Abstract
Associative learning and sensory integration are two behavioral processes that involve the sensation and processing of stimuli followed by an altered behavioral response to these stimuli, with learning requiring memory formation and retrieval. We found that the cellular and molecular actions of scd-2 dissociate sensory integration and associative learning. This was discovered through investigation of a Caenorhabditis elegans mutation (lrn-2 (mm99)) affecting both processes. After mapping and sequencing, lrn-2 was found to be allelic to the gene, scd-2. scd-2-mediated associative learning and sensory integration operate in separate neurons as separate processes. We also find that memories can form from associations that are processed and stored independently from the integration of stimuli preceding an immediate behavioral decision.
Significance Statement
We show that the mutation lrn-2, a learning mutant derived from a random mutagenesis screen is allelic to scd-2, a receptor tyrosine kinase. Differences in the role of scd-2 provide the first evidence for genetic, cellular, and behavioral dissociations of sensory integration and associative learning in C. elegans. We show that scd-2 uses different genetic and neuronal pathways for its role in sensory integration versus associative learning. Furthermore, this dissociation shows that sensory integration and associative learning are separate phenomena and that memories can form from associations independent of initial sensory integration. This implies memory formation can be separated from real-time sensory perception.
Introduction
Although Caenorhabditis elegans has a relatively small and simple nervous system compared with mammals, it is capable of behavioral processes such as associative learning and sensory integration (Wen et al., 1997; Ishihara et al., 2002; Giles et al., 2006). Within C. elegans, these two behavioral processes are seemingly similar. Associative learning involves the pairing of two sensory stimuli, so that one becomes predictive of the other and leads to a persistent modified behavioral response to the individual conditioned cues. Sensory integration involves the processing of multiple sensory stimuli followed by a modified behavioral response in the presence of the sensory stimuli; however, unlike associative learning, this response may not require the retrieval of a memory. If there are further differences between sensory integration and associative learning (other than presence or absence of memory formation), then such differences may help illuminate the mechanisms of learning and memory function, and indeed how associations are formed.
lrn-2 (mm99) is an EMS (ethyl methanesulfonate, a mutagen that causes random point mutations) derived C. elegans mutant with associative learning deficits across multiple associative learning paradigms. Because these deficits include both olfactory and gustatory learning, lrn-2 is unlikely to be active in either olfactory or gustatory sensory neurons alone. It was originally isolated in a screen for mutants with deficits in associating salt with starvation (Wen et al., 1997), but further research has shown lrn-2 to have other deficits in associative learning about olfactory cues such as diacetyl (Morrison et al., 1999), and in occasion setting (Law et al., 2004). Here we reveal a deficit in the lrn-2 mutant in another associative learning paradigm that involves the pairing of the odor and pathogenicity of Pseudomonas aeruginosa (PA14) bacteria (Zhang et al., 2005). We have found that lrn-2 has a deficit in learning about PA14, and the use of this paradigm has led us to a connection between associative learning and sensory integration in C. elegans (Fig. 1A,B). The PA14 learning paradigm used for most of the following experiments is modified from Zhang et al. (2005) by combining the training and testing phases into a simpler assay (Fig. 1A), with comparable results to the original method when the original method was employed in our hands (Fig 1B,C). Although lrn-2 has an increased approach to PA14 after exposure during training, which could be the result of learning that PA14 has nutritive value, it does not learn that PA14 is pathogenic. This appetitive learning about nutritive value may be masked in the wild-type by aversive learning about pathogenicity. Because lrn-2 affects multiple associative learning paradigms it is likely that it is active downstream of the sensory neurons. This differentiates it from many of the genes involved in benzaldehyde-starvation associative learning, a lrn-2 independent memory that apparently is stored in the primary olfactory sensory neuron AWC (Pereira and van der Kooy, 2012). Associative learning using pathogenic bacteria has been shown to require parts of the TGF-β pathway, specifically dbl-1, which interacts with sma-5 in the hypodermis (X. Zhang and Y. Zhang, 2012). However, the genetic and molecular mechanisms that connect pathogenic bacteria associative learning to other paradigms affected by lrn-2, such as associative salt learning and diacetyl learning, have not been studied previously.
Materials and Methods
Strains
Strains used in this paper include Bristol N2, UT2 lrn-2 (mm99), UT1320 lrn-2 (mm99); publ-5::GFP, JT249 scd-2 (sa249) V, RB783 scd-2 (ok565) V, TY3553 scd-2 (y386) V, FX3084 scd-2 (tm3084) V, VC980 fsn-1 (gk429) III, ZM488 fsn-1 (hp1) III, ZM588 fsn-1(hp1) III; juls1 [unc-25p::snb-1::GFP+lin-15(+)] IV; scd-2(ok565) V, pgcy-28.d::scd-2;pmyo-3::GFP, UT1321 pceh-2::scd-2;pmyo-2::mcherry. Strains were grown at 20°C under standard conditions (Brenner, 1974).
Sequence analysis and alignment
Sequence reads were mapped to the C. elegans reference genome version WS230 (http://www.wormbase.org) using the short-read aligner BWA (Li and Durbin, 2009). Single-nucleotide variants and small insertions and deletions were identified and filtered with the help of the SAMtools toolbox (Li et al., 2009). Each variant was annotated with a custom-made Perl script and gene information downloaded from WormBase.
1 h choice assay
PA14 and OP50 were grown overnight in lysogeny broth (LB) medium and resuspended at an absorbance of 1.0 at 600 nm. Twenty-five microliters of each suspension was spotted on opposite sides (4 cm from the center) of a 10 cm Petri dish filled with 30 ml of Nematode Growth Medium and allowed to air-dry for 2 h. 1 μl of 10mm sodium azide was applied to each bacterial spot. Fifty to 150 young adult worms were then placed in the center of the dish and allowed to move freely for 1 h. At 1 h, worms at each bacterial spot were counted.
Egg-laying assay
This assay was modified from Gardner et al. (2013). Fifty microliters of either OP50 or PA14 were evenly spread on small plates and incubated at 37° for 48 h. Worms were grown from eggs on OP50 for 48 h at 23°C. Two worms were then placed on each plate, which was then sealed and incubated at 20° for 40 h. For counting, adult worms were immersed in a droplet of bleach solution and dissolved. Eggs produced per worm were counted as the number of eggs retained after dissolving and the number of eggs laid on the plate added together.
Killing assay
<100 young adult worms were placed on small plates containing either OP50 or PA14 lawns on nematode growth medium (NGM). The numbers of live worms were counted at multiple time intervals. Worms that were moving, had a pumping pharynx, and were not sticks were considered to be alive.
The PA14 associative learning assay
This assay is modified from Zhang et al. (2005, their Fig. 1A) to combine training and testing into a single step. Age synchronized young adult worms that were raised on NGM with OP50 at room temperature were used for this assay. Ten centimeter Petri dishes were filled with 30 ml of NGM. Once dry the plates were seeded with two bacterial lawns by pipetting 100 µl of OP50 and PA14 each obtained from overnight cultures grown in LB medium. These lawns were allowed to grow for 48 h before the commencement of the assay. One hundred to 200 worms were washed in M9, and the placed on the PA14 lawn via Pasteur pipette. Once the worms were dry, the plates were sealed in Parafilm and left for the remainder of the assay (4 h). The assay plates were then refrigerated and scored later. The number of worms remaining on the PA14 lawn were counted and divided by the total number of worms on the plate to provide the percentages seen in the assay figures.
In Figure 1B the assay included separate training and testing phases. Preparation was the same as above until the plating of worms onto PA14 plates. In this case, training plates contained a PA14 lawn only. One hundred to 200 worms were washed in M9, and the placed on the PA14 lawn via Pasteur pipette. Once the worms were dry, the plates were sealed in Parafilm and left for the remainder of the assay (4 h). Worms were then washed off these plates and placed on the center of testing plates with 50 µl point sources of PA14 and OP50 on either side of the plate for 1 h. In naïve conditions, worms were placed on OP50 plates for 4 h in lieu of PA14 training. These worms were then subjected to the same testing phase as PA14 exposed worms.
Sensory integration and copper acetate memory experiments
The sensory integration assay was performed as described by Ishihara et al. (2002, their Fig. 1A). The assay testing for a learned response to copper (II) acetate is modified from those methods. Ten centimeter NGM Petri dishes used for the training phase were split into three groups. For the paired condition, a barrier of 30 µl of 30 mm copper (II) acetate solution (in water) was spread across the middle of the plate and the gradient was allowed to set for 18 h. Then, age synchronized, young adult worms washed in M9 were placed on one side of the copper barrier, and a 2 µL droplet of 1:100 diacetyl was placed on the opposite side. Worms were dried with a Kimwipe, the plate was sealed with Parafilm and left for 1 h. In the control conditions, worms were either exposed to a 30 µL drop of 30mM copper (II) acetate without diacetyl or a naïve condition with no stimuli added to the plate. The testing phase used plates with a 30 µL drop of 30mM copper (II) acetate left for 18 h. After the completion of the training hour, worms were gently washed off the training plates with M9. Worms from all conditions were then pipetted onto the center of testing plates, dried with Kimwipes, sealed, and left for 1 h. Plates were then refrigerated and scored. Scoring for sensory integration was measured by counting the numbers of worms on each half of the plates and dividing the number on the copper side by the total number on each plate.
Statistical analyses
Statistical analyses were performed with Prism (GraphPad). The t test was used for comparisons of two variables, whereas experiments with multiple conditions were analyzed using ANOVA with Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons. For experiments in which groups have two independent variables for comparison, two-way ANOVA was used, with Tukey’s test for multiple comparisons. These analyses are summarized in Table 1.
Results
Mapping and identification of the Lrn-2 mutation locus
Zhang et al. (2005) showed that C. elegans forms associative memories about PA14’s olfactory cues and pathogenicity, and the lrn-2 deficit shown here in this assay (Fig. 1A,B,C) demonstrates that this form of associative learning has shared genetic requirements with salt associative learning, diacetyl associative learning, and sensory integration (Fig. 1D,E; Wen et al., 1997; Morrison et al., 1999; Ishihara et al., 2002). We first tested whether deficits in PA14 learning might be secondary to the effectiveness of PA14 pathogenicity. However, in both N2 and lrn-2, PA14 exposure causes an equivalent decrease in egg production, indicating that both strains are similarly affected by the pathogenicity of PA14 (Fig. 1F; Gardner et al., 2013). It is still possible that the learning deficit in lrn-2 is related to an inability to detect PA14 pathogenicity, despite being affected by it. N2 and lrn-2 also have equivalent approach to both PA14 and OP50 Escherichia coli after 1 h showing that they do not differ in baseline odor sensation (Fig. 1G). There is no significant difference in the rate at which N2 and lrn-2 worms are killed by PA14 pathogenicity (Fig. 1H).
lrn-2 emerged as a recessive loss-of-function mutation from a random mutagenesis screen (Wen et al., 1997; Morrison et al., 1999). We determined the locus of the lrn-2 mutation within the genome to investigate further its function within associative learning pathways. Preliminary snip-SNP mapping indicated that a portion of chromosome V was the likely region-of-interest. Whole-genome sequencing was performed on both lrn-2 and the N2 reference strain, and after analysis of the sequence using SAMTools, a short list of non-silent, non-synonymous mutations was found in the identified region of chromosome V (Li et al., 2009). The only mutant gene that also was not found in the reference strain within the snip-SNP targeted region was scd-2. The mutation is a missense mutation causing an A–P amino acid change in the extracellular glycine rich region, which has been shown to be conserved in Drosophila and human homologues of scd-2, and there is evidence in Drosophila that this region is important for protein function (Lorén et al., 2003). In tests to confirm scd-2 as the site of the lrn-2 mutation, the lrn-2 mutant failed to complement an scd-2 mutant strain, and insertion of a fosmid (WRM0614dF06) containing wild-type scd-2 into a lrn-2 mutant background rescued associative learning (Fig 2 A,B). The complementation test crossed scd-2 (ok565) worms with a glowing lrn-2 strain. The glowing lrn-2 strain was created by crossing publ-5::GFP worms in a wild-type background into lrn-2, selfing the progeny, identifying which progeny displayed the lrn-2 learning phenotype in a PA14 assay, and confirming with sequencing for the mutation identified previously by whole-genome sequencing. Together, the complementation test and the fosmid rescue confirm that the lrn-2 mutation is located in the scd-2 gene.
The scd-2 gene codes for a receptor tyrosine kinase that is homologous to mammalian anaplastic lymphoma kinase (ALK), which has been shown to affect mouse learning and memory in a suppressive manner (Reiner et al., 2008; Weiss et al., 2012). Worms with mutations in scd-2 show deficits in sensory integration, and mutations in hen-1, the suggested ligand of scd-2, are salt learning deficient (Ishihara et al., 2002; Shinkai et al., 2011).
As predicted, three other strains with separate scd-2 mutant alleles [including a null mutant with a large deletion (ok565)] have similar learning deficits to the lrn-2 strain in the PA14 associative learning assay (Fig. 2C). While worms carrying different scd-2 alleles have learning deficits of slightly varying magnitudes (Fig. 2C), this is likely caused by differences in the functional severity of the mutated scd-2 gene product. Similarly, lrn-2 mutant worms show a deficit in sensory integration much like scd-2(sa249), where the deficit is represented as the inability to integrate opposing cues when presented with an attractive diacetyl odor point source, sensed by AWA neurons, beyond an aversive copper acetate barrier, sensed by ASH neurons (Fig. 1E; Shinkai et al., 2011). To examine whether the “scd” (suppression of constitutive dauer) phenotype had an influence on PA14 associative learning, two other scd strains were tested. Neither scd-1 nor scd-3 have notable deficits in learning about pathogenic bacteria, indicating that the role of scd-2 is likely not caused by processes related to a deficit in dauer development (Fig. 2D).
FSN-1 acts upstream of SCD-2 in sensory integration, but not in associative learning
The F-Box gene fsn-1 regulates synapse formation, as part of a SCF ubiquitin ligase complex, and SCD-2 is a downstream target of FSN-1 (Liao et al., 2004; Li et al., 2011). FSN-1 has an intracellular interaction with SCD-2, likely through ubiquitination, causing SCD-2 downregulation and preventing development of abnormal synapse morphology at the neuromuscular junction (Liao et al., 2004). Li et al. (2011) showed that scd-2 also plays a role in sensory integration and found that FSN-1 suppresses SCD-2’s effect on sensory integration. We also confirmed that the fsn-1;scd-2 double mutant has a deficit in sensory integration similar to scd-2 alone (Fig. 3A). We predicted that SCD-2 could have a similar relationship to FSN-1 in associative learning. However, the results of testing fsn-1 (hp1), scd-2 (ok565), and double mutants (fsn-1;scd-2) in the PA14 associative-learning assay shows that the double mutants demonstrate wild-type learning (Fig. 3B). This differs from the double mutant’s sensory integration phenotype (Fig. 3A) and indicates that although the fsn-1 single mutant does not have a deficit, there is an interaction between FSN-1 and SCD-2 that is revealed by the double mutant. This interaction rescues wild-type learning although there is a mutation in scd-2 present. Thus, the role of FSN-1, as part of the SCF ubiquitination complex, has a different interaction with SCD-2 in associative learning, and this difference in the interaction between these proteins provides evidence for a dissociation of the molecular pathways governing sensory integration and associative learning.
scd-2 expression is required in different neurons for sensory integration and associative learning
The site of activity for scd-2 in sensory integration has been found to be within the AIA interneuron, because expression of wild-type scd-2 in AIA [under the AIA-specific gcy-28.d promoter (pgcy-28.d::scd-2;pmyo-3::GFP) within a scd-2 mutant background] is sufficient to rescue wild-type sensory integration (Fig. 4A; Shinkai et al., 2011). If the roles of scd-2 in sensory integration and associative learning are part of the same process, then it could be expected that AIA interneurons are similarly important for learning, as the mechanism would be shared. However, the differential role of fsn-1 in mediating sensory integration, as opposed to associative learning, suggests that scd-2-mediated learning and memory may require other sites of action. If scd-2 has independent roles in sensory integration and associative learning, it would not necessarily require AIA expression for learning. Using the same rescue strain as the previous sensory integration rescue experiment, which expresses scd-2 exclusively in AIA, we tested whether AIA expression was sufficient to rescue associative learning using the PA14 pathogenic bacteria assay (Shinkai et al., 2011). Expression of wild-type scd-2 in AIA was not sufficient to rescue learning in an scd-2 mutant background (Fig. 4B). This suggests that the site of scd-2 activity in associative learning and memory formation is in neurons other than AIA. Two remaining candidate neurons with endogenous expression are PVT and NSM (Zhang et al., 2014). NSM neurons are serotonergic and are involved in sensation of bacteria, and control of foraging behaviors, both of which affect PA14 learning (Zhang et al., 2005; Rhoades et al., 2018). Expression of wild-type scd-2 in NSM using the ceh-2 promoter in an scd-2 mutant background was not sufficient to rescue sensory integration (Fig. 4C), and indeed sensory integration is AIA mediated. However, expression of wild-type scd-2 in NSM does lead to a rescue of associative learning when trained to PA14 (Fig. 4D), which indicates that wild-type scd-2 expression in NSM is sufficient for associative learning. Therefore, wild-type scd-2 must be expressed in different neurons for sensory integration (AIA) and associative learning (NSM), showing that these two phenomena are also dissociated at a cellular level.
Training to copper acetate and diacetyl produces an scd-2 independent memory
The gene scd-2 plays roles in both sensory integration and associative learning; roles that may be part of two independent processes that can be dissociated at the genetic and cellular levels. Indeed, sensory integration involves the sensation of two stimuli and an immediate behavioral choice in the presence of both stimuli. This description of sensory integration differs from associative learning only in the lack of the persistence of the behavioral change (in the form of a memory) with associative learning. If associative learning and sensory integration are truly independent processes, an organism should be able to learn while sensory integration is blocked, and vice versa. Furthermore, the same sensory cues that lead to a sensory integration decision could simultaneously form an associative memory. To investigate whether there was a memory formed by presentation of two cues to test for sensory integration, a modified version of the diacetyl-copper sensory integration assay that includes separate training and testing phases was used. This would demonstrate whether diacetyl and copper form an associative memory while the worm is integrating these two cues. During training, N2 and scd-2 worms were exposed to diacetyl and copper, as in the sensory integration assay (Fig. 1D) for 1 h, along with naïve and copper only control groups. Both strains were then tested on plates with a point source of copper acetate for 1 h and their chemotaxis responses were recorded. When trained in presence of both attractive diacetyl and aversive copper acetate, worms form a memory of the association between these stimuli (Fig. 5A). This is shown by the attenuation of copper aversion for conditioned worms in the testing phase compared with single stimulus trained animals (Fig. 5A). When tested to diacetyl instead of copper, there is no change in diacetyl approach, implying that there is a ceiling effect (Fig. 5B). Furthermore, this learned response to copper requires simultaneous presentation of both copper and diacetyl, because there is no learning when presented sequentially (Fig. 5C). This indicates that copper approach reflects an associative memory. Because both N2 and scd-2 worms are able to learn about copper acetate, this memory must be separate from scd-2-mediated memories and it must not require scd-2-mediated sensory integration to associate copper acetate and diacetyl. Although an scd-2 mutation often leads to a deficit in learning, scd-2 worms are able to form a memory when copper and diacetyl are paired. This is possible because there are multiple learning circuits for different types of memories in worms; scd-2 mutant worms are able to pair benzaldehyde and starvation, and can successfully learn about mechanosensory stimuli (Nuttley et al., 2002). Similarly, not every gene involved in benzaldehyde starvation learning plays a role in scd-2-mediated PA14 or salt learning. This is a clear dissociation of sensory integration and associative learning at the level of behavior, suggesting that memories formed by pairing copper acetate and diacetyl are formed independently of the sensory integration of the same two stimuli as measured by the sensory integration assay.
Discussion
The results of our experiments suggest that wild-type scd-2 plays independent roles in associative learning and sensory integration. Although the complete genetic and cellular circuits for these two processes are not fully known, they both appear to use some of the same genetic machinery within independent circuits. Associative learning requires a persistent alteration of the behavioral response that can be observed during testing after the initial pairing of cues and in the presence of the conditioned cue alone. This persistent response is caused by the formation of a memory that can be repeatedly accessed when cues are presented at testing. Sensory integration also a change in behavioral response, but this response is an immediate decision after cue exposure, not a persistent learned choice. It is conceivable that sensory integration is a necessary early step in processing sensory information that leads to an associative memory. If this were the case, we would expect to see scd-2 acting in the same pathways for both processes, but our data suggest that scd-2 operates in independent genetic and cellular pathways. We describe this difference as a dissociation because it is these data concerning the difference in scd-2 activity that identify the independence of the genetic and cellular circuits involved.
Associative memories that require scd-2 cover a variety of sensory modalities (Wen et al., 1997; Ishihara et al., 2002), but remain separate from AWC primary olfactory neuron-mediated associations such as benzaldehyde/starvation pairing (Nuttley et al., 2002), and memories formed during the initial integration of diacetyl and copper acetate. Worms with scd-2 mutations do not have known deficits in chemosensation, mechanosensation, thermosensation, nor in non-associative learning (Rankin et al., 1990; Morris et al., 1997; Mori, 1999). Further, the present data show that associative memories of a copper and diacetyl pairing can be present in the same scd-2 mutants that are unable to undergo sensory integration of copper and diacetyl nor PA14 associative learning (Fig. 5A, see below). Indeed, the present work provides evidence that sensory integration and associative learning are dissociable at the genetic, cellular, and behavioral levels. These data suggest that there are deeper differences between sensory integration and associative learning in C. elegans than simply the formation of separate memories. Not only are sensory integration and associative learning dissociated, but also learned associations can be formed through a process completely independent from sensory integration that must be occurring simultaneously and in parallel. With scd-2 orthologs present in other species, including mammals (Morris et al., 1997), it is possible that this dual role in dissociated processes is conserved across taxa, providing opportunity to investigate the function of mammalian learning and memory genes in this genetically tractable nematode model.
The transcription factor FSN-1 has an established role in regulating synapse formation at the neuromuscular junction, and fsn-1 mutants display both abnormal synapse morphology (Liao et al., 2004) and enhanced sensory integration (Li et al., 2011). Experiments with double mutants of fsn-1 and scd-2 show an scd-2-like sensory integration deficit (Fig. 3A; Li et al., 2011). Although this relationship regulates both sensory integration and the neuromuscular junction, the roles of SCD-2 and FSN-1 in associative learning about pathogenic bacteria must be part of an independent pathway because the double mutant does not produce a deficit in PA14 associative learning. Because fsn-1;scd-2 double mutants are phenotypically similar to fsn-1 alone in associative learning about pathogenic bacteria, there must be a different interaction between FSN-1 and SCD-2 compared with their activity during sensory integration. If these two proteins did not interact in PA14 learning, the double mutant would be expected to show a deficit similar to scd-2 alone; the fsn-1 mutation somehow masks the scd-2 learning deficit (Fig. 3A,B). There is further evidence of separate fsn-1-dependent and -independent pathways containing shared components from previous research. The insulin-like protein, INS-6, acting through the DAF-2 receptor has an antagonistic relationship with FSN-1 in which reduced insulin signaling rescues the fsn-1 abnormal synapse morphology phenotype (Hung et al., 2013). Furthermore, INS-6 plays a role in enabling associative learning about pathogenic bacteria by repressing transcription of ins-7 (Chen et al., 2013). Thus, wild-type ins-6 plays a role in synapse formation that is phenotypically similar to the role of wild-type scd-2, but its role in associative learning may not be through negative regulation of FSN-1. The effect of ins-6 on learning also happens through a different mechanism than its antagonistic relationship with fsn-1 in sensory integration; this further supports the notion of that sensory integration and associative learning are independent processes with some shared genetic components. Although a connection between scd-2 and ins-6 remains unknown, we have found in the present results that scd-2 could have a similar relationship with to fsn-1 as ins-6, in which sensory integration appears to require fsn-1 suppression but associating pathogens with odor operates independently.
Expression of wild-type scd-2 from fosmid injection employing the endogenous promotor rescues both sensory integration and associative learning, but the individual cells necessary for each process must be established. We have shown that targeted expression of wild-type scd-2, under an AIA interneuron-specific promoter, although sufficient for rescue of sensory integration in an scd-2 mutant background, is not sufficient for rescue of associative learning (Fig. 4A,B). However, wild-type scd-2 expression in NSM did lead to a rescue of the associative learning phenotype in the mutant, so NSM neurons are the cellular location of scd-2 activity during PA14 associative learning (Fig. 4D). NSM neurons endogenously express wild-type scd-2 (Mckay et al., 2003; Zhang et al., 2014), and are serotonergic, located in the head, play a role in sensation of bacteria, and mediate foraging behavior (Avery et al., 1993; Rhoades et al., 2018). There also is evidence that NSM regulates attractive food related response to bacteria, which could indicate that the present findings concerning the scd-2 mutation are caused by the inability to properly identify whether PA14 or OP50 are the more attractive or nutritious bacteria (Zhang et al., 2005). These features of NSM are all consistent with the features of PA14 learning, because learning about PA14 requires serotonin, uses bacteria as an unconditioned stimulus, and leads to modified foraging behavior when worms no longer approach PA14. The difference in cellular localization of scd-2 expression in sensory integration and associative learning provides another dissociation of these two processes at the circuit level. Although scd-2 is involved in both processes, these data indicate that scd-2 expression and activity are required in separate neurons in two dissociated circuits that have differing genetic interactions with fsn-1. Although neuronal circuits for sensory integration have been proposed previously (Ishihara et al., 2002; Shinkai et al., 2011), the neuronal circuit for PA14 learning is less complete. ADF has been shown to be important for serotonergic activity, ins-6 and ins-7 have been shown to communicate through URX and RIA, dbl-1 activity requires release from AVA to ASI or hypodermal cells (X. Zhang and Y. Zhang, 2012; Chen et al., 2013), and we now know that scd-2 also plays a role in NSM. Because the wild-type ligand of scd-2, hen-1 can still rescue sensory integration even when expressed in neurons that do not endogenously express it; hen-1 may not need to be expressed in neurons that synapse directly with NSM for scd-2 learning (Ishihara et al., 2002). Thus, it is possible that detection of PA14 odors by AWC (Ha et al., 2010) leads to HEN-1 release from AIY, which travels to SCD-2 receptors in NSM, causing an learned alteration of foraging behavior. Nevertheless, how this part of the proposed neuronal circuit connects with the URX, ADF, and AVA neurons will require further study.
The existence of multiple associative learning circuits, both scd-2 independent and dependent, provides an opportunity to investigate whether sensory integration plays a role in the initial sensory cue processing step in memory formation. scd-2 Does not have deficits in all forms of associative learning, so the finding that pairing copper acetate and diacetyl leads to the formation of an scd-2-independent memory (Fig. 5A) is not remarkable on its own. However, the evidence that an scd-2-independent memory can be formed after training to copper acetate and diacetyl cues, despite a deficit in integrating those cues, suggests that the formation of such a memory uses a mechanism of sensory association that is independent of the decision-making process during sensory integration. Therefore, sensory integration of opposing stimuli is not the process by which the cues are associated to form memories, and associative learning must be occurring both simultaneously and independently of sensory integration. Although the initial associative mechanism used to form copper acetate and diacetyl associative memories is still unknown, the dissociation at the level of sensory integration and associative learning is consistent with the dissociations of these two behavioral processes at the circuit and molecular levels.
In worms, if associations can be formed independently of sensory integration, then there must be a difference in how a memory is represented versus how integrated stimuli are represented within the worm nervous system, much like the differential processing of contextual cues in mammals (Skinner et al., 1994; Good et al., 1998). One possibility is that sensory integration leads to a blending of two stimuli in a single representation that leads to an appropriate motor response, whereas associative learning encodes an association between the separate representations of two stimuli. For example, integration of copper and diacetyl would create a temporary blended representation that is interpreted as “copper tastes different and better mixed with diacetyl”, and the worm’s motor response would depend on salience of these cues when presented together. Because a sensory integration response is not memory dependent and only functions in the presence of both stimuli, it can be predicted that the worm is responding to a temporary perceived representation of a blended stimulus. Because associative memories can be retrieved after training by a single associated cue without required simultaneous presentation, it is likely that the worm processes associative learning as two separately stored but paired cues instead of a blended representation. Memory recall could activate a representation of the conditioned stimulus that co-activates a separate representation of the unconditioned stimulus and triggers the appropriate behavioral response. Such an engram preserves the associative information learned during training, while allowing post-training retrieval without the presence of both stimuli simultaneously.
Sensory integration and associative learning, although both involve detection and processing of sensory cues followed by a behavioral choice, are dissociated at three distinct levels. Differences in the FSN-1 interaction with SCD-2 dissociate these processes through their molecular mechanisms. Activity of SCD-2 is required in different cells for sensory integration and associative learning, indicating a dissociation of the neuronal circuitry behind these processes. The ability to form scd-2-independent associative memories without scd-2-mediated sensory integration indicates that these processes are also functionally dissociated at the behavioral level. This dissociation suggests that wild-type scd-2 is an important regulator of multiple processes. scd-2 also has been shown to regulate forgetting of memories dependent on AWA sensory neurons, while our findings show an scd-2-mediated independent regulation of memory formation of AWC-based PA14 learning (Kitazono et al., 2017) The independent importance of wild-type scd-2 in forgetting, sensory integration, and associative learning shows that it is an important gene to study for further understanding of nematode behavior. Although the details of the molecular, cellular, and behavioral mechanisms behind sensory integration and associative learning are not fully known, scd-2 provides evidence that they are two independent behavioral processes governing C. elegans behavior. Understanding the nature of such dissociations may be able to inform how learning and memory occur both within the nematode, and potentially, across taxa.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests
This work was supported by Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (501100000038). Some strains were provided by the CGC, which is funded by NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs (P40 OD010440). Other strains were provided by the National Bioresource Project. We thank Dr. Takeshi Ishihara for providing the AIA rescue animals, Dr. Peter Roy for discussions and microinjection support, Dr. Karen Maxwell for the use of her laboratory space for PA14 assays, and the worm group in the van der Kooy laboratory for discussion and support. The genomic sequencing was provided by Canada’s Michael Smith Genome Sciences Center.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.