in some DLPFC neurons, which is particularly important in visual working memory: DLPFC neurons were shown to have receptive/response fields with a contralateral bias (across the population) in working memory task delay periods (Funahashi et al., 1989; Ikkai and Curtis, 2011), which is not surprising if it shares information with FEF and PPC. Indeed, findings from human neuroimaging suggest that DLPFC is connected to FEF as well as to PPC functionally as well as anatomically (de Schotten et al., 2011; Vossel et al., 2014), and one physiologic study that recorded all three regions simultaneously in a sensorimotor decision task showed that sensory information “flows” from early visual regions to LIP, FEF, and DLPFC, and task-related signals flows from DLPFC and LIP to FEF (Siegel et al., 2015).
Patients with DLPFC lesions exhibit increased pro-saccade errors on anti-saccade trials (Guitton et al., 1985; Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 1991; Ploner et al., 2005), suggesting it has a direct role in suppression. However, it has been difficult to dissociate a suppression role specifically of DLPFC from a role in task set establishment (Johnston and Everling, 2006; Johnston et al., 2009), as reflexive saccade errors following a DLPFC lesion could be explained by disruption to anti-saccade task-set signals to overcome the pro-saccade bias. A TMS pulse to DLPFC during the preparatory phase in an anti-saccade task did result in increased pro-saccade errors (Nyffeler et al., 2007), and “intermittent” TBS (thought to have excitatory effects; Huang et al., 2005) over DLPFC produced a reduction in pro-saccade errors (in patients with bipolar disorder; Beynel et al., 2014). In another study, however, a TMS pulse to DLPFC at the end of the preparatory period increased anti-saccade as well as pro-saccade latency, but not direction errors (Nagel et al., 2008).
TMS to DLPFC has also been shown to affect end point accuracy in memory saccades (Brandt et al., 1998), and DLPFC lesions resulted in higher variability in memory-guided saccade end points, with nonsignificant reductions in amplitudes (Pierrot‐Deseilligny et al., 2003), and a single-pulse TMS study did find that DLPFC pulses disrupted contralateral saccade amplitudes during the target memory component of a delayed saccade task (Müri et al., 1996). However, it has also been concluded in one lesion study that DLPFC was not necessary for performing the spatial calculations in a memory-guided saccade task (Mackey et al., 2016), and a study using cTBS to DLPFC did not find amplitude deficits to either ipsilateral or contralateral anti-saccades (Cameron et al., 2015).
Implications from the double perturbation
As outlined above, individual lesion or TMS studies have indicated that FEF, PPC, and DLPFC are important to pro-saccade and anti-saccade tasks. However, there is a high level of variability across studies in the types of behavioral deficits one observes. This may be the result of relative unfocused effects of a TMS perturbation, or lesion, on the underlying populations, and/or network-level effects that extend beyond the role of an individual node. This implies that caution should be taken in assuming that any one TMS (or lesion) study can definitively define the role of an oculomotor region. In this study, we focus on the effects of a double perturbation compared with a single perturbation in a single paradigm and environment, acknowledging that the specifics of the paradigm may make direct comparisons with other studies difficult.
FEF versus control cTBS conditions: anti-saccades
We did not find evidence to suggest an augmented impairment effect (Hypothesis A) from the double perturbation across any of the saccade behaviors. Substantial evidence did suggest impairments to anti-saccade amplitude in FEF cTBS conditions when PPC pulses were present; however, because there was not substantial evidence that PPC pulses on their own caused impairments, nor were the effects greater following the double perturbation relative to following FEF cTBS alone, we conclude that cTBS to FEF on its own was consequential to anti-saccade amplitudes. We suggest that FEF cTBS had a “distributed” effect on processing in the network (Hypothesis B).
For saccade reaction times, we found evidence for greater impairments from the double perturbation compared with FEF cTBS on its own. The observation that a second perturbation produces a deficit that is not otherwise observed unless the first node is perturbed, is the argument to indicate compensation by that second node (Sack et al., 2005). We do not, however, believe our findings here indicate compensation by PPC (the second node), because the combined FEF cTBS plus PPC pulse conditions did not actually reveal substantial evidence for impairing behavior (Table 3, Percentage correct). In fact, the late PPC pulses on their own produced impairments that were greater than the double perturbation for contralateral anti-saccades (Hypothesis E). We conclude therefore that later PPC pulses were disruptive to the motor component of the anti-saccade. Following FEF cTBS, however, a compensatory mechanism might be revealed by other network structures that aid in anti-saccade generation. One possibility is that after FEF cTBS, there is compensation by DLPFC–colliculus projections to contralateral SC saccade neurons (Everling and Johnston, 2013), reducing the disruptive effect from a PPC pulse on the same network structures. This is sensible, considering the PPC pulses also produced substantial anti-saccade performance benefits in percentage correct directions, and human EEG evidence has shown that the posterior parietal/occipital cortex is involved in triggering express pro-saccades (Hamm et al., 2010), possibly by a cortical-SC mechanism (Watanabe et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2013). A PPC pulse could therefore disrupt the bias toward stimulus-driven saccades, thus indirectly facilitating anti-saccade performance.
Altered SC function could contribute to both the behavioral deficits, as well as to compensatory effects in either visuomotor or executive control for the following reasons: it receives widespread projections from the retina, subcortical, and cortical brain regions, including FEF, PPC, and DLPFC, and thus, its activity is influenced by the afferent signals it receives; it has a spatial map for programming a saccade to a particular spatial location; it has the internal architecture for directly translating visual information into the motor commands, which it also sends to the brainstem saccade generator circuits, and finally, it has fixation and saccade neurons, which could play a role similar to those described in FEF (Munoz and Everling, 2004; Munoz and Schall, 2004; Boucher et al., 2007; Watanabe and Munoz, 2011).
However, we acknowledge that these effects could be driven in part by the auditory/or somatosensory influence of the pulse (Duecker et al., 2013; Duecker and Sack, 2013), which could engage a startle-like reflex that inhibits ongoing motor commands by also acting on the SC or brainstem saccade generator circuits (Xu-Wilson et al., 2011; perhaps with less of a consequence in cases of compensation). As the goal of this study was to compare hypotheses regarding the double vs single perturbation situations, the important comparisons are those between the PPC pulses following control versus verum cTBS, which both have the same auditory/somatosensory influences of the PPC pulse.
DLPFC versus control cTBS conditions: anti-saccades
We found strong evidence for “compensation” by PPC (Hypothesis C) following DLPFC cTBS for ipsilateral (rightward) anti-saccade amplitudes, but not substantial evidence for an augmented effect (Hypothesis A). Importantly, there was not substantial evidence that the PPC pulses alone produced an impairment. This finding is consistent with a compensatory mechanism, in that the second perturbation impairs a node that has assumed a greater contribution (Sack et al., 2005; Hartwigsen et al., 2016). We note that these effects were lateralized, as compensation was seen only in this ipsilateral direction. cTBS to DLPFC alone produced impairments in the contralateral direction, suggesting that DLPFC perturbations were more consequential for contralateral anti-saccades. The finding on its own is interesting as it suggests that DLPFC may be part of the vector inversion process previously emphasized to involve FEF and PPC (Munoz and Everling, 2004; Medendorp et al., 2005; Moon et al., 2007). However, the mixed findings from previous TMS and lesion studies lend support to a hypothesis that the spatial calculations for anti-saccades are performed by a distributed process. We can only speculate that compensation occurs in some circumstances depending on the particular task demands, such as spatial working memory complexity.
As with FEF cTBS, there was no evidence that any of the conditions impaired the percentage correct direction, but there was evidence for greater SRT impairments from the combined double perturbation compared with DLPFC cTBS alone. As addressed, the late PPC pulse impaired SRT on its own, suggesting that the effects are more related to that of the PPC pulse.
FEF versus control cTBS conditions: pro-saccades
There was no evidence to suggest that TMS to FEF or PPC impaired pro-saccade amplitudes, suggesting that other regions in a wider network are sufficient for the spatial calculations for a pro-saccade (Munoz and Schall, 2004). There were findings to suggest that the late PPC pulses following FEF cTBS impaired pro-saccade correct directions and that PPC pulses substantially increased reaction times, suggesting a detrimental effect of the PPC pulse, possibly by impairing PPC–SC signals (as described previously). We acknowledge, however, that because we rejected trials when reaction time was less than the PPC pulse time, the outcome measures of the late PPC pulse are biased as coming from pro-saccade trials with a slower latency.
DLPFC versus control cTBS conditions: pro-saccades
As with FEF cTBS, DLPFC appears not to be critical to pro-saccade amplitudes. Interestingly, the late PPC pulse following DLPFC cTBS impaired rightward pro-saccade performance compared with DLPFC cTBS alone, but it is difficult to interpret this as compensatory as this condition did not actually produce substantial evidence for an impairment (BF10 < 3; Table 6).
Conclusions
Our findings for a general lack of augmented effects from two TMS perturbations to critical nodes in anti-saccade programming suggest that these saccade behaviors are governed by distributed computations. Yet, if these regions are critical for behavior, how can we reconcile a lack of augmented effects from a double perturbation? Given evidence that anti-saccade vector inversion is developed simultaneously in FEF and PPC neuronal populations, our cTBS effects may be interpreted as being consequential for the communication of information between nodes (Sporns et al., 2007; Bullmore and Sporns, 2009) rather than for perturbing nodal computations only. FEF, DLPFC, and PPC are part of interconnected frontoparietal networks that are recruited when attentional control is needed (Dosenbach et al., 2008; de Schotten et al., 2011; Ptak, 2012; Vossel et al., 2014; Tschentscher et al., 2017). FEF and DLPFC may be critical nodes in terms of network-level processes, behaving as “connector hubs” for long-range information flow (Sporns et al., 2007; Bullmore and Sporns, 2009). A cTBS perturbation to FEF, or DLPFC, may therefore be consequential for the communication of information. Neuronal oscillations (not addressed in this study) nevertheless have been shown to be modulated in a cortical oculomotor network by TMS (Marshall et al., 2015) and could represent a “collective-order process” in network-level representations and interactions (Buzsáki, 2006, p 25). Together, this study illustrates how network interactions are important over summated contributions of individual nodes.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgments: We thank Paul Gaalman for assisting in MRI data collection.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported financially by the Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, which approved the study design and execution.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.